This content was uploaded by our users and we assume good faith they have the permission to share this book. If you own the copyright to this book and it is wrongfully on our website, we offer a simple DMCA procedure to remove your content from our site. Start by pressing the button below!
‘good morning’ bom DIA; ‘good morning’ aqui não se trabalha mais com a semente e essas coisas lá, ‘here you don’t work with seeds and like those things’ (.) ‘damn it but...’ [de milho nós temo ainda (.) alGU[:ma coisa ‘maize we’ve still got a bit’ [ e::hh de ‘no’ milho não ‘maize I don’t want’ eu queria:: (.) SORgo;= ‘I wanted (.) millet’ =[não. ‘no’ [não existe MAIS; ‘doesn’t exist’ mir han BLOSS milje. ‘we only have maize’ bloß milje. (-) ‘only maize’
Being a ‘colono’ and being ‘daitsch’ in Rio Grande do Sul ‘I don’t know why why they do it!’ na (piA isso) também não tem? ‘at (the XXX3) they haven’t got it either?’ NÃO ‘no’ eu SEI (.) ‘I know’ mas (.) só de deiz quilo (.) ‘but only (in) ten kilo (sacks)’ mas com dez quilo(.) não vai (.) ‘but with ten kilos (.) it doesn’t work (.)’ não (--) ‘no (--)’ sim; (-) ‘yes’ é; (.) ‘well;’ infelizmente. ‘unfortunately.’ isso é lei (ele) ‘it’s a law’ se não (não) [(te trouxe ) ‘if it wasn’t (we’ld have it)’ [mas essas leis são (.) ‘but those laws are’ PUta mas que SA: [co; ‘shit, what a drag!’ [É::H ‘right’ (se vê) quem tem uma coisinha pequena (eh) ‘(if somebody comes) who has a small piece’ ((of land)) (‘yes’) ( [ ) [zehn kilo du:sd=de (.) ‘(with) ten kilos you can do’ <eu sei eh > ‘I know yes’ wieviel INseie né> (.) ‘how much sowing right?’ Ijo;= ‘sure’
103
104
Peter Auer, Jacinta Arnhold, and Cintia Bueno-Aniola
36
km10: =um monte de coisa né
37
a2:
38
a1:
39
40
k10:
41
a1:
42
k10:
43
a1:
44 45
k10: a2:
46
k10:
47
‘a load of things right’ (tá isso é [claro) ‘(that’s it that’s clear)’ [ÉH: que ‘right also’ eu acho ruim (.) que nem pro pessoal vem aqui ‘I think ((it is also)) bad (.) also for the people who come here pra pegar milho né to take ((=buy)) maize right’ não eu [SEI ‘no, I know’ [quero tantos quilos tantos quilos ja aber ‘‘I want so many kilos so many kilos’ well but’ ja das GEHT ja [net ‘this doesn’t work of course’ [(a gente faz escondi:do assim) eh ‘(they do it under the counter like that) right’ (-) ( ) o que dava né> 24 a2: Filippu Mangiaficu, > Sucabrodu > Vossia (‘Your honour’)
Pino (15)
> Pinocchio
Carmelo (17) Flavio (16) Silvia (15)
> Calimero Incognito > Flamingo > pesce (‘fish’)
Mariangela (18)
> Bloody Mary
Claudia (15)
> Schlaudia Kiffer
Dani (13)
> Fruchtzwerg
Giovanni (17) (Gio, Giuvà)
Italy + breakdance international, media local German slang hip hop; Southern Italy, folklore Southern Italy (popular polite address term) popular Italian literature (film, comics) Italian comic (assonance), unspecific local German slang, astrology3 urban lifestyle (a cocktail) 4 fashion, media; 5 soft drugs, subculture media, advertising (German)
The reported names of the other members allude to various cultural domains. Some of them point to Italy and popular literary or media figures, like Pinocchio or Calimero, a Sicilian comic figure. Both names are formally based on alliteration or assonance (< Pino, Carmelo), as is Flavio > Flamingo, a name for which we did not find a meaningful (semantic) explanation. Silvia, the only female breakdancer, goes by the German pseudonym Fisch, referring to her group membership as well as to her astrological sign (Pisces). Finally some of the names evoke the world of the (German) media, show business and advertising, such as Schlaudia Kiffer, an onset cluster reversal of Claudia Schiffer, which was adopted by the youngsters from a popular German TV comedy show (“RTL Samstag Nacht”). It alludes simultaneously to the German top model and to mari-
128
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
juana smoking (< kiffen, ‘to smoke grass’). Fruchtzwerg is a yogurt product for children made by the brand Danone, and hence applied to Dani, the youngest group member. Bloody Mary for Mari(angela) evokes a wellknown cocktail beverage (thus ‘lifestyle’) and produces a certain ‘dramatic fictionalization’ and de-ethnization of the very traditional Italian name Maria. A basic (linguistic) motivation for the creation of pseudonyms is form: alliteration, assonance or rhyme with the real name. At the same time they are chosen in accordance with some personal features of the bearer and his or her relation to a certain cultural milieu.9 This raises the question of how such names are created and used in interaction, which will be discussed under different perspectives in the following sections.
3.
Presenting oneself as a hip hopper
Most of the nicknames presented above could not be directly observed in our recorded data. All of them have been reported and confirmed by group members, but when our recordings of group interaction took place, only a few of them were used. Although they function as code names in the framework of hip hop culture, they may not necessarily be used as a form of address, but rather as ‘artists' pseudonyms’, to (re-)present, or refer to, participants and their posse in hip hop related events (Bierbach and BirkenSilverman 2002a). The following sequence extracted from an ingroup conversation is a good example of a situation where group members display their ‘glocal’ hip hop identity. During a rather technical conversation about breakdance, one of the boys (Gio – ‘the boss’) suddenly changes the interactional key and initiates a scenario in which an ‘internationally famous’ Bboy greets the group in a fictitious encounter among breakdancers: (1) B-Boy Earthquake – Paris meets Mannheim (21.7.1999) The different languages involved in conversational code-switching are represented by different typographies: Sicilian; Italian; German; English/French. The following transcription symbols are used: K = comment referring to the line above; GK = general comment; #xxxx# = metalinguistic comment; |xxxxx| = parallel sequences; + quick response; / breakoff; * pause; >piano #* #>na vuci< con calma |(...)|# ‘#> # * # >a voice < calmly |(...)|#’ #IMITATES PLANE NOISE# # SINGS #
K 3
Francesco:
| ja | | ‘yes’ |
4
Gio:
frati boni eh
‘I come from Pa-ris’ >> 6
Pino:
äh sein Name/ se name (...) ‘eh his name/ the name (…)’
7
Gio:
jo isch komm os Braunschweig un isch finds toll hier die B-Boys ‘yo I come from Braunschweig and I see it’s real great here, the Bboys’
8
Gio:
und heut viel Party abgeh * ‘and a big party going on today’ *
9
Silvia:
#LAUGHS#
In the framework of hip hop culture, this performance is an act of boasting: the naming, the allusion to hip hoppers from abroad, the evocation of a spectacular entrance by imitating the noise of an airplane, and finally the compliment made to the Mannheim group by the fictitious visitor adds up to produce an upgrading of their own group. Besides the use of hip hop names (B-Boy Earthquake, Deko) and a prestigious toponym (Paris), a dense pattern of code-switches can be observed which serve to contextualize different speakers and localize them in different cultural settings: The first turn, which triggers the following scene, starts in Sicilian, with a shift to Italian (l. 1: Francoforte fino a). It is interrupted by Gio’s imitation of an airplane, who then continues in Sicilian and Italian, in a singsong mode, marking the beginning of a scene (l. 2: ‘a voice, calmly’). Francesco gives a back channel signal in German (l. 3), and Gio goes on with a greeting (l. 4: frati boni, a Sicilian adaptation of the hip hop address term ‘brothers’) which presumably represents the voice of the arriving guest star, who ad-
130
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
dresses the Mannheim Italians. Gio then introduces himself as ‘B-Boy Earthquake’ in Mannheim dialect pronouncing his hip hop code name in Italianized English (bi-bò < ‘B-boy’). He then switches to English for the answer of the fictitious visitor, thus suggesting – together with the emphatically pronounced reference to Paris – that he is dealing with an internationally renowned personality (l. 4–5). Pino starts a quest for (more) information in German and self-corrects in English, but he is interrupted by Gio’s next turn – again in Mannheim dialect, despite the reference to a city in Northern Germany (l. 7: Braunschweig). This last turn of the fictitious encounter is a bit cryptic, as it is not quite clear whether the code-switch back to German represents another (third) person who compliments the group, or still the famous ‘Deko’ from Paris, since no further name is introduced. In any case, the sequence presents a nice example of polyphony evoking a multilingual and multicultural hip hop world which is created spontaneously in conversation by the use of appropriate code names and a few formulaic expressions.
4.
Media experts: Interviewing fellow Italians in Germany
Naming, as has been shown in the preceding section, constitutes an important element of verbal interaction. It occurs in opening sequences to introduce oneself and/or other co-present participants and, more recurrently and more freely, in addressing and in referential expressions. The choice and form of names vary according to situational parameters, communicative genre, etc.; in fact, as basic indexical items, name forms, together with other stylistic cues, contribute to contextualize genres, (formal) situations and social relations. Of course, to introduce oneself or others formally would be inadequate in in-group communication, unless a stranger comes in – or when a microphone evokes a media setting and transforms copresent group members into media professionals, guests or an audience. In fact, one of the striking features in our data is that a sequential format (‘introducing oneself’) is often performed as a game or ludic scenario – as in example (1) – staging desirable cultural identities and social membership relevant to the group members at that moment. The performance of media genres is another important resource to construct presentations of self and to relate oneself and others to specific social worlds. The presence of a microphone may be an occasion for such a performance. The following sequences were recorded during the group’s journey to Paris in which Sara,
Names and Identities
131
our Sicilian fieldworker, participated as a relative and friend of some of the group members. During this informal and relaxed situation, her nevertheless omnipresent microphone came into the sight of some of the group members, who turned this into an occasion to embark on ‘microphone related’ performances: (2) Interviewing Italians in Germany (Journey to Paris, 8.6. 2001) Participants: Giovanni (Gio), Sara (sara), Lino, Francesco (F) GK
#THE BOYS ARE FOOLING AROUND, SOMEBODY IMITATES FARTS# still still / alle ruhisch sara nimmt auf, scheisse ‘quiet quiet/everyone calm down sara is recording, shit’
1
Gio:
2
?:
(#UNINTELLIGIBLE#)
3
?:
voll unauffällig ‘real subtle’
4
sara:
#LAUGHS#
5
Gio:
halló ‘hello’
6
sara:
> >
7
Gio:
#PRETENDS TO HOLD A MICROPHONE# Mein Name ist Casella mit C und 2 L, und ‘my name is Casella with c and two l’s, and’
8
Gio:
neben mir steht öh Spoto mit S und P. Und was sagen Sie zu dieser Situation dass ‘next to me is ah Spoto with s and p. What do you think about that situation that’
9
Gio:
wir nach Frank#reich gehen#? ‘we are #going to France?#’ #LAUGHING #
K 10 ??:
#LAUGHTER# sì sì (#INCOMPREHENSIBLE #) #LAUGHTER# ‘yes yes’ (#UNINTELLIGIBLE #)
11 Gio:
okay danke und jetz rüber zu mein Assistent Francesco Incoglionito ‘okay, thank you, and now to my assistant Francesco Incoglionito’
12 ?:
äh ‘uh’
132
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
13 Gio:
mit In- noch nich Incoglionito mit ähm Invegliuto was sa’n Sie zu Frankreich? ‘with In-, not yet Incoglionito with uh Invegliuto what do you think about France?’
14 Lino:
talíu a tía talíu ‘I look at you I look’ (= I’m watching you) # LAUGHTER #
K 15 F:
je tali-a tia je talíu ‘I (= French ‘je’) look at you’
16 Gio :
#soga in Französisch machn die mein Vata nach # #‘even in French these guys are imitating my father’# # UPSET VOICE AND LAUGHTER #
K
In this sequence the actual situation – a holiday trip to Paris among friends – is redefined by Gio upon discovering that Sara is carrying her microphone, even in this informal situation where she was not supposed to ‘work’. He takes the floor and contextualizes the script of a radio or TV-interview, first by an opening signal (5, halló, which may be a greeting or a microphone test), then by an opening formula typical of media talk: he introduces himself (Casella) and a partner (Spoto), to whom a question is addressed, to a fictitious audience. The interesting aspect in this self-presentation – which, as the whole sequence, is performed in German – is the (partial) spelling of the Italian family names. This certainly alludes to a well-known experience of migrants, i.e. that their names are unfamiliar to the Germans and must be spelled out to be understood correctly.10 The choice of the spelled items seems quite appropriate, as in Casella – C and double L – which denotes ‘typically Italian’ orthography, while the pronunciation /S/-/P/ in Spoto would be unusual in German. It is therefore legitimate to interpret this particular way of presenting the speakers’ names as ‘performing being Italians in Germany’ in a scenario addressed to a fictitious German audience. The following part of the ‘interview’, however, becomes more in-group oriented. Calling up the next speaker (labelled as ‘my assistant’, in accordance with genre specific conventions in the mass media, and at the same time confirming the established ‘real life’ hierarchy in the group), Gio distorts his partner’s family name to create a pun (10): Incoglionito, meaning something like ‘stupid’ (or ‘being made an idiot’), with obscene connotations due to the etymology of the word (< coglioni, ‘balls’). Apparently such a pun is perceived by the speaker himself as a bit strong in this context
Names and Identities
133
(and there is no sign of appreciation by the other participants, such as laughter), so he initiates a repair, first by adding an attenuating modifier to the nickname (‘not yet’), and then using the correct name when addressing him directly (l. 13). The first reaction to this turn is a code-switch into Sicilian by Lino (who was not addressed): talíu a tia talíu ‘I’m watching you’ (l. 14) is pronounced like a warning, thus interrupting the interview format and challenging the speaker’s role as a ‘media professional’. This ‘inappropriate’ turn is repeated with a slight variation by Francesco, the addressed speaker, who adds a French clitic pronoun to the Sicilian verb – ‘je talí-a tia je talíu’ –, certainly triggered by the preceding question concerning France. He is therefore interpreted by Gio as ‘imitating my father – even when speaking French’, since the use of Sicilian, together with the prosodic mode of the utterance and tone of voice which cues it as a reprimand or warning, connotes elder family members, mostly parents, in the sociosymbolic repertoire of the group.11 The fictitious performance of mass media genres is one of the most popular rituals to construct a common peer group culture (Bausch and Sting 2001). Here, it is used by a dominant group member, first to stage an Italian identity by the choice of markedly Italian family names, but also to tease a companion by exploiting his patronymic for a pun. The interview format, in return, is subverted by two (rival) participants, who use code-switching into Sicilian to frame a different script: not that of the clever TV-reporter ‘on mission’ to Paris, but that of the overprotected son of an Italian immigrant, controlled by his father wherever he goes. The construction of competing cultural models – a ‘modern’ and a ‘traditional’ one, the fictitious (and prestigious) world of mass media vs. the trivial reality of Sicilian family norms – is thus mingled with rivalry games in the peer group. Several minutes later, the TV script is brought up again briefly, this time using the names of popular German television hosts: (3) TV hosts – with “mezzogiorno” looks (Journey to Paris, 8.6.01) 1
F:
Mikro is schwer isch gebe weiter mein Kollege Kai Ebel ‘microphone is heavy I go to my colleague Kai Ebel’
2
Lino:
+ ja, und hier im Studio von Kai Pflaume #cu capiddi tisi com un calabrisi# + ‘yes, and here in the studio of Kai Pflaume (‘plum’) with his hair straight like a calabrese’
K
#LAUGHS#
134
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
Both names introduced here refer to well-known TV hosts, the first triggering the second by the coincidence of the first names. Moreover, Pflaume (‘plum’) as a family name produces a funny effect by its literal meaning as well as by its colloquial connotation when used as a pejorative form of address meaning ‘coward’ or ‘incompetent’, which, in the sequential position, refers to the first speaker, Francesco. The teasing effect is heightened by a rhymed apposition in Sicilian, which transforms the addressed interactant into a hybrid: a German TV host with the physiognomy of a Southern Italian provincial. Summing up, we have shown that Italian and German names (real and adopted as well as attributed ones) are used to evoke specific social worlds as well as to tease and to experiment with social status within the peer group. We shall return to this second aspect later and first discuss another scenario where fictitious Italian names play an important part.
5.
Mangiaficu – the Southern peasant and macho
Let us now look at another episode, where the format ‘introducing oneself’ is used in a subversive way, this time constructing a Southern Italian macho identity vis-à-vis a German woman who is perceived as an intruder into the group’s territory. The group has gathered in the basement of a neighborhood center (‘Café Filsbach’), in the multiethnic neighborhood where most of them live and where the breakdance group is allowed to use a room for practicing. They call this room ‘our place’; hence an elderly German woman – in fact a school teacher known to one of the boys, who passes by chance – is not expected in this environment, and is actually felt to be there ‘illegitimately’. Contrary to the examples discussed so far, it would be a normal and polite act in this encounter to introduce oneself formally. However, some of the boys subvert the format parodistically. This scene is interesting for our discussion because it operates with a pseudonym to represent a stereotypical (southern) Italian identity: (4) Mangiaficu. Performing a ‘Southern Macho’ (Café Filsbach, 8.2.1999) 1
Dani:
2
Gio:
| oh die Frau B. | is ja do ‘| Oh, Mrs. B. | is over there’ | cu è chista ? | |‘who is this one’|?
Names and Identities 3
Dani:
#LAUGHS #
4
Gio:
Frau B., #Mangiaficu # ‘Mrs. B., # Mangiaficu #’ #INTRODUCES HIMSELF#
K 5
Silvia: K
# Frau B. # #‘Mrs. B.’! # #TINY VOICE#
6
Dani:
7
Gio:
8
Gio:
|Mangia|ficu Filippu ‘|Mangia|ficu Filippu’
9
Silvia:
#|LAUGHS|#
10 Gio:
135
s/ dis is meine Lehrerin, | also | meine alte Lehrerin, geil, Frau B. ‘this is my teacher, | well |, my former teacher, great, Mrs. B.’ | signó | |‘Signora’|
hallo Sucabrodu ta/ ‘hello Sucabrodu’
11 Frau B: was macht ihr hier ? Mrs.B: ‘What are you doing here?’ 12 Gio:
(..|.......|) (..|.......|)
13 Dani:
|redn| grad, wir |tanzn hier ‘|we are| just talking, we |dance here
14 s: 15 Gio: K 16 Frau B: Mrs.B: 17 Gio:
| |’
| ich inter|view die grade ‘|I’m just having| an interview with them’ #|interviu:en |# #|‘interviu:wing’|# #IRONIC, IMITATING SARA# | Interview | wozu ? ‘|an interview, | for what?’ ja B/ Invasion of Beat de break dance group * forever ‘yes B/ Invasion of Beat the break dance group * forever’ (#IN ENGLISH#)
136
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
Let us first take a look at language choice and code-switching in this sequence. Dani’s first turn, announcing the presence of Mrs. B, is in (Mannheim) dialect, i.e. the unmarked and ‘public’ variety which is used in ‘normal’ in-group communication as well as for communicating with nonItalians. Tone of voice and the syntactic construction express surprise (l. 1). Gio’s almost simultaneous question in Sicilian, with the rather impolite demonstrative cu è chista (‘who is this one?’), cue this turn as in-group directed. Dani’s reaction (l. 3) is short (nervous?) laughter. Gio then performs a parody of a polite introduction (l. 4), using a German address term (‘Frau B.’) followed, probably with a corresponding gesture, by a fictitious and rather grotesque Italian name, or rather soprannome (nickname), to introduce himself: Mangiaficu (‘Fig-eater’, with a marked Sicilian pronunciation). Silvia’s backchannel echo of the German address term, in a tiny, high pitched voice (l. 5), seems to indicate that she perceives the parody and the inherent provocation the name implies (see below). Dani then introduces his former teacher to the group, in near standard German and with a rather neutral formula, followed by an exclamation in youth language (geil) which, addressed to a teacher, seems rather out of place (l. 6).12 This turn overlaps with Gio’s reformulation of his first self-introduction, this time in Sicilian. The dialectal honorific signó and the expansion of the chosen pseudonym add to the parodic character of the scene he is performing (l. 7). Silvia laughs, and this may encourage Gio to go on with another funny Sicilian surname (Sucabrodu, ‘brothsucker’), preceded by an informal (English) greeting signal (hello, l. 10). He is interrupted by the woman, who apparently does not react to his humorous advances (l. 11), and the conversation continues for a short while in a neutral question–answer format between the teacher and Sara (accompanied by Gio’s short ironic echoes), until the term interview and Mrs. B.’s question (l. 16) trigger an English reply, as Gio mentions the breakdance group, adding an appraising forever (l. 17). Here again the use of different language varieties is indexical of different social worlds, and corresponds to different interactional configurations. Moreover, the choice of the fictitious names in the performance of the introduction format carries additional symbolic meaning: Mangiaficu embodies the comical image of the stereotypical Southern maschio, suggesting a poor mezzogiorno country man who is nonetheless a notorious womanizer, convinced of his irresistible charm – a meaning which is reinforced by the obscene connotations the lexical components of the names imply.13 All these connotations are present in a satirical song by the Sicilian cantautore
Names and Identities
137
Brigantony (“L’intervista”), which is well-known and often alluded to in the group. Especially Gio often claims this name (Mangiaficu) and variations like Sucabrodu for himself when performing the Sicilian macho ‘dragging’ or provoking a (non-Italian) woman, or in competitive discourse referring to other Southern Italian males. Thus, the use and performance of these names conveys a double message: addressed to the German woman – who is certainly aware of, and irritated by, the parodistic gesture of the ‘introduction’ and the inappropriate use of a foreign language in this situation – it represents a simple provocation (exploiting her ignorance of the Italian dialect), and to the group members, who capture the more precise double meanings of the names, it provides amusement. The provocative effect of this opening sequence is further built up by the subsequent splitting of the conversation in two parties (not shown in the extract): while Sara and Dani try to keep up a normal conversation with the teacher in German, Gio grabs the microphone and, joined by an ‘ally’ (Carmine), continues to address her in Sicilian, in a sort of ‘dissing’ or ‘signifying’, resembling the ritual insults described by Labov (1972). Both boys compete, in a fast exchange of turns, in formulating grotesque fictitious descriptions of the woman’s physique and looks, some of them rhyming, and summing up to a series of subverted (negative) ‘compliments’. 14 Evidently, what is at stake here is not only ethnicity (as expressed by the use of the dialect), but also gender relations as well as relations between different age groups. By their performing and subverting a Sicilian–German male–female ‘dragging’ scenario, the boys establish maximum social distance between themselves and the German woman. She is excluded from their in-group territory by the use of a language she does not understand and by the refusal of normal (polite) communicative behavior, and finally by the ridiculous image the boys construct of her with names, descriptions and labels.15 This implies at the same time a subversion of power and status relations: from the perspective of Mangiaficu, or the Southern macho, the woman – representing German mainstream society, adulthood and authority as a school teacher – and the low status young immigrant school boys change places. The boys embody superior young, strong males, disposing of a language and insider knowledge which are not available to the older woman. However, this scenario is also parodic. By the choice of the names, the hyperbolism of the expressions, and laughter, it can also be understood as a caricature of the stereotypical Italian (and particularly Sicilian) male that
138
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
prevails in German society, i.e. it embodies an ascribed stereotypical immigrant identity. While in the preceding example an identity cluster of masculinity, ethnicity and youth is staged to ward off a female ‘intruder’ and at the same time amuse the in-group, the emblematic names with which this scenario was initiated can be used in other contexts with quite different functions. In the following sequence, for instance, they are used to downgrade a compaesano rival in the hip hop scene, contextualizing bad manners, incompetence and provincialism. (5) Michele Sucabrodu – or the phoney DJ (Café Filsbach, 8.2.1999) 1
Carmine: Michele hats gesagt (...) ‘It’s Michael who said that (...)’
2
Gio:
3
Carmine: Michele Mangiaficudín ‘Michael Cactusfigeater’
4
Carmine: des is kein DJ ‘he’s not a DJ’
5
Gio:
#LAUGHS AND APPLAUDS#
6
Pino:
ma Italy DJ ‘but (an) Italy DJ’
7
Carmine: ein DJ mit CD ‘a DJ (operating) with CD’
8
Silvia:
> >
calmati signorino * > senti< ‘calm down young man * >listen< ’
In this case of intra-ethnic conflict talk, the unacceptable behavior of a member of the Italian community who gossips about group members is implicitly categorized by the attribution of pejorative mezzogiorno peasant labels such as Sucabrodu and Mangiaficudín to mark social distance. It should be noted that these names are used here in the same combination as in example (4), but by two speakers (Gio and Carmine) complementing each other, as if mentioning one name triggered the other, indicating that these belong to a shared repertoire of indexical cultural formulae.16 Such emblematic names seem to function as prototypical labels of a well-defined
Names and Identities
139
range of sociosymbolical meanings evident to all group members. In the context of the hip hop scene, labelling someone a ‘backward Italian Southerner’ draws a demarcation line between ‘us’ and ‘him’, in this case between ‘authentic’ and ‘unauthentic’ hip hoppers or DJs. The critique of a compaesano’s bad manners is thus extended to his lack of professional (or artistic) competence in the urban hip hop world, suggesting that such a character could hardly be a good DJ, but merely an ‘Italy (i.e. ‘provincial’?) DJ’ (l. 6) – a case of dissing. With this categorization of the adversary, the boys implicitly place themselves ‘on the right side’: they know what a real hip hop DJ is and how he is supposed to behave as a friend. In opposition to the boys participating in the dissing sequence, one of the girls (Silvia, l. 8), reprehends the most vehement speaker, switching to Italian and addressing him by the honorific signorino. This is a rather archaic term of address for young upper class men in Southern Italy which, used among equals, and specially when addressing a working class boy, can only be used to mark irony and/or disapproval. In this context, Silvia’s turn, formulated in standard Italian which is unusual in in-group communication, ironically ratifies the social distance the boys construct between themselves and their adversary, and evokes at the same time the inappropriateness of the quarrel, i.e. cues a reprimand: this is a futile case of male rivalry.
6.
Boys talking, girls joining? Communicative style and gendered identities in the peer group
The synthesis of rustic, backward and masculine (or macho) values inherent in emblematic names like Mangiaficu etc. tends to be attached to Sicilian and mezzogiorno culture in general. This is certainly one of the main reasons that most of the girls in the group reject the dialect. Some of them explicitly call it ‘a peasant language’ and claim not to use it themselves, unless they have to communicate with elder relatives, or during holidays ‘back home’. In some of the recorded conversations, the girls label the boys masculi siciliani when they criticize their macho behavior, thus confirming the ethnicity-masculinity identity cluster illustrated above. In fact, all the examples discussed so far are similar in that they are performed by male group members. Considering the overall communicative behavior of boys and girls in our data, we note that it follows clearly gendered patterns: boys show off, boast, and provoke, while girls join in as secondary speakers and evaluate the boys’ performances either implicitly
140
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
(e.g. by laughter) or explicitly (e.g. with critical comments, as in example 5). In the encounters we were able to document, the girls were much less inclined to perform ludic or fictitious presentations of self and preferred ‘straight’ communication when talking about themselves and the group.17 One of the rare exceptions where several girls participate in a joking round will be presented below (example 8).18 We do not claim that these observations reflect general gender patterns in the Italian migrant community. Though it is known that migration tends to favor traditional cultural patterns, it also encourages particularly the female members of migrant communities to break away from (disadvantageous) gender roles and the cultural patterns that sustain them. 19 Our assumption is that the recurrent communicative patterns we find in our data are due first to the mixed group setting (and particularly breakdance, a usually male-dominated subculture) and, second, the age group of (post-)adolescence where ‘doing gender’ is a specifically relevant issue. Considering these aspects, many of the performative self-presentations of the young men can be interpreted as ‘gendered’ reactions to the presence of young females, heightened by the participation of a young female Sicilian fieldworker. A more or less spectacular mise-en-scène of both masculinity and ethnicity thus functions as a method to compete for female attention and to gain recognition among male companions. Also, the following examples belong to a type of exclusively male game in our group, similar to the facetious verbal duels reported in research on urban youth subculture (see below). While in the preceding examples provocation was mainly directed towards outsiders, the following teasing sequences, based on funny and rather downgrading rhymes derived from participants’ first names, seem to challenge internal group status and thus are a means of competing for (the girls’?) attention. 6.1. Names and teasing rituals Teasing based on interactants’ names seems to be a universally cherished practice in children’s and teenage peer groups as a ludic method to construct social identities and express in-group relations. Lytra (2003) found that nicknames and teasing are closely related, nicknames being mostly used to frame teasing formats in the pre-adolescent peer group she studied. Lepoutre (1997), who observed teasing practices as part of the ritual verbal behavior of young immigrant boys in a Parisian suburb, classifies teasing
Names and Identities
141
based on members’ names among the two most salient verbal games in this milieu. He calls them vannes directes because they address co-present parties, whereas vannes référenciées refer to non-present thirds (mostly ritual insults of the mother) and correspond to the signifying practices or ‘dozens’ described by Labov (1972). Contrary to real insults, the performance of vannes is marked as a ritual format and thus not considered an offence, but rather a socially accepted practice in male peer groups (Lepoutre 1997: 173–180, 206–213), although they too can be quite rude and often question the opponent’s virility or gender identity. Besides teasing formulae referring to physical properties of the opponent, second meanings and rhymes based on first or family names are frequent. Lepoutre suggests that such teasing practices, by repetition and conventionalization, might in fact be at the origin of nicknames (sobriquets)20 as socially meaningful identifiers of peer group members. This type of vannes directes, or teasing rituals based on names, appears in our data particularly in very informal in-group settings such as the bus journey mentioned above (example 2). In the following extract, the principal target is Giovanni, the most dominant group member. The episode occurs shortly after his microphone performances which we discussed above. (6) Teasing rituals: Challenging the leader (Journey to Paris, 8.6.2001) 1
Lino:
jaja Giovanni du-u bis n g/Kakadu #LAUGHS # ‘yeah, Giovanni you- / are a cockatoo’ #LAUGHS #
2
F:
#INCOMPREHENSIBLE #
3
Gio:
va caccari ‘go and shit’
4
Sara
#LAUGHS #
5
Lino:
Kakadu Giovanni macapu #LAUGHS # ‘cockatoo Giovanni macapu’ #LAUGHS #
6
Gio:
Giovanni (mi (...) tu?) ‘Giovanni (me (#UNINTERPRETABLE#) you?)’
7
Lino:
Giovanni cacca li banni ‘Giovanni shits in his diapers’
8
(all):
#LAUGHTER#
142 9
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman Lino:
Giovanni cacca otto anni ‘Giovanni shits eight years’
10 X:
#INCOMPREHENSIBLE #
11 Y:
#INCOMPREHENSIBLE #
12 F:
Giovanni senza panni ‘Giovanni without his diapers’
13 (all):
#LAUGHTER#
14 Lino:
ma chi dici #Giovanni CU i panni# ‘but what are you saying #Giovanni WITH diapers #’ # LAUGHING #
K 15 Gio:
ca ü’hai i palli ‘that’s where you have your balls’
This teasing sequence is started by Lino, next to Gio the most dominant and active speaker in this episode. He opens up a rhyming format addressed to a ‘target’ interactant who is addressed by his first name. Note that the first rhyme is actually not based on the name itself, but uses a postponed personal pronoun in order to rhyme with Kakadu (‘cockatoo’). This first turn is thus formulated in German. Gio, however, replies in Sicilian, picking up the first syllable of the attributed noun which – certainly on purpose – contains an assonance to the Italian (and German homophone) cacca (‘shit’). This scatological reference becomes the basis of the following series of rhymes with Giovanni (hence in Italian) in which mainly Lino, but also other male group members, participate. This seems to be a rather childish game, with its constant reference to faces, certainly a main resource of children’s humor (cf. Bierbach 1996), but as Còveri (1993) found, it is a central feature of Italian youth language as well, which he calls coprolalia. The effect of this coprolalia sequence is to put the target person on an infantile level, made even more explicit by reference to diapers (panni, l. 7), i.e. attacking Gio’s status as a young man and a group leader. The game ends with Gio’s reply to Lino’s insisting on presenting him in diapers (l. 14), which gives the teasing a sexual turn, focussing on Lino’s masculinity, which infantilizes him in return (l. 15). The reactions of the group members (laughter, increasing participation of other – unidentified – boys) show that this game, playfully questioning a leader’s status by bringing up age and gender identity, is quite successful.
Names and Identities
143
Similar yet less extensive formats are addressed to other group members. They contain rhymed nonsense attributes, often scatological allusions and formulaic expressions in Sicilian or German, and seem to be drawn from a common (group) repertoire which makes them sometimes hard to understand for outsiders.21 In other cases, names of group members are inserted in song texts, converting them to (pseudo-)romantic lovers in stereotypical Mediterranean settings, or similarly in fictitious (and mostly ‘dirty’) narrative sequences, co-constructed by two or more boys. 6.2. Names, teasing and membership The more or less ritualized performative acts which involve members’ names contextualize aspects of male adolescent identity. They contribute to testing status as well as expressing membership in the peer group. Moreover, there are other forms of nicknaming, used in less marked interactional formats among the boys, e.g. in-group terms of address, such as cornuto (‘cuckold’) in Italian, but most of them in German, e.g. du Fisch (‘fish’, with a pejorative or sexual connotation) du Affe (‘monkey’), and Kacker, Waldkacker, Kakalake22 (‘shitter’, ‘forest shitter’, ‘cockroach’) based on coprolalia. The most neutral and most frequently used term among the ‘big boys’ (Lino, Pino and Gio) is Langer (‘tall one’). They differ from individual nicknames insofar as they can apply to any of the group members; some of them (Langer, Alter) are just common youth language, but the more original ones (like Kakerlake) might also be promoted to a personal nickname. Despite their rudeness, these forms of address seem to express nothing but peer relations and male companionship; together with the more spectacular performative formats they make up an essential part of the boys communicative group style. Moreover, the use of dominantly German expressions produces a sort of de-ethnization and ties them to the local urban youth culture. In contrast, Sicilian name varieties – like Giuvà (Giovanni), Carmè (Carmelo), Francè (Francesco), appear to be more marked and can be used to perform a Sicilian persona, e.g. to mimic the speech of (elder) relatives or other members of the Italian community, as in the typical greeting formula: ou Giuvà, tuttu appostu? (oh G., ev’rything all right?), quoted by Lino to evoke a Sicilian relative.
144
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
Up to now, we have found only one short instance where a girl’s name gives rise to a short teasing–nicknaming sequence, addressed to Sara and alluding to her Sicilian background: (7) Sara (Journey to Paris, 8.6.2001) 1
F:
lass die SaRahaRa (#INCOMPREHENSIBLE #) ‘leave SaRahaRa (in peace)’ (#INCOMPREHENSIBLE #)
2
(all):
#LAUGHTER#
3
Gio:
SaRahaRa ‘SaRahaRa’
4
Lino:
> >
5
Gio:
Salermo ‘Salermo’
The joke is obviously based on the resemblance between Sara and Sahara, additionally highlighted by a very salient apical (= Italian) pronunciation of the /R/. After several repetitions, Lino adds a Sicilian toponym as ‘family name’, both being finally fused into Salermo. This not very elaborate nicknaming format is interesting insofar as it focuses on Sara’s Sicilian identity, which she often asserts and puts into practice by the ostentative use of the dialect (unlike the other girls in the group). Hence the teasing might be interpreted as mocking her patriotism. But more than that it demonstrates that she is integrated into the group by the bonding effect of nicknames and teasing (Lytra 2003: 48).
7.
Toponymy and social status
As the last examples have shown, teasing practices in the group relate to internal status and membership as well as to different aspects of social identity involving age, gender and ethnicity (with the social values these imply). The last conversational example we would like to discuss exploits toponymy and local references as a resource for joking about participants’ social status. As much as personal names or nicknames, local references expressed by toponyms can function as highly symbolic indicators (Le-
Names and Identities
145
poutre 1997; Melliani 2000), concerning place of residence (stigmatized vs. prestigious neighborhoods in a city, rural vs. urban areas etc.) as well as places where people (pretend to) spend their holidays (long distance and exoticism produce increasing prestige). The following sequence is interesting not only because, contrary to most of our conversational data, girls participate actively in the joking, but also because it demonstrates participants’ acute awareness of social categorization and social positioning. It therefore contradicts recent studies on youth culture and identity, according to which social class is hard to detect as a relevant category in young people’s conversation (Androutsopoulos and Georgakopoulou 2003: 5). (8) Samalú: globetrotting around the neighborhood (15.7.2000) 1
Pino:
> letztes Jahr in Samalú > last year in Samalú’
2
Lino:
wo? ‘where?’
3
Pino:
Samalú ‘Samalú’
4
Francesco: Sandhofen Mannheim Lu/ Ludwigshafen ‘Sandhofen Mannheim Lu/ Ludwigshafen’
5
GK
# LAUGHTER#
6
Gio:
#Samalú # #‘Samalú’# #SINGING#
K 7
s:
# in Samalú # #‘in Samalú’#
8
K
# LAUGHING#
9
Mari:
Terrasien ‘Terracia’
10 Pino:
des is Tarrazien, wo warn wir noch? ‘this is Tarrazia, where else were we?’
11 Gio:
isch war auch/ ‘I was also/’
146
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
12 Mari:
Balkonien ‘Balconia’
13 Pino:
konien ‘konia’
14 Gio:
wir fahrn doch hin oder? ‘will we go there or not?’
15 Pino:
he? ‘huh?’
16 Gio:
wir fahrn nach ding äh/ ‘we’ll go to (thing äh) /’
17 Silvia:
Fenstonien ‘Window-nia’
18 Pino:
+ onien Fenstonien + ‘ow-nia Window-nia’
19 Gio:
wo is H fünf Mannheim ‘where H five Mannheim is at’
20 GK
# LAUGHTER #
21 Gio:
da um die Ecke beim | Dönerladen ‘at the corner where the | döner shop is at
22 Lino:
| |’
| oa weißt noch | weißt noch? ‘| oah do you still remember| do you still remember?’
23 Gio:
+ wa/ + ‘wha/’
24 Lino:
+ des ah diese äh da quannu c'e(ra) die eine kleine Blonde "hey wir warn schon überall in Eu/ wir warn schon überall in Europa" "ja ja Australien Amerika"
25
+ ‘that ah this äh there when there was that small blonde girl “hey we've already been all over Eu/ we've already been all over Europe” “oh yes Australia America”’
26 GK
# LAUGHTER #
27 Gio:
in Europa (...) ‘in Europe (...)’
28 GK
# LAUGHTER #
Names and Identities
147
In this conversation about the summer holidays, the (fictitious) toponym Samalú introduces the point of the joke already at the beginning, making the narrative opening a sort of riddle, the solution to which is given by the co-narrator (Francesco). The exotic sound, suggested by (open) syllable structure and word-final accentuation, stands in sharp contrast to the trivial reality since it turns out to be an acronym of a working class neighborhood (Sandhofen) and the two neighboring industrial towns (Mannheim, Ludwigshafen). This is in fact the area where the young people live, a low prestige region in terms of lifestyle, environmental qualities and leisure commodities. The success of the verbal creation is several times ratified by repetition and laughter (Gio’s singing version in line 6 underlines the ‘exotic’ sound of the name) and leads to an expansion of the format. From l.9 to l.17, Mari and Silvia cooperate, proposing German pseudo-regionyms which ironically refer to ‘staying at home during holidays’. The potential loss of face which the evoked situation implies is compensated for by the ludic transformation of words referring to trivial places at home (terrace, balcony, window) into pseudo-regionyms, using the corresponding derivational suffix (-ien). The conventionalized model of the series is Terrasien, to which the girls add Balkonien and Fenstonien. Up to l.18 the boys intervene only as secondary speakers,23 until Gio proposes additional – socially and subsequently also ethnically marked – localizations: H5, a low prestige inner city neighborhood, ‘the döner shop’ as an emblematic place related to immigration and popular, low priced food (l.19). As a conversational coproduction all these contributions give a perfect illustration of what the acronym Samalú means to the group, and how living and spending holidays there is perceived socially. In the third part of this episode, the first speaker, Lino (l.22), adds a further expansion, a narrative containing a (fictitious?) boasting dialogue with a ‘blonde girl’ (representing mainstream society), which stylizes the boys (himself and Gio) as experienced world travellers. Laughter confirms the unrealistic contents of the performed narrative. In this sequence, the social meaning of the message is exclusively cued by the emblematic toponyms, evoking clearly defined social values shared by all participants.
148
8.
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
Conclusion: How to do things with names
As we have shown in this article, personal names, pseudonyms or nicknames play an important role in the construction and performance of social identities. This is due to their emblematic potential and their capacity to evoke different cultural contexts as well as different social features of a person in a condensed form. The list of code names attested for the Italian breakdancers and their peers gave a first clue of the range of cultural references relevant to the group. At the same time, the (plurilingual) diversity of the names shows how all kinds of linguistic and cultural (re-)sources are used in name creation, from brand and product names to literary characters and, most often, prominent public personalities from the world of mass media and show business. We then showed how fictitious, adopted, or variants of real names are exploited, together with language or dialect variation (code-switching), to contextualize and perform specific communicative genres, social characters and cultural milieus, such as the world of hip hop or the media. These scenarios contribute to the construction of modern urban identities and thus enhance the image of the interactants. At the same time, the boys’ parodies of the stereotypical Southern macho (Mangiaficu), performed in Sicilian dialect, serve to establish boundaries and to protect their territory against outsiders as well as to disqualify ‘unworthy’ members of the Italian community. What is of special interest in these episodes is the syncretism of ethnic, socio-cultural and gender identities. Our data represent particularly male adolescent identity constructions and the communicative style of a male peer group in the presence of and presented to female members as an audience, and is thus an additional strong incentive for spectacular performative acts. The very caricatured presentation of the ‘Southern macho’ as a sort of ‘ethno-comedy’ (Kotthoff 2004) is also a subversive reply to mainstream prejudices. Social categories like age and gender (more than ethnicity) were shown to have an important part in teasing rituals centered around members’ first names. In this respect, our observations coincide with research findings on adolescent peer groups, e.g. in France and Greece. Our focus on naming practices is not meant to suggest that this is the only communicative device, or contextualization cue, with which identities and sociosymbolical meanings are constructed. The data show that there is always a range of linguistic and pragmatic resources which are exploited simultaneously, such as code-switching or other types of code variation, or
Names and Identities
149
sequential and formal properties contextualizing communicative genres or formats. The choice of examples we presented might give an insight into the methods and the material resources young second generation immigrants use to construct sociocultural identities related to relevant aspects of their social world by creating a communicative style that makes them recognizable as a peer group and confirms them as legitimate members of ‘glocal’ urban culture.
Notes 1. Among others, and specifically with reference to Italian and identity construction, cf. di Luzio 1984; Auer and di Luzio 1986; for contextualization in general, Auer and di Luzio (eds.) 1992. 2. Sending the children to school in Italy for a couple of years is in fact one of the reasons why perform poorly at school in Germany. 3. Pisces, one of the astrological signs. 4. The historical origin of the name, i.e. the popular nickname for Mary Queen of Scots (Maria Stuart), is probably not known by the group members. 5. Claudia Schiffer is a German top model, very present in the media at that time. 6. Cf. Carmelo, 8.2.99: Isch bin stolz auf den Namen, wir ham ein nationalen Namen genomm von unsre Nationalität (‘I’m proud of that name, we chose a national name from our nationality’). 7. It is probably also an intertextual reference to popular film titles, i.e. the series of ‘space invasion’ movies in the 90s; cf. Bierbach and Birken-Silverman 2002a. 8. It is not always possible to distinguish between a ‘code name’ in terms of hiphop culture (i.e. a self chosen ‘pseudonym’ for crew members, similar to artists’ names in the world of show business) and nicknames, which are usually attributed by other group members or friends (in Italian, soprannomi, or nomignoli, cf. Ruffino 1988), but can also, as in the case of internet communication, be created by the user himself as a pseudonym (cf. Wilhelms 2002). 9. Cf. Ruffino 1988 for a detailed classification of Sicilian soprannomi, according to form and function. The names created by our migrant adolescents differ from the traditional ones in the sense that they are not always dialectal and because of their international references, but correspond in this latter aspect to recent trends among young people in Sicily, as reported by Paternostro and Sottile 2005. 10. Such a procedure is rather reminiscent of school or other institutional settings, but is also possible in interethnic encounters and evokes in any case a situation that is familiar to the participants.
150
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
11. It is remarkable how a very subtle cue such as the insertion of a French clitic pronoun into a Sicilian dialect utterance is perceived by the addressee and used to turn a previous communicative failure and potential loss of face into a joke: self irony demonstrates being ‘a good sport’ and allows the speaker to keep the last word. 12. The expression geil (‘sexually aroused’) has lost its original meaning and is widely used as a qualifying adjective or, often, as an interjection (almost synonymous with cool); however, its use is still perceived as being appropriate only in youth in-group communication. 13. Figs and broth evoke rustic and easily available food in the mezzogiorno; at the same time, fico viz. fica and succare have obscene sexual connotations. 14. The whole sequence is too long to be reproduced here; for a more detailed presentation and discussion cf. Bierbach and Birken-Silverman 2002b. 15. The ‘dissing’ sequence ends comparing the woman with the British comedy figure Mr. Bean, with ‘Mr. Bean’s wife’, and with ‘Fantozzi’s daughter’, an Italian comedy figure represented by a stout male actor, i.e. funny and awkward comedy figures very familiar to adolescents. 16. The subtle semantic variation – Mangiaficu (fig eater) > Mangiaficudín (cactus fig eater) – reinforces the pejorative value of the name, as cactus figs (< fichi d' India) are characterized by thorns and are picked by shepherds in the country, and are therefore associated with poor people’s nutrition in Sicily. 17. In one of the recorded conversations, where the boys mostly fooled around, one of the girls tells the interviewer: “Wir Mädels ham eigntlisch nur die Wahrheit gesagt …” (‘Us girls just told the truth’). 18. Particularly revealing with respect to gendered patterns is an interactional episode of a playful interview performed by the boys in the role of the interviewer and the girls as the interviewees, discussed in detail in Bierbach and Birken-Silverman 2004. 19. This has been confirmed by recent empirical research on different populations, cf. Bednarz-Braun and Heß-Meinig 2004. 20. This is actually the procedure which is at the historical origin of family names, cf. Koß 1990. 21. We were not able, for instance, to find out the meaning and the source of the expression magapu, repeated over and over in this episode. 22. “Aber des is zweideutig gemeint” (‘but with a double meaning’), i.e. with a scatological connotation like the preceding. 23. Possibly the variant Tarrazien, introduced by Pino as a repair, gives a more Italianized version of the place name.
Names and Identities
151
References Androutsopoulos, Jannis 1998 Deutsche Jugendsprache: Untersuchungen zu ihren Strukturen und Funktionen. Frankfurt/Main: Peter Lang. Androutsopoulos, Jannis and Alexandra Georgakopoulou (eds.) 2003 Discourse Constructions of Youth Identities (Pragmatics & Beyond 110). Amsterdam: Benjamins. Auer, Peter 1986 Kontextualisierung. Studium Linguistik 19, 22–47. Auer, Peter and Aldo di Luzio 1986 Identitätskonstitution in der Migration: konversationsanalytische und linguistische Aspekte ethnischer Stereotypisierungen. Linguistische Berichte 104, 327–351. Auer, Peter and Aldo di Luzio (eds.) 1984 Interpretive Sociolinguistics: Migrants, Children, Migrant Children. Tübingen: Narr. 1992 The Contextualization of Language. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins. Bauman, Richard 1978 Verbal Art as Performance. Rowley, MA: Newbury House. 2000 Language, identity, performance. Pragmatics 10(1), 1–5. Bausch, Constanze and Stephan Sting 2001 Rituelle Medieninszenierungen in Peergroups. In: Wulf, Christoph et al. (eds.), Das Soziale als Ritual. Zur performativen Bildung von Gemeinschaften. Opladen: Leske und Budrich, 249–323. Bednarz-Braun, Iris and Sigrid Heß-Meinig 2004 Migration, Ethnie und Geschlecht. Theorieansätze – Forschungsstand – Forschungsperspektiven. Wiesbaden: Verlag für Sozialwissenschaft. Bierbach, Christine 1996 Chi non caca un kilo zahlt 20 Mark Strafe. Witze von Kindern zwischen zwei Kulturen. In: Kotthoff, Helga (ed.), Das Gelächter der Geschlechter, 2nd ed. Constance: Universitätsverlag, 247–273. Bierbach, Christine and Gabriele Birken-Silverman 2002a Kommunikationsstil und sprachliche Symbolisierung in einer Gruppe italienischer Migrantenjugendlicher aus der HipHop-Szene in Mannheim. In: Keim, Inken and Wildfried Schütte (eds.), Soziale Welten und kommunikative Stile. Tübingen: Narr, 187–215. 2002b Le parler glocal des jeunes immigrés italiens à Mannheim. Paper presented at the Colloque “Variation, catégorisation, et pratiques discursives”, Université de Paris III, 12–14 September 2002.
152
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
2004
Inszenierte männliche Anmache und ‘Migranten-Girlies’. Das Gelächter der Geschlechter in einer Gruppe italienischer Migrantenjugendlicher. Deutsche Sprache. Zeitschrift für Theorie, Praxis, Dokumentation 32(3), 240–269. Birken-Silverman, Gabriele 2003 ‚Isch bin New School und West Coast… du bisch doch ebe bei de Southside Rockern’: Identität und Sprechstil in einer BreakdanceGruppe von Mannheimer Italienern. In: Androutsopoulos, Jannis (ed.), HipHop. Globale Kultur – lokale Praktiken. Bielefeld: Transcript, 273–297. Bühler, Karl 1934 Sprachtheorie. Die Darstellungsfunktion der Sprache. Jena: G. Fischer. Butler, Judith 1990 Gender Trouble: Feminism and the Subversion of Identity. New York: Routledge. Cameron, Deborah 1997 Performing gender identity: Young men’s talk and the construction of heterosexual masculinity. In: Johnson, Sally and Ulrike H. Meinhof (eds.), Language and Masculinity. London: Blackwell, 47–64. Chambers, Iain 1996 Migration, Kultur, Identität. Tübingen: Stauffenburg. Còveri, Lorenzo 1993 Novità del/sul linguaggio giovanile. In: Radtke, Edgar (ed.), La lingua dei giovani. Tübingen: Narr, 35–48. Di Luzio, Aldo 1984 On the meaning of language alternation for the sociocultural identity of Italian migrant-children. In: Auer, Peter and Aldo di Luzio (eds.), Interpretive Sociolinguistics: Migrants, Children, Migrant Children. Tübingen: Narr, 55–85. Goffman, Erving 1959 The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Gueli Alletti, Marilene 2003 “Familekt” in einer sizilianischen Migrantenfamilie in Mannheim: Zweisprachige strukturelle und konversationelle Muster. Unpublished MA thesis, University of Mannheim. Gumperz, John J. 1982 Discourse Strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1992 Contextualization revisited. In: Auer, Peter and Aldo di Luzio (eds.), The Contextualization of Language (Pragmatics & Beyond 22). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins, 39–54.
Names and Identities
153
Habermas, Jürgen 1981 Theorie des kommunikativen Handelns. Frankfurt/Main: Suhrkamp. Hu, Adelheid 2003 Mehrsprachigkeitsforschung, Identitäts- und Kulturtheorie: Tendenzen der Konvergenz. In: De Florio-Hansen, Inez and Adelheid Hu (eds.), Plurilingualität und Identität. Zur Selbst- und Fremdwahrnehmung mehrsprachiger Menschen. Tübingen: Stauffenburg, 1–24. Kallmeyer, Werner and Inken Keim 2003 Linguistic variation and the construction of social identity in a German–Turkish setting: A case study of an immigrant youth group in Mannheim, Germany. In: Androutsopoulos, Jannis and Alexandra Georgakopoulou (eds.), Discourse Constructions of Youth Identities. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 29–47. Keupp, Heiner, Thomas Ahbe, Wolfgang Gmür, Renate Höfer, Beate Mitzerlisch, Wolfgang Kraus, and Florian Straus 1999 [2002] Identitätskonstruktionen. Das Patchwork der Identitäten in der Spätmoderne. Hamburg: Rowohlt. Koß, Gerhard 1990 Namenforschung. Eine Einführung in die Onomastik. Tübingen: Niemeyer. Kotthoff, Helga 2004 Overdoing culture? Sketch-Komik, Typenstilisierung und Identitätskonstruktion bei Kaya Yanar. In: Reuter, Julian (ed.), Doing Culture. Bielefeld: Transcript. Labov, William 1972 Language in the Inner City: Studies in the Black English Vernacular. Philadephia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Le Page, Robert and Andrée Tabouret-Keller 1985 Acts of Identity. Creole based Approaches to Language and Ethnicity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lepoutre, David 1997 Coeur de banlieue. Codes, rites et langages. Paris: Odile Jacob. Lytra, Vally 2003 Nicknames and teasing: A case study of a linguistically and culturally mixed peer-group. In: Androutsopoulos, Jannis and Alexandra Georgakopoulou (eds.), Discourse Constructions of Youth Identities. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 47–74. Melliani, Fabienne 2000 La langue du quartier. Appropriation de l’espace et identités urbaines chez des jeunes issus de l’immigration maghrébine en banlieue rouennaise. Paris: L’Harmattan.
154
Christine Bierbach and Gabriele Birken-Silverman
Paternostro, Giuseppe and Roberto Sottile 2005 L’antroponimia giovanile tra nickname e ncúria. Un indagine in area palermitana. Paper presented at the Convegno “Giovani, lingue e dialetti”, Sappada/Plodn, 29 giugno – 3 luglio 2005. Robertson, Roland 1995 Glocalization: Time–space and homogeneity–heterogeneity. In: Featherstone, Mike, Scott Lash, and Roland Robertson (eds.), Global Modernities. London: Sage, 25–44. Ruffino, Giovanni 1988 Soprannomi della Sicilia occidentale. Onomata. Revue Onomastique 12, 480–486. Wilhelms, Nike 2002 Gästebuchkommunikation italienischer HipHop-Fans im Internet. Unpublished MA thesis, University of Mannheim.
Chapter 6 Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time: A case study of a group of German– Turkish girls in Mannheim/Germany Inken Keim 1.
Aim of the study
In this paper, I present some aspects of a youth group’s construction of a communicative style and show how the group’s stylistic repertoire changes over the course of their growing into adulthood. My paper is based on an ethnographic case study of a group of Turkish girls, the ‘Powergirls’, who grew up in a typical Turkish migrant neighborhood in the inner city of Mannheim, Germany.1 The aim of the case study was, on the basis of biographical interviews with group members and long-term observation of group interactions, to reconstruct the formation of an ethnically defined ‘ghetto’-clique and its style of communication and to describe the group’s development into educated, modern, German–Turkish young women. In this process, a change in the group’s stylistic repertoire could be observed. I will analyze the group’s socio-cultural identity in terms of its communicative style. From my perspective, identity is not to be regarded as an ‘essential’ phenomenon representing a predictive or explanatory variable to human behaviour as it is, for example, in social identity theory (e.g. Tajfel 1978). Following the conversation analyst’s perspective as it is outlined in Antaki and Widdicombe (1998a and b), I argue that identity is something that is produced in interaction. The analysis of identity is concerned with its occasioned relevance ‘here’ and ‘now’ and with its consequences for the interaction and the local purposes of interlocutors. From this perspective, the construction of socio-cultural identity is part of the routine of everyday life and everyday interaction, where identities can be produced in order to affiliate with or to disaffiliate from relevant others and relevant social groups (e.g. Widdicombe and Wooffitt 1995). In the following analysis, style is regarded as a central means of expression of the ‘Powergirls’ socio-cultural identity. From this perspective, the
156
Inken Keim
construction of a genuine peer group style is motivated by key experiences of social life, and the choice of stylistic features is closely related to the group’s self-conception and their positioning in relation to relevant others. Stylistic transformations that can be observed in the process of the girls’ growing into adulthood are conceptualized as indices to their changing selfconception at different phases of their lives. After a short outline of the present migration situation in Germany and a short characterization of the socio-cultural context in which the peer-group formation took place (2), I will present the concept of style as it is applied in this paper and focus on those stylistic aspects which are constitutive for style construction (3). In the following sections (4 and 5), some of the features that are constitutive of the Powergirls’ peer group style are presented in more detail. The final sections focus on the gradual stylistic changes in the course of the girls’ growing into adulthood and the widening of their stylistic repertoire, first in out-group (6) and then in in-group communication (7).
2.
The ‘Powergirls’ migration context
Migration from Mediterranean countries to Germany began after the erection of the Iron Curtain and of the Berlin Wall. From the late 1960s onwards, German industry needed workers for skilled and unskilled jobs. ‘Guest workers’ were recruited, especially from Italy, Spain, former Yugoslavia, and Turkey. Since the guest workers’ residence in Germany was planned for only a short period of time, a temporary residence permit as well as a temporary work permit restricted their legal and social status. But gradually, the guest workers’ stay became longer and longer; the workers brought their wives and children, who grew up in Germany and went to German schools. Many migrant families have been living in Germany for over 30 years, and most of their children view Germany as their home country. In the course of time, migrant ‘ghettos’ emerged and stabilized in many inner city districts. Preschool institutions and schools were and still are badly equipped for the instruction of children from various cultural and linguistic backgrounds. Many teachers saw and still see migrant children as double semi-linguals with serious deficits. A high percentage of migrant children are not successful in school and have few opportunities on the job market.
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
157
Out of frustration with their children’s educational and professional failure and out of fear that they would become more and more estranged from ‘their culture’, many Turkish parents tried to educate their children with increasing rigidity along their own traditional norms and values. One of the central problems for young migrants has been coming to terms with their parents’ traditional demands and, at the same time, experiencing failure in and exclusion from more advanced educational and professional worlds in Germany. The children’s ability to cope with often contrasting traditions and demands from different social worlds is fundamental in the process of forming their own socio-cultural identity. The ethnographic research on which this paper is based was carried out in an inner city district of Mannheim, an industrial town of 320,000 inhabitants in southwestern Germany. Over 21% of Mannheim’s population are migrants,2 most of them of Turkish origin. The district under study, traditionally a working class district, has a migrant population of over 60%; it is called a ‘migrant ghetto’ by inhabitants of the district as well as by outsiders. The Turkish population has a highly organized infrastructure and lives in close networks where Turkish or ‘migrant Turkish’ (see below) is the dominant language.3 In everyday life, standard German is not necessary, and most children come into contact with it, for the first time, in preschool with their German teachers. Since up to 100% of the preschool children have a migration background, they soon begin to develop bilingual practices, code-switching and code-mixing, as well as morphologically and lexically reduced German learner varieties mixed with elements from other languages. When they start school, their competence in standard German – a precondition in the monolingually oriented German school – is not very high. Up till now, the district’s primary schools have not succeeded to build upon the children’s bilingual abilities and to foster their proficiency in standard German. As a consequence, most migrant children are not very successful in school. In the German school system, children have options between three school types at the end of primary school (at the age of ten): children with the best marks go to the Gymnasium; others go to the Realschule, a more practically-oriented school type, and children with low marks go to the Hauptschule, the lowest secondary school type with a very negative image. Because of their low school marks, most migrant children have only one choice, the Hauptschule. So, in the course of time, the Hauptschule of the district has become a school for migrant children, where 90% of the pupils have a migration background. Teachers adjusted to this situation by reduc-
158
Inken Keim
ing their educational standards with the consequence that it has become even more difficult for migrant children to succeed in schools outside the migrant district. One of the findings of our ethnographic research is that migrant children develop different socio-cultural orientations and communication practices depending on their school careers.4 Going to the Hauptschule for a tenyear-old child implies (since the Hauptschule is situated in the migrant district) that he/she will grow up in an environment and in peer groups where German–Turkish mixing or highly marked ethnolectal German varieties are the normal ‘codes of interaction’.5 When the adolescents complete the Hauptschule at the age of 15 with low marks or without a qualification,6 as 25–30% of the students do, they have almost no opportunity to obtain a professional qualification.7 These youths typically develop an antieducational and non-professional orientation. They align with other migrant peer groups, where members are proud to be a school failure, engage in sports or music, and wish to become a good football player, boxer, hip hopper, or break dancer. They typically marry partners from their parents’ home villages and live with them in the migrant district. Children of the district who, at the age of ten, have the chance to go to the Gymnasium or the Realschule (10–20% of an age-group) develop quite different social orientations. Since both types of schools are situated outside the district, the children have to enter German educational worlds where migrants are a small minority. For the first time in their lives, they experience the negative image of the Turkish migrants in terms of abuse such as scheiß ausländer (‘fucking foreigner’) and dreckiger (‘dirty’) or dummer Türke (‘stupid Turk’).8 In these schools, they have to cope with new educational, linguistic, and social standards for which they usually are not prepared. A typical reaction to these experiences is the organization of an ethnically defined peer group along with the dissociation from or the upgrading of ethnic features. There is a third educational career: with a good Hauptschule-diploma, adolescents have the option to attend various Fachschulen and obtain a qualification that enables them to go to Fachoberschule and later on perhaps even to a university. German teachers call this career der langsame Weg (‘the slow path’). They recommend it to those migrant children who, from their perspective, have an ‘ability to learn’ but do not yet have the necessary competence in German. Pupils with this school career live in the migrant district until the age of 15. After that, they, too, have to enter
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
159
school worlds outside the migrant district, where they encounter similar experiences as the other children. The ‘Powergirls’ belong to the small portion of district children who were quite successful at school. Some of the girls went to the Realschule or Gymnasium at the age of 10; others took ‘the slow path’. So, in the course of their educational career, all of the girls had to leave the migrant district, some very early, the others later on at the age of 15. The formation of an ethnic group started not long after some of the girls attended the Gymnasium. Here, they experienced the Schock des Lebens (‘shock of their lives’) because they were not up to the new linguistic, educational, and social demands. Their parents could not help them, and, since they were too ashamed or too proud to ask for assistance from their German peers or teachers, they felt helpless, alone, and excluded. Trying to understand their situation, they soon arrived at an ethnic interpretation and considered their ‘Turkish-ness’ to be the reason for failing in school and for being excluded by their German peers. At the age of 12 or 13, they joined with other Turkish girls, formed an ethnic group, and called themselves ‘Turkish Powergirls’. On the one hand, they struggled against the German school world, where they felt marginalized and excluded, and, on the other hand, they revolted against their parents’ educational principles, especially against the traditional Turkish female role, since they had been exposed to other female models in their new surroundings. Gradually, the group developed into a wild, aggressive ethnic clique that even became criminal for a period. As the girls grew older, they started to visit one of the district’s youth centers where, at least, they found help with their school problems and new models for their further social, educational, and professional development. That was the time when I first met the ‘Powergirls’. I had the opportunity to observe them over a longer period of time and to document their gradual development.9 The main topics in the group’s discussions were the girls’ relationships to their families and the Turkish community and their experiences in schools outside the migrant district. In the course of these discussions, a new social identity emerged (see Keim 2002). The ‘Powergirls’, who up till then had defined themselves as a rebellious ‘Turkish’ group, gradually came to see themselves as something ‘new’, as ‘modern, German–Turkish’ young women who wanted to be socially and professionally successful and who were determined to fight against restrictions put on them by both the migrant community and German society.
160
3.
Inken Keim
Social style and social identity: A dynamic relationship
Our concept of social style is influenced by cultural (Clarke 1979; Willis 1981), ethnographic (Heath 1983), and sociological (Strauss 1984) concepts in which style is related to a group’s culture and its social identity.10 In this tradition, cultural style is the product of the adjustment of human communities to their ecological, social, and economic conditions. Striving for social integration as well as for social differentiation is a part of these conditions. Cultural or social styles correspond to schematic knowledge of social behavior, and their relevant traits reflect distinctive features of the respective social and cultural paradigm. From this perspective, a sociocultural style is defined as the specific solution for existential needs and aspirations. The specifics of a socio-cultural style become obvious through a comparison across different social worlds. In the following section, I want to focus on some aspects of style formation that are relevant to the ‘Powergirls’’ stylistic development. (a) Style is a complex and holistic means of expression. It is signaled by co-occurring features on the prosodic, lexical, syntactic, and lexicosemantic level as well as by the realization of specific activity types or specific genres and conversational structures. Elements from all these levels are combined along the same line, in a homologous way, and form a unique ‘gestalt’. In this ‘gestalt’ formation, further dimensions of expression are included such as outward appearance (clothing, make-up, piercings), body movement, preference for specific music or sport trends, etc.11 (b) In sociological and ethnographic research, further aspects of style formation are discussed. Style is seen as an ‘aesthetic performance’ (Soeffner 1986), a unification of features in order to give a holistic self-presentation, high-lighting those features which contrast to other socio-stylistic paradigms. The issue of contrast is central in Irvine’s concept of style (2001) as “part of a system of distinction in which a style contrasts with other possible styles and the social meaning signified by the style contrasts to other social meanings” (22). From this perspective, style is a relational concept: it exists only for participants of a group or milieu who interpret it in relation to another group or milieu (see Hinnenkamp and Selting 1989; Auer 1989).
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
161
(c) Social styles differ from one another. They are ascribed to social groups or milieus and have social meaning. Solidarity, affiliation, or identification with a social group or milieu is symbolized by using its style. In relation to other social groups or milieus, style functions as a means for differentiation and separation, as described especially in research on youth languages or youth cultures: cf. concepts such as ‘Kontrasprache’ (Bausinger 1972), ‘anti-language’ (Halliday 1976), ‘subculture’ (Hebdige 1979; Widdicombe and Wooffitt 1995), or ‘counter-culture’ (Clarke et al. 1979; Willis 1982). Aspects of differentiation are also central in studies on ethnicity (see Barth 1969; Schwitalla and Streek 1989; Czyzewski et al. 1995) and on social categorization (see Sacks 1979; Hausendorf 2002). (d) Style is interactionally produced. Speakers as well as recipients participate in the formation of a style, its maintenance and its change. Styles are not determined: they are performed as socially and interactively meaningful productions and can be adjusted to situational and interactional requirements. By abrupt style switchings or gradual style shiftings, locally different contexts or footings (Goffman 1974) can be accomplished. These aspects are essential for the description of the ‘Powergirls’’ style whose formation can be related to two processes of differentiation: the girls’ emancipation from the traditional Turkish female role and their opposition to the German school world. In the course of these differentiation processes, the ‘Powergirls’ created a style that contrasted on all stylistic dimensions with the ‘traditional young Turkish woman’ as well as with the teachers’ expectations at the Gymnasium.12 Both contrasts made the girls fall back on features taken from the communicative behavior of Turkish male groups of the district, characterized by aggressiveness and coarse language. The teachers at the Gymnasium rejected the ‘Powergirl’ style rigorously because it contrasted sharply with the schools’ ideology of cultivated behavior. Two girls were even expelled from school because of their rudeness. These experiences and the insight that a higher school qualification was the only way to become professionally successful and financially independent of their families13 effected a gradual change of social orientations and a gradual transformation of style. Stylistic elements, which so far had been evaluated by the peer-group as ‘not belonging to us’, were tried out,
162
Inken Keim
and gradually accepted. Along with this constant reconstruction of the stylistic repertoire, the following questions arise: – How much stylistic continuity is possible since processes of repertoire reconstruction are not necessarily harmonious, and conflicting stylistic means may collide; – are there phases in the stylistic development where different styles coexist; – or is a unique style constructed with various stylistic facets? These questions will be discussed in the course of the following outline of the ‘Powergirls’’ development from a ‘ghetto’ clique to young university students. In the following sections, I want to focus on two sets of stylistic features: a) the use of different varieties, Turkish and German, and b) the choice of specific communicative practices such as rough and coarse provocations and insults as the stylistic means for the symbolization of being a ‘Powergirl’. The first set of features (4) will be outlined very roughly,14 but the second will be presented in more detail (5).
4.
The use of different varieties
I start with a rough outline of the group’s linguistic development regarding the use and evaluation of the three varieties: ‘Mannheim Turkish’, German–Turkish mixing, and monolingual German. When I first met the ‘Powergirls’, they were still closely linked to the social life of the migrant community; some had just finished the Hauptschule and attended a Realschule or Fachschule outside the district. For those girls, ‘Mannheim Turkish’ and especially German–Turkish mixing were the essential means of ingroup communication. Monolingual German was not important for them, and some girls had no routine of using it over longer interactional stretches. They told me mixing was the most comfortable code and, as I observed, the most important one in in-group communication. ‘Mannheim Turkish’ is the variety of Turkish spoken by second and third generation migrants, especially with their elders. The name is derived from a comparison between the Turkish spoken in the home villages of the parents and the varieties spoken in Mannheim. ‘Mannheim Turkish’ has some of the typical characteristics of the Turkish varieties in Germany such as deletion of the question-particle, use of personal pronouns in unfocused
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
163
positions, avoidance of embedded gerund-constructions, etc.15 Some of these characteristics are caused by influences from German; others point to a loosening of grammatical norms or could be seen as the result of a dialect levelling. But most features of ‘Mannheim Turkish’ correspond to the Turkish dialects of the regions the families come from.16 In the case of the ‘Powergirls’, mixing was preferred in in-group communication, especially in everyday interactions such as narrations and arguments. In mixing, the girls use their bilingual competence for discursive and socio-symbolic functions.17 Until now, we could not find another migrant youth group that had developed such highly elaborate mixing practices; therefore, we assume that mixing as well as its discursive functions are part of the ‘Powergirls’ peer-group style. Those ‘Powergirls’ who had to leave the migrant district early in the course of their educational career had, when I met them, already acquired a high competence in monolingual German. But in in-group communication, mixing was their preferred code of interaction. The mixing of these girls differed slightly from that of the others in the higher proportion of German structures and elements. In some interactions (for example, discussions about their school affairs), German was their dominant language. This shows clearly that in the course of their educational career outside the migrant district, the girls’ linguistic competences and preferences had changed: in specific constellations together with specific topics, the relevance of mixing had decreased, and the relevance of German had increased. Two years later, when all girls attended schools outside the migrant district, they all had acquired a high competence in monolingual German. For the oldest girls, who had just started to attend a university, German had become the central means of expression in all professional domains. But in in-group communication, all girls still preferred mixing. At this time, it had become a means for symbolizing their affiliation with the category of the ‘German–Turk’ and their dissociation from the Turkish-speaking world as well as from the German-speaking majority. When I asked them about their ideal life-partner, they spontaneously answered that they would only marry a German–Turk, a man who could mix the languages. Thus, the formation of German–Turkish mixing as well as the use of monolingual German is closely related to the speakers’ processing of social experiences and to their construction of a genuine socio-cultural identity.
164
5.
Inken Keim
Coarse language – a stylistic marker of the ‘Powergirls’
When I first met the ‘Powergirls’ (they were 15–19 years old), coarse language was a constitutive characteristic of their communicative style. Its use is closely related to their emancipation from the category of the ‘young traditional woman’. The defining features of this category can be characterized as the following: the young woman restricts her life to the house and the family, submits to the norms and values of the family, behaves in an unassuming way, and follows the orders of her elders. Chastity and modesty are highly evaluated virtues that are symbolized by clothing as well as behavior. A life outside the house and contact with boys is strictly forbidden. For a girl or young woman, it is obligatory to wait on other family members, to stay in the background in the presence of her elders, and to keep quiet in the presence of older men. To address older people in an outspoken manner or to contradict them would be offensive, at least in public. Many Turkish migrant families try to educate their girls according to this model in order to shield them from modern, western influences, as did the ‘Powergirls’’ parents. But the girls revolted against this model and strictly rejected it. One of the girls describes its features as follows: (1)18 01 AR:
die sind so furschbar unterwürfig * bedienen die älteren * ‘they are so terribly obsequious they wait on the older ones
02 AR
servieren tee↓ * und gehn wieder still in die ecke↓ serve them tea and then they go quietly into their corner
02 AR:
des find=isch einfach schre”cklisch↓ I think that is really terrible’
And another girl describes her experiences with a neighbor, whom she sees as a candidate for the category: (2) 01 DI:
weißt du jedesmal wenn ich bei denen warĹ ** hat sie immer ‘you know, every time I was in their house she acted always
02 DI:
schön brav äh die dienerin gespieltĹ * hat immer tee gebracht like a servant, very obediently she brought tea
03 DI:
und gebäck gebracht und und die leute bedientĹ and cookies all the time and she waited on people
04 DI:
saß immer brav zu hauseĹ * ähm hat immer des getan always stayed at home, like a good girl, and she always
und ähm m/ and she
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time 05 DI:
165
was die eltern gesagt haben did what her parents told her’
The ‘Powergirls’ developed an ‘anti-traditional’ self-conception: they disobeyed their parents’ orders, preferred stylish clothes, make-up and piercings, went out with boys, danced in discos, and experimented with drugs. They enjoyed undisciplined, rude, and coarse ways of speaking and behaved very generally in a wild and aggressive way. From the perspective of their Turkish elders, such behavior was unusual for young women but rather typical for young men living in ‘street gangs’. Undisciplined behavior was expressed, for example, by ignoring turn-taking rules, interrupting each other, and shouting each other down (see below, examples 8 and 9). For the expression of coarseness, the girls drew on rough ways of speaking that they had observed in Turkish male groups who practiced verbal duellings and ritual insults. 5.1. Coarseness in verbal duellings In the district under study, games such as tavla and billards are part of everyday life for Turkish men. Such games are played in Turkish coffee houses, exclusively visited by men. In these games, playful insults with drastic expressions, swear formulas, coarse sexual formulas, and verbal duellings are constitutive elements.19 The aim of these verbal activities is to distract the adversary with advice or insults, to make him feel insecure, and to provoke him. The provocative turn follows the action of the game. Traditional Turkish women do not play such games, at least not in public. But for the ‘Powergirls’, tavla and billards were favorite games and part of their leisure-time activities. In order to demonstrate the kind of coarseness the girls enjoyed along with these games, I will give a short example taken from a weekend excursion I had the chance to document at the beginning of my observation. Hatice and Teslime, 16 and 17 years old, are playing billards. Before the following transcript starts, Hatice has commented on her successful moves with statements in German such as das war gut (‘that was good’) or ich hab deinen ball getroffen (‘I hit your ball’). Teslime reacts neither to Hatice’s moves nor to her remarks. Her silence causes Hatice to provoke her: she poses provocatively in front of the billiard table, laughs as if she is confident of victory, switches into Turkish, and starts speaking in a sweet, seductive voice (Turkish segments are in bold type):
166
Inken Keim
(3) 01 HA: seksi oyun yapalım↓ *1,5* PLAYS, HITS ‘let’s play in a sexy way’ K SWEET VOICE 02 TE: →>oruspua÷zını * sikyim< ‘I fuck your mouth’ K: SHARP VOICE 04 TE: PLAYS, DOES NOT HIT →allah belamı vermesi:n↓← ‘Allah shall not curse me’ 05 HA: LAUGHS niye be↑ * ha: * ben an/ anladım niye versin↓ ‘why oh, I have got it why He should curse you’ 06 HA: PLAYS, DOES NOT HIT →>siktira÷zını * sikyim< (‘I fuck your mouth’, 03). With this, she ratifies the playful competition in Turkish and continues it by topping the previous
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
167
move.22 Teslime plays the next ball. She also does not hit and uses a religious formula as a kind of self-reproach (allah belamı vermesi:n ‘Allah shall not curse me’, 04). 23 Since the formula does not belong to the standard repertoire of formulaic abuses, Hatice does not understand its meaning at first and asks niye be (‘why’, 05). But as she begins to understand, she confirms Teslime’s bad play by using the positive version of the formula: ha: * ben an/ anladım niye versin (‘oh, I have got it why He should curse you’, 05). She then continues with the game. She plays the ball, and because she does not hit again, she comments on her failure with the abuse formula siktir (06), a shortened version of siktittir git (‘let yourself be fucked’). With this formula, she resumes the verbal duelling that was interrupted by the religious formula. When Teslime, too, does not hit the target, she uses a further version of the abusive formula that Hatice had used in line 03, which has a slightly more drastic quality: a÷zina sikyim (‘I fuck into your mouth’, 07).24 When Teslime hits the target with the next move (08), Hatice ends the playful competition: →des is do=normalerweise ein faul gell↑ dass du mein stein zuerschd triffschd← (‘that normally is a foul, you know, that you hit my ball first’, 09/10). She reproaches Teslime for foul play and changes the frame of interaction from ritual insults to an earnest reproach. The end of the playful interaction and the beginning of the new interaction modality are signalled on various linguistic levels: faster tempo, louder and angry voice, code-switching into German, and the meta-linguistic comment on the adversary’s move. The speaker opens another frame and speaks like a young German from Mannheim – using some colloquial elements25 – who reproaches her friend for unfairness in a game. 5.2. Coarse language in calls to order Calls to order using coarse language were used for playful criticism as well as for the expression of irritation and anger. It includes formulas such as halts maul langer (‘shut up, man’), verpiss dich (‘piss off’), and siktir lan26 (‘fuck you, man’), or terms of abuse such as orospu (‘whore’) and orospu çocu÷u (‘child of a whore’). But coarse language in calls to order is not treated as an insult: recipients accept or ignore them, and they often top them as in the following example:
168
Inken Keim
(4) 01 I: 02 TU:
>→hosch gekifft↑←< ‘did you smoke pot’
siktir lan ‘fuck you, man’ +halts:=maul=langer ‘shut up, man’
Since the consumption of hashish is prohibited in the youth center, Hilal’s (HI) question >→hosch gekifft↑←< (‘did you smoke pot’, 01), addressed to the girl sitting next to her, Tuna (TU), is delicate and risky even though it is spoken fast and in a low voice. The recipient treats the question as an infringement and rejects it with a coarse call to order: +halts:=maul=langer (‘shut up, man’, 02). She either thinks that the speaker has no right to ask her such a question or that the situation is too dangerous for the topic. But Hilal obviously interprets the situation differently and counters the call to order with a more drastic formula: siktir lan (‘fuck you, man’, 01).27 She tops Tuna’s call to order and emphasizes the opposition by code-switching into Turkish. Tuna does not react, and the dispute is settled. By performing the dispute with formulaic means, the critique and its rejection obtain a ritual character: both parties demonstrate a readiness to fight, and, at the same time, they settle the critical situation. 5.3. Situational restrictions for the use of coarse language Even though drastic expressions are characteristic, the ‘Powergirls’ know that outsiders, especially teachers, evaluate them extremely negatively, and there are situations where this knowledge becomes manifest. In the following example, two girls are involved in an argument in which a series of abuses occur such as öküz (‘ox’), sakat (‘nitwit’), and amcık beyin (‘brain of a cunt’). The girls do not realize that a teacher (who understands Turkish) overhears their verbal fight. When one of the girls becomes aware of the teacher, she reacts in the following way: (5) 01 TE:
>rezil oldukĻ< * si"ktir * oruspu çocu÷uĻ * ‘we disgraced ourselves completely, fuck you child of a whore, beat it’
The speaker finishes the argument with the self-reflexive comment >rezil oldukĻ< (‘we disgraced ourselves completely’) and expresses her shame for using vulgar expressions in the presence of an outsider and a person on whose evaluation she depends. With a final angrily spoken insult si“ktir
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
169
oruspu çocu÷uĻ* (‘fuck you, child of a whore, beat it’), she turns away and leaves the room. The comment rezil olduk reveals a rule for vulgar expressions: they are specific to in-group communication, and the girls try to avoid them in the presence of outsiders on whose good opinion they depend because they know that they disapprove of them.28 But the speaker’s use of vulgar expressions even in the course of lamenting her coarse language reveals how stabilized and routinized these ways of speaking are in in-group communication. Coarse language together with the male address forms mann, lan, and langer are closely related to the group’s self-conception, contrasting maximally with the social category of the ‘traditional young Turkish woman’. As a consequence of the intensive revolt against the category of the ‘traditional young Turkish woman’, the girls’ rough and rude behavior was gradually used in other contexts, too. As long as they attended the district schools, they got on quite well. But in schools outside the district, especially the Gymnasium, the teachers did not tolerate their behavior. And here, they got into serious trouble. They interpreted the teachers’ criticism and rejection of their behavior in an ethnic frame: they felt negatively evaluated because they were Turks and because of the German teachers’ prejudices against Turks, and they reacted accordingly with an up-grading of their ethnicity. They rejected the teachers as ausländerfeindliche Deutsche (‘Germans hostile to foreigners’) and emphasized their Turkishness by enforcing their aggressive and coarse behavior. They enjoyed shocking everyone with the consequence of marginalizing themselves and being marginalized by their German schoolmates and teachers. Retrospectively, they characterize their behavior in this phase of their lives as unverschämt, frech und einfach asozial (‘insolent, rude, and truly anti-social’). For some of the girls, this attitude grew into a hostile relationship towards the teachers with very serious consequences. One of the girls who was no longer accepted as a student and had to leave school explains this in the following way: (6) 01 HI:
des haben die lehrer zu mir gesagt ‘the (German)teachers told me
02 HI:
dass isch ähm- * dass halt der tonĹ die musik machtĻ that I ehm * that I need to change my tune
03 HI:
dass isch misch halt falsch ausdrücken würdeĻ and that I express myself in a wrong way
170
Inken Keim
04 HI:
dass die misch missverstehnĻ (...) that they misunderstand me
05 HI:
isch weiß was die meinen halt dass isch- ** and I know what they mean that I
06 HI:
dass isch halt zu grob binĻ that I am too rude’
The speaker portrays the teacher-pupil conflict as based on differences of social style, and her failure in school is presented as a consequence of her stylistic shortcomings. On the basis of such experiences, the girls came to understand that the teachers, whom they treated as their enemies, were ‘gate-keepers’ to their future professional careers. Consequently, they became more and more sensitive to situational demands on their verbal and nonverbal behavior: they learned to distinguish between situations where it was possible to use rude ways of speaking from situations where they were better avoided, and they learned to come to terms with the social conventions and values of the social worlds in which they wanted to succeed. They learned to control their behavior and to adjust it to new contextual conditions. Along with a change in their self-conception from the ‘Turkish Powergirl’ to a socially and professionally successful ‘German–Turkish young woman’, they gradually oriented towards communicative practices associated with this new category. In this process, more formal and elaborate forms of communication were acquired, including ‘polite talk’ with conventional politeness formulas.
6.
Politeness in out-group communication
The youth centre that the ‘Powergirls’ began to visit regularly enforced this process. Here, they learned new communicative styles, especially in interaction with their favorite social worker, Naran, a young, modern German– Turkish academic. Since she grew up and finished her university career in Germany, she was well acquainted with life in the Turkish migrant community as well as with the social and educational demands in German institutions. The young women respected Naran as their abla (‘older sister’), and they followed her advice. Even though she tolerated the girls’ rude ways of speaking, she would never tolerate rudeness addressed to herself. If the girls disregarded rules of politeness towards her, they had to apologize using the relevant formulas, or they had to offer other compensatory actions.
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
171
In some of these interactions, differences between the girls’ and the social worker’s communicative styles come into focus. These show that even though the girls knew how to be ‘polite’ they still dissociated themselves from that kind of behavior, signalling that it belonged to others and not (yet) to their peer group. This can be demonstrated by the following example. On a weekend trip (in the second year of my observation), when the social worker tried to explain some organizational details, she could not get the girls’ attention and did not succeed in getting the floor. The girls interrupted her, spoke all at once, or tried to shout each other down. When, at last, Naran got the floor, she reproached the girls for their undisciplined behavior, returned to the official agenda, and continued with the organisational information. With this, the transcript starts: (7) 01 NA:
für den fernsehraum ←krieg ich schlü“ssel↓→ * →das heisst ‘I get the keys for the television room that means
02 NA:
we=ma← a“bends noch was unternehmn↑ * wir ham selbst die if we do something in the evening we have the opportunity
03 NA:
möglichkeit a“bzuschließn↓ to shut the room by ourselves’
>isch möscht was fragen< ‘I would like to ask something’
04 TE:
05 TE:
** >bitte< ‘yes,please’
darf isch bitte am donnerstag * ‘may I watch TV please on Tuesday
entweder elf uhr morgens either at 11 o’clock in
06
TE: oder zehn uhr abends fernsehn kuckn↓→ the morning or at 10 o’clock in the evening’
07
HL:
08
TE: s=is sehr wichtig ‘it’s very important’
09
NA:
GIGGLES SOFTLY
>sehr wichtich↑ wir kuckn mal< ‘very important we’ll see’
Right after the information about the TV-room, the first point of Naran’s announcement, Teslime (TE), one of girls who had just been rebuked for rude behavior, intervenes and asks for the floor >sch möscht was fragen< (‘I would like to ask something’, 04) even though it is clear to all participants that Naran has not yet finished. Reluctantly (longer pause), Naran gives Teslime the floor, and she presents her request: ←darf isch bitte am
172
Inken Keim
donnerstag * entweder elf uhr morgens oder zehn uhr abends fernseh kuckn↓→ * (‘may I watch TV please on Tuesday either at 11 o’clock in the morning or at 10 o’clock in the evening’, 05/06). Explicitly asking for the floor and speaking in a soft way marks a clear contrast to Teslime’s former behavior. The request is formulated in a rather elaborate and ‘polite’ way of speaking using conventional politeness markers such as the modal verb dürfen and the bitte-formula and giving an account for the request s=is sehr wichtig (‘it is very important’, 08). With this, Teslime follows Naran’s call for a change in behavior and demonstrates that she is perfectly able to speak in a polite way. Naran does not reject Teslime’s request for the floor, but gives her the floor, and she even does not rule out that the request may be granted >sehr wichtichĹ wir kuckn mal< (‘very importantĹ we’ll see’, 09). This reaction indicates that she treats Teslime’s politely formulated request not as an intervention but as a kind of repair for her previous (bad) behavior; and she shows her that polite forms are effective in interaction. The reaction of another girl (HI) to Teslime’s polite request is interesting as well. Her soft giggling (07) signals that Teslime’s change in behavior is unexpected and has comical effects for her. The style Teslime uses with Naran does not correspond to the group’s normal way of speaking, and by emphasizing the stylistic difference, a certain social distance towards Naran is established. This interaction sequence makes evident that two different communication styles coexist in the stylistic repertoire of the girls at this developmental stage and that they differentiate very clearly between different social worlds by using different stylistic means together with markers of disaffiliation.
7.
Elaborate and polite ways of speaking in in-group communication
In the course of the older group members’ successful participation in the world outside the migrant community, new forms of communication were gradually incorporated into in-group communication. This process was reinforced when the group experienced for the first time that verbal means that had been effective so far no longer met with new requirements. A crucial situation occurred when, one and a half years after the conversational exchange discussed in the previous section (6), the girls had to manage a complex and demanding project without the help of their social worker. The girls were then 17 to 21 years old. They planned to produce a video
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
173
film about themselves in order to take part in a supra-regional film contest. The film was of great importance to them, offering an opportunity to present themselves to the public and depicting a picture of modern migrant women, which was quite different from the stereotypical view of Turkish women dominant in German society.29 The group started with a meeting of all members in order to draw up the plot of the film and to plan its production. Right from the beginning, the girls experienced their inability to cope with a situation in which they had to organize a creative process over a longer period of time and to integrate members’ often contrasting views and proposals without excluding anyone. The meeting started chaotically. Trying to establish a working order, the group elected a moderator, who had to organize the discussions and decisions on the film project. This type of situation, where an elected moderator has to cope with a new role and new tasks, provided the context for introducing new stylistic means. In the course of this meeting, new and more elaborate ways of speaking were gradually brought in and tried out; some were rejected, and others were accepted and incorporated into the group’s stylistic repertoire. The elected moderator was Didem, one of the older girls, who had just begun to attend a university. The first opportunity for introducing new stylistic means arose when communication threatened to break down. This happened when the girls, talking about their film ideas, interrupted each other and all tried to speak at once until a wild confusion arose with reproaches, counter reproaches, and rude insults. The moderator tried to calm everybody down by calls to order like ruhe (‘be quiet’) and jetzt hört ma zu mensch * seid doch ruhig (‘now you listen and be quiet’), but nobody obeyed her orders. The noise became louder and louder until one of the young women complained: (8) 01 SU:
terribly, my ears I swear’ <ja“Ļ halt=s maulĻ> ‘ou yeah, shut up’
03 TU:
04 DI:
und die schrei"en and they are yelling
KNOCKS ON THE TABLE <wir haben hier gerade eine besprechung ‘we are having a meeting
174
Inken Keim
05 DI:
falls ihr das nischt mitbekommen ha"btĻ und wer nischt in case you did not notice and I ask those
06 DI:
dazu gehört> bitt isch raus zu gehnĻ du wolltest who are not participating to leave the room you wanted to genauĻ ‘exactly’
07 SU:
08 DI: 09 HA:
was sagenĹ jetz mach ma weiterĻ say something please continue’ yani i/ ‘well
isch zum beispiel I for instance’
Suna’s (SU) complaint about the noise is rejected by a rude call to order from Tuna (TU): <ja” halt=s maul>Ļ (‘oh yeah, shut up’, 03). The rude rejection of a discussant’s justified complaints could be the beginning of the meeting’s break down. The way the moderator intervenes suggests such an interpretation: she knocks on the table, gives an explicit definition of the situation as a besprechung (‘meeting’, 04), and asks participants who do not accept it to leave the room. In contrast to the previous turns of Suna and Tuna,30 the moderator switches into an elaborate way of speaking: <wir haben hier gerade eine besprechung (…) wer nischt dazu gehört >bitt isch raus zu gehen (‘we are having a meeting here (…) I ask those who are not participating to leave the room’, 04/06). This constitutes a marked contrast to the kind of language used in the previous turns. With this move, the moderator succeeds in putting an end to the interaction chaos. She obtains a positive response from one of the participants: genauĻ (‘exactly’, 07), and as soon as the others are silent, she gives the floor to the girl who was interrupted before: du wolltest was sagenĹ jetzt mach ma weiterĻ (‘you wanted to say something, please continue’, 06/08), and the girl continues with her statement (09). The conversational discipline only lasts for a short time; and it is not long before the participants talk all at once again and try to shout each other down. Thereupon, the moderator proposes a procedure that she became familiar with at the university and that she announces as her last attempt to establish order: she proposes that each speaker who has the floor takes a rod, symbolizing that it is her turn. As long as she holds the rod, no one should interrupt her. Only the moderator is allowed to take the rod from a speaker and to give it to another, who then gets the floor. This proposal is rejected, first by Tuna’s negative evaluation in Turkish ay ne gıcık (‘how stupid’) and then by Funda’s comment in German des is bei uns in der clique ganz schlecht (‘that is completely unsuited for our clique’). These
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
175
comments highlight the social meaning of the proposed procedure: it is seen as belonging to the style of another social world to which the two younger girls do not (yet) belong and which is not (yet) accepted as part of their repertoire. By rejecting elements of a social world not (yet) their own, the two girls reveal a high stylistic sensitivity. But the moderator insists on the proposed procedure and explains that for her, it is the only effective way to accomplish some kind of interaction order: (9) 01 DI:
des is=n einziges cha"os hierĻ un=des is die einzige ‘here, we have only chaos and my proposal is the
02 DI:
möglischkeit o/ eusch mal irgendwie zum schweigen only way to silence you somehow
03 DI:
zu bringenĹ
04 HA:
und du willst jetzt grad was and you want to say something right ahĻ
05 DI:
sagenĹ GIVES THE ROD TO HA und alle anderen now and all the others
06 DI:
halten jetzt die klappeĻ keep their mouths shut now’
By explaining the reason for her insistence in introducing the new procedure, the moderator uses no peer group expressions but more elaborate formulations: des is=n einziges cha"os hierĻ un=des is die einzige möglischkeit o/ eusch mal irgendwie zum schweigen zu bringenĹ (‘here, we have only chaos, and my proposal is the only way to silence you somehow’, 01/03). She continues to speak in this way as she performs the new procedure, handing over the rod to the girl who wanted to speak and giving her the floor: du willst jetzt grad was sagen (‘you want to say something right now’, 03/05). After that, she addresses the other participants with a further call to order, thereby switching into more colloquial expressions: und alle anderen halten jetzt die klappe (‘and all the others keep their mouths shut now’, 05/06). With this move, Didem succeeds with the new procedure and re-establishes the interaction order. In her turn, Didem combines different stylistic means: along with the presentation and explanation of the new procedure, she uses more elaborate ways of speaking; but when she addresses the other girls, calling them to order (06), she switches into a typical peer group formula used when somebody is annoyed, without being as coarse as the other girls’ utterances.31
176
Inken Keim
Therefore, it can be assumed that Didem distinguishes between stylistic means of the peer group that agree with her role as moderator from others that disagree with it. By choosing only ‘moderate’ expressions and combining them with elaborate ones, she enacts her conception of a ‘moderator’ through her ways of speaking, and the other girls accept her performance. In the course of the meeting, the moderator introduces even conventional politeness forms that respect speakers’ negative face-wants; and the other girls accept them, too. As it happens, Didem herself interrupts another speaker, apologizes explicitly and re-establishes the speaker’s right in the following way: (10) 01 DI: 02 SU:
03 DI:
‘or or/ one moment, you could something o"hĻ K ANGRILY
entschuldigungĻ * du bist dran excuse me it is your turn’
In (01), Didem interrupts Suna’s (SU) previous turn (outside the transcript) and starts with a new suggestion for a film scene. Suna stops Didem with her angry cry o”hĻ (02), whereupon Didem explicitly apologizes for interrupting: entschuldigungĻ (03). Then she gives the floor to Suna with the formula: du bist dran (‘it is your turn’, 03), thereby re-establishing her speaking right. Taking into account another speaker’s right and apologizing explicitly for an interruption are new practices in in-group communication. Up to now, such procedures had only occurred in interactions with outsiders, for example, with Naran, other social workers, or teachers. Since the procedure was effective in the interaction between the moderator and Suna, another group member also tries it out. At the end of the meeting, one of the girls who had been extremely undisciplined, Facilet (FA), starts speaking in a polite way: (11) 01 DI:
jetzt ähm- ** folgendesĻ wenn wir mit diesem film anfangen ‘and now, what I want to say, when we start with the film
02 DI:
jaĹ ** äh also ihr solltet zuverlässig seinĻ zu jedem yes, you really should be reliable and you should
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time 03 DI: 04 FA:
177
|treffen auch kommenĻ | jaĻ come to every meeting yes’ das nächste mal (…) |darf isch noch was sagenĹ| bitteĹ ‘may I say something please the next time (...)’
Before the moderator finishes her last appeal (01/03), Facilet asks for the floor: darf isch noch was sagenĹ bitteĹ (‘may I say something, please’, 04), using conventional politeness forms such as the modal verb dürfen and the bitte-formula. The utterance is spoken in an earnest and eager way without any signs of ironic or playful modalization. The addressee grants the request, and the speaker presents her proposal. In this short interaction, a ‘polite’ way of speaking is realized in an unmarked and natural way without signs of dissociation. Even though it is practiced in interaction with the moderator, it was initiated by a discussant for the first time. With this, ‘polite’ speaking has entered in-group communication, and it is accepted and practiced by group members. But the incorporation of new stylistic means into a group’s stylistic repertoire is a dynamic process, and it does not advance in a linear way. Every new element is tried out and evaluated if it agrees with the group’s ideas about adequate behavior. This can be demonstrated by another example taken from the same meeting. Here, the recipients do not accept the new stylistic elements introduced by the moderator. As Didem closes the group meeting with a formula, she provokes distancing reactions: (12) 01 DI: 02 TU:
03 DI: 04 FA: K&
|danke | für eure- gute ‘thank you for coming thank you for your good |bitteĻ| ‘you’re welcome’
kooperationĻ cooperation’
auf wiedersehenĻ ‘good bye’ *1,5* END OF THE MEETING
(‘thank you for coming’, 01) is a formula for closing a formal meeting to which the chairperson has invited the participants. One of the girls, Tuna (TU), reacts with the bitteĻ- formula (‘you’re welcome’, 02) although no response is expected in the closing ritual. With this reaction, Tuna falls back on another formulaic ritual – thanks and acceptance – and produces the second part of that ritual (acceptance). Her reaction has a non-serious, playful quality indicating that she is making fun
178
Inken Keim
of the closing ritual produced by Didem. The reason behind Tuna’s reaction could be, on the one hand, that the formula does not correspond to Didem’s role in the situation: only the person who initiated the meeting could use it. On the other hand, the formula might be too formal and inconsistent with other stylistic features. Didem’s next closing formula danke für eure gute kooperation (‘thank you for your good cooperation’, 01/03) is also not accepted, made evident by Facilet’s (FA) surprised and amused exclamation (04). It is interesting that Facilet, who took over the moderator’s polite way of speaking a short time before (see example 12), now distances herself from her. These reactions make it evident that the girls, who accepted Didem’s performance as the moderator up till now and who even incorporated some of her ways of speaking into their own, reject stylistic means that do not agree with their idea of adequate behaviour in the present situation. They work together with Didem to construct the stylistic means of a group moderator, but they object to the (too formal) situational definition that Didem produces in her closing rituals.
8.
Conclusion
The formation of the ‘Powergirls’’ style was related to two social categories relevant to the girls’ self-definition. Their dissociation from the ‘traditional Turkish woman’ was expressed through the selection of maximally contrasting features, taken from ‘male worlds’ of the migrant ‘ghetto’, features which are ‘rough, coarse and aggressive’. These features were used in all situations in which the girls wanted to express their opposition to the traditional female category. In opposition to German teachers of the higher educational institutions, the girls highlighted their ‘Turkish-ness’ by the extensive use of elaborated Turkish–German mixings as well as behavior contradicting all teacher expectations. The ‘Turkish Powergirls’ realized that their rebellious and anti-social behavior excluded them from social and educational success. This led to the construction of a new self-conception and to integration into educational and professional German worlds, expressed on the stylistic level by the incorporation of elaborate and polite ways of speaking. The formation of a style is a dynamic process and in constant progress. There are phases in which various styles with conflicting means co-exist in
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
179
a group’s repertoire and are separately used in order to adjust to or to create different contexts. Additionally, there are phases where the gradual incorporation of new means into peer group communication can be observed. When group members obtain more and more experience in the use of stylistic means characteristic of the outside world, they differentiate their stylistic repertoire according to situational and functional tasks, combining new forms with features of their former in-group style. As the young women’s further development shows (some of them became teachers or social workers, others are still university students), the acquisition of new stylistic means does not imply giving up in-group forms. All former group members still have a high competence in coarse language and ritual insults. But they have learned to switch in a virtuoso manner from one style to another according to different situational and contextual purposes: vulgar expressions in response to a ‘ghetto kid’ may be followed by conventional politeness forms in order to create a more elaborate or formal situation. The formation of a social style follows biographical processes and adjusts to new demands emerging from new social orientations and aspirations.
9.
Transcript conventions
The transcription uses the signs of the German alphabet in analogy to the rules of pronunciation in German for the representation of the phonological and phonetic features of the spoken language, including dialectal speech. In addition, we use the following notation: *, ** ↑, ↓, – ←, → ” : = ich |hab das| gesehn |niemals | # #
short pause, longer pause rising, falling, and middle intonation slower, faster tempo louder, softer voice strong accent strong lengthening slurring manner of articulation, linking different words overlap extension of comments
180
Inken Keim
LAUGHS Ü
nonverbal expressions of a speaker translation line
Notes 1. This study is part of a research project on “German-Turkish variation and the construction of social styles of communication in young migrant groups in Mannheim, Germany.” This project was funded by the DFG (2000–2004) and carried out by Sema Aslan, Ibrahim Cindark, Werner Kallmeyer, and Inken Keim. The aims of the study were the description of the group members’ linguistic and communicative repertoires, their bilingual practices, and the reconstruction of their in-group style of communication in relation to their social experiences. We selected three groups of young migrants and documented their in-group and out-group communication over 2–3 years. One group was studied by I. Cindark (2005 and i. prep.), the other by S. Aslan (2005) and the third group, the ‘Turkish Powergirls’, by myself. For a presentation of the research project, see Kallmeyer and Keim (1999); for a short overview of the three groups and their style of communication, see Keim (2003); for project papers and publications, see: http://www.ids-mannheim.de/prag/sprachvariation/public.htm#tp-3. 2. According to statistics, in October 2004, 67,000 migrants from 177 nations were living in Mannheim, most of them in inner city districts; see Mannheimer Morgen, 26.10.04, Nr. 249, p. 20. 3. There are Turkish stores, banks, doctors, hairdressers, advocates, service stations, cafes, restaurants, etc. In the past years, numerous Turkish companies were founded where young migrants can find a job. 4. For a detailed description, see Keim (2007). 5. For a more detailed description, see Keim (2005). 6. At the end of Hauptschule, students have to pass a final examination in order to obtain the Hauptschule-diploma. Without this diploma and without good average marks, they do not have a chance to take an apprenticeship. 7. According to the newest statistics, only 16% of the adolescents leaving the Hauptschule in Mannheim get a professional training; see Mannheimer Morgen, 03.12.2004, Nr. 281, p. 20. 8. Many of my young informants, not only the ‘Powergirls’, spoke about such experiences. 9. When I met the girls at the youth center, they were 15–19 years old. After having offered them some help with their homework, I had access to the group. As soon as some of them got better marks at school, I was accepted at their group meetings and was allowed to (audio and video) record their group activi-
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
10.
11. 12.
13. 14.
15. 16.
17.
18. 19.
181
ties as well as other situations (family meetings, week-end excursions). I observed the group for 3 years, visited their meetings once or twice a week, and did biographical interviews in German with all group members. Sema Aslan did ethnographic interviews with the girls’ parents in Turkish. I was present at all the documented group interactions. After the end of the observation, I kept in touch with the girls, was invited to their weddings, and am still informed about their social and professional lives. The conversational material was transcribed and translated by Necmiye Ceylan and Emran Sirim, German-Turkish bilinguals, who grew up in the migrant community in Mannheim. This concept was developed in the course of various research projects on different social groups in an urban context; see Kallmeyer (ed.) (1994), Keim (1995), and Schwitalla (1995). For a further development of the concept, see Kallmeyer and Keim (1996 and 2003). See the papers in Hinnenkamp and Selting (eds.) (1989), in Selting and Sandig (eds.) (1997), and in Keim and Schütte (eds.) (2002). Other Turkish girls as well as German girls characterized the ‘Powergirl’-style and the ‘Powergirls’ behavior as asocial, non-feminine, and not what a (Turkish) girl should do. This insight was reinforced in the course of many discussions with the social worker of the youth center. For a detailed description see Kallmeyer, Keim, Aslan and Cindark: Variationsprofile. Zur Analyse der Variationspraxis bei den Powergirls. Online. http://www.ids-mannheim.de/prag/sprachvariation/fgvaria/Variationsprofile.pdf, 08.07.2004, and Keim (2007). For detailed description of ‘Mannheim Turkish’, see Cindark and Aslan (2004) and Sirim (2004). Cindark and Aslan’s study focuses on some features of ‘Mannheim Turkish’ such as the use of personal pronouns, the question particle, and the pluralmarker and compares them with the Turkish spoken in Turkey. They show that only in 1–7 % of the cases does ‘Mannheim Turkish’ differ from the Turkish spoken in Turkey. The authors conclude that speakers of the second and third generation have a high structural competence in Turkish (morphology and syntax), but on the lexical level, there are gaps, and expressions are used that are unusual in Turkey. For a detailed analysis of these functions, see Keim (2007); see also http://www.ids-mannheim.de/prag/sprachvariation/fgvaria/Variationsprofile.pdf, 08.07.2004. See Appendix for transcription conventions. Verbal duellings among young Turkish men that are not related to games are described by Dundees, Leach, and Özkök (1972); Labov (1972) analyzes ritual insults used by young blacks in the US. In his ethnography of a Turkish street gang in Frankfurt, Tertilt (1996) also describes verbal duellings.
182
Inken Keim
20. For the concept of contextualization, see Gumperz (1982) and Auer (1986). 21. For the concept of frame, see Goffman (1974). 22. Topping is constitutive for verbal duellings or ritual insults used in male youth groups, see Labov (1972); Dundes, Leach, and Özkök (1972); Schwitalla (1994), also for rap and hip-hop. Topping also occurs in female groups, see Keim (1995), Chap. 5. 23. Turkish parents use the formula allah belani vermesin (‘God shall not curse you’) with their children, for example, when they have done something very wrong. Here, Teslime uses the formula as a self-reproach for not hitting the ball. 24. According to my German-Turkish informants, the second version a÷zına sikyim with a÷zın in the dative/ directional position, marked by –a, is slightly more drastic than the first version a÷zını sikyim, where a÷zın is in the accusative, marked by -ı. 25. Regional colloquial elements are the palatalization of /s/ in the consonant cluster /st/ in zuerschd and triffschd as well as the shortening and raising of the vowel in des instead of das and the tag gell . 26. The German address form langer (‘man, old boy’) and the Turkish address form lan (‘man’), taken from male repertoires, are normal in the ‘Powergirls’ in-group style. 27. The girls know that TU occasionally smokes pot; therefore, HI’s question does not refer to a secret. HI rejecting TU’s critique implies that from her perspective the question is no great infringement on TU’s privacy. 28. The Turkish informants to whom I presented the conversational material confirmed the ‘Powergirls’’ impression that coarse expressions are negatively evaluated by outsiders. From an outside perspective, it is unusual for a young Turkish woman to use such language. 29. In connection with the video project, intensive discussions about the girls’ selfconception took place. In these discussions, the new model of a ‘self-assertive German-Turkish young woman’ emerged, who strives for social and professional success; see Keim (2007). 30. Suna’s utterance has markers of ‘youth language’ such as the use of the adjective voll as a modal adverb in die schreien voll and the intensifying formula isch schwör. Tuna’s call to order halt=s maul is a very rude colloquial expression. For the description of ‘youth language’, see, for example, Androutsopoulos 1998. 31. Throughout the meeting, Didem does not use the kind of coarseness that the other girls use in the same situation, for example, halt=s maul (‘shut up’) or siktir (‘fuck you’).
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time
183
References Androutsopoulos, Jannis 1998 Deutsche Jugendsprache. Untersuchungen zu ihren Strukturen und Funktionen. Frankfurt: Peter Lang. Antaki, Charles and Sue Widdicombe 1998 Identity as an Achievement and as a Tool. In: Antaki, Charles and Sue Widdicombe (eds.), Identities in Talk. London/New Delhi: Sage, 1–14. Antaki, Charles and Sue Widdicombe (eds.) 1998 Identities in Talk. London/New Delhi: Sage. Aslan, Sema 2005 Aspekte des kommunikativen Stils einer Gruppe weltläufiger Migranten türkischer Herkunft: Die ‘Europatürken’. Deutsche Sprache 4(4), 327–356. Auer, Peter 1986 Kontextualisierung. Studium Linguistik 19, 22–47. 1989 Natürlichkeit und Stil. In: Hinnenkamp, Volker and Margret Selting (eds.), Stil und Stilisierung. Tübingen: Niemeyer, 27–60. Barth, Frederik 1969 Introduction. In: Barth, Frederik (ed.), Ethnic Groups and Boundaries. Bergen-Oslo/London: Univ. Forl., 9–38. Bausinger, Hermann 1972 Dialekte, Sprachbarrieren, Sondersprachen. Frankfurt: Fischer. Cindark, Ibrahim 2005 ‘Die Unmündigen’. Eine soziolinguistische Fallstudie der emanzipatorischen Migranten. Deutsche Sprache 4(4), 299–326. in prep. Die Unmündigen – der kommunikative soziale Stil der emanzipierten Migranten. Dissertationsprojekt, Universität Mannheim. Cindark, Ibrahim and Sema Aslan 2004 Deutschlandtürkisch? http://www.ids-mannheim.de/prag/soziostilistik/Deutschlandtuerkisch.pdf, 08.07.2004. Clarke, John et al. 1979 Jugendkultur als Widerstand. Milieus, Rituale, Provokationen. Frankfurt: Syndikat. Czyzewski, Marek, Martina Drescher, Elisabeth Gülich, and Heiko Hausendorf 1995 Nationale Selbst- und Fremdbilder im Gespräch. Kommunikative Prozesse nach der Wiedervereinigung Deutschlands und dem Systemwandel in Ostmitteleuropa. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.
184
Inken Keim
Dundees, Alan, Jerry Leach, and Bora Özkök 1972 The strategy of Turkish boys’ verbal dueling rhymes. In: Gumperz, John and Dell Hymes (eds.), Directions in Sociolinguistics. The Ethnography of Communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 130–160. Goffman, Erving 1974 Frame Analysis. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books. Gumperz, John 1982 Discourse Strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Halliday, M. A. K. 1976 Anti-languages. American Anthropologist 78, 570–584. Heath, Shirley 1983 Ways with Words. Language, Life, and Work in Communities and Classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hausendorf, Heiko 2000 Zugehörigkeit durch Sprache. Eine linguistische Studie am Beispiel der deutschen Wiedervereinigung (Reihe germanistische Linguistik 215). Tübingen: Niemeyer. Hebdige, Dick 1979 Subculture. The Meaning of Style. London/New York: Methuen. Henne, Helmut 1986 Jugend und ihre Sprache. Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter. Hinnenkamp, Volker and Margret Selting (eds.) 1989 Stil und Stilisierung. Tübingen: Niemeyer. Irvine, Judith 2001 ‘Style’ as distinctiveness: The culture and ideology of linguistic differentiation. In: Eckert, Penelope and John Rickford (eds.), Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 21–43. Kallmeyer, Werner (ed.) 1994 Exemplarische Analysen des Sprachverhaltens in Mannheim (Kommunikation in der Stadt. Teil 1). Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter. Kallmeyer, Werner and Inken Keim 1996 Divergent perspectives and social style in conflict talk. Folia Linguistica 30(3–4), Special issue, Interaktive Sozioliguistik, Helga Kotthoff (ed.), 271–298. 1999 Deutsch-Türkische Sprachvariation und die Herausbildung kommunikativer Stile in Migrantengruppen türkischer Herkunft. Manuscript, Mannheim.
Socio-cultural identity, communicative style, and their change over time 2003
185
Eigenschaften von sozialen Stilen der Kommunikation: Am Beispiel einer türkischen Migrantinnengruppe. In: Erfurt, Jürgen (ed.), ‘Multisprech’: Hybridität, Variation, Identität (OBST 65). Osnabrück: Universität, 35–56.
Keim, Inken 1995 Kommunikative Stilistik einer sozialen Welt ‘kleiner Leute’ in der Mannheimer Innenstadt (Kommunikation in der Stadt. Teil 3). Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter. 2002 Sozial-kulturelle Selbstdefinition und sozialer Stil: Junge Deutschtürkinnen im Gespräch. In: Keim, Inken and Wilfried Schütte (eds.), Soziale Welten und kommunikative Stile. Tübingen: Narr, 233–259. 2003 Social style of communication and bilingual speech practices: Case study of three migrant youth groups of Turkish origin in Mannheim/Germany. Turkic Languages 6(2), 284–298. 2005 Kommunikative Praktiken in türkischstämmigen Kinder- und Jugendgruppen in Mannheim. Deutsche Sprache 3(4), 198–226. 2007 Die ‘türkischen Powergirls’. Lebenswelt und kommunikativer Stil einer Migrantinnengruppe in Mannheim. Tübingen: Narr. Labov, William 1972 Rules for ritual insults. In: Sudnow, David (ed.), Studies in Social Interaction. New York/London: The Free Press, 120–169. Sacks, Harvey 1979 Hotrodder: A revolutionary category. In: Psathas, George (ed), Everyday Language. Studies in Ethnomethodology. New York: Irvington, 7–21. Sandig, Barbara and Margret Selting 1997 Einleitung. In: Selting, Margret and Barbara Sandig (eds.), Sprechund Gesprächsstile. Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter, 1–8. Schwitalla, Johannes 1994 Die Vergegenwärtigung einer Gegenwelt. Sprachliche Formen der sozialen Abgrenzung einer Jugendlichengruppe in Vogelstang. In: Kallmeyer, Werner (ed.), Exemplarische Analysen des Sprachverhaltens in Mannheim (Kommunikation in der Stadt. Teil 1). Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter, 467–509. 1995 Kommunikative Stilistik zweier sozialer Welten in Mannheim-Vogelstang (Kommunikation in der Stadt. Teil 4). Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter. Schwitalla, Johannes and Jürgen Streeck 1989 Subversive Interaktionen. Sprachliche Verfahren der sozialen Abgrenzung in einer Jugendlichengruppe. In: Hinnenkamp, Volker and Margret Selting (eds.), Stil und Stilisierung. Tübingen: Niemeyer, 229–252.
186
Inken Keim
Selting, Margret and Volker Hinnenkamp 1989 Einleitung: Stil und Stilisierung in der interpretativen Soziolinguistik. In: Hinnenkamp, Volker and Margret Selting (eds.), Stil und Stilisierung. Tübingen: Niemeyer, 1–26. Selting, Margret and Barbara Sandig (eds.) 1997 Sprech- und Gesprächsstile. Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter. Sirim, Emran 2004 Bilinguales Sprachverhalten bei jungen Deutschtürken. Unpublished M.A. thesis, University of Mannheim. Soeffner, Hans-Georg 1986 Stil und Stilisierung. Punk oder die Überhöhung des Alltags. In: Gumprecht, Hans-Ulrich and Ludwig Pfeiffer (eds.), Stil. Geschichten und Funktionen eines kulturwissenschaftlichen Diskurselements. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp, 317–341. Strauss, Anselm 1984 Social worlds and their segmentation processes. In: Denzin, Norman (ed.), Studies in Symbolic Interaction 5. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 123–139. Tajfel, Henri 1978 Differentiation Between Social Groups. Studies in Social Psychology. London: Academic Press. Tertilt, Hermann 1996 Turkish Power Boys. Ethnographie einer Jugendbande. Frankfurt/Main: Suhrkamp. Widdicombe, Sue 1998 Identity as an analysts’ and a participants’ resource. In: Antaki, Charles and Sue Widdicombe (eds.), Identities in Talk. London: Sage,191–206. Widdicombe, Sue and Robin Wooffitt 1995 The Language of Youth Subcultures: Social Identity in Action. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Willis, Paul 1981 ‘Profane Culture’. Rocker, Hippies: Subversive Stile der Jugendkultur. Frankfurt: Syndikat. 1982 Spaß am Widerstand: Gegenkultur in der Arbeiterschule. Frankfurt: Syndikat
Chapter 7 Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication* Kathryn A. Woolard 1.
Introduction
The notion that social identities are interactionally constituted and linguistically displayed is now quite well-established in linguistic anthropology, as well as in interactional sociolinguistics and sociology. Situated, contingent identities are constructed and enacted through the deployment of variable linguistic resources in interaction. Conversation analysts in particular have elaborated on this view of the contingent discursive achievement of identities. However, the linguist Robert LePage also influentially analyzed linguistic and stylistic choices as “acts of identity” on the part of individual speakers: “the individual creates the patterns for his verbal behavior so as to resemble those of the group or groups with which from time to time he wishes to be identified” (Le Page 1988: 31). Penny Eckert (2000) and colleagues have since developed the idea that speakers style unique social identities for themselves by cobbling together choices from an array of sociolinguistic variables. Whether from micro-sociological or sociolinguistic viewpoints, social identity is now generally seen as a form of discursive practice, and thus as fluid, multiple and negotiable. What has been less satisfactorily decided is what and where the limits of such speaker-centered, agentive interactional processes are. LePage included several caveats or “riders” to his model, to capture constraints on a speaker’s willful conjuring of an identity through linguistic practices. However, these were primarily cognitive and psycholinguistic constraints on a speaker’s ability to produce a target linguistic form. Questions about more social constraints on identity construction remain unexplored. How much freedom of range does identity-in-interaction have? How are the practices that produce ethnolinguistic identity distributed over space and time? What is the relevant social compass of the constitution and display of identity?
188
Kathryn A. Woolard
In one attempt to delineate the relations between interactionallyestablished identities and the macro-social order, the sociologist Don Zimmerman distinguished three types of identities that have different “home territories”: discourse, situational, and “transportable identities” (Zimmerman 1998). Race and gender are “transportable” in Zimmerman’s schema. Zimmerman treats these as forms of identity that “tag along with individuals as they move through their daily routines” because they are usually “visible,” on the basis of physical or (material) cultural insignia (Zimmerman 1998: 91). This conceptualization has been refined by Auer and Mair (2001), who rightly underline that such identities may be transported in more ephemeral behavioral insignia, including speech. Ethnolinguistic identities, i.e. ethnic identity categories defined by language affiliation such as Catalan and Castilian in Barcelona, are thus transportable identities. In his introduction to this volume, Auer asks to what extent participants can mobilize heterogeneity within the linguistic systems of their repertoire in order to symbolically express their affiliation with or disaffiliation from a given social group. Interactional sociolinguistics has generally highlighted speaker agency, creativity and fluidity in such symbolic expressions, and has also emphasized production. In this article, I will focus on the facets that correspondingly have been underplayed in the interactionist consensus: constraint, imposition, and the (in)validating reception of a speaker’s linguistic production of identity claims in interaction.
2.
Gatherings, bystanders, and identity
The principal argument of this article is that social identities, even those established in and through interaction, are transportable and tag along not only or necessarily because they are embodied in visible or hearable characteristics, nor because they are irrepressibly given off (as for Erving Goffman) in linguistic variables “from below” consciousness (as for William Labov), nor because they are produced or mobilized afresh in each new encounter (as in some conversation-analytic accounts). Rather, linguistically-displayed identities can be exported beyond transitory encounters and reified as features of individuals through chains of participation in gatherings, in social networks and in institutional settings. I develop this suggestion on the basis of Erving Goffman’s (1981) wellknown argument that the common two-person speaker-hearer model is inadequate for the consideration of significant aspects of social interaction.
Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication
189
There are not just two kinds, but various types of ratified and unratified parties to an encounter, Goffmann observed. He pointed out the significance of the “gathering”, which he defined as an aggregate of persons in visual and aural range of one another in a physical arena in which an encounter takes place (Goffman 1981: 136). “Routinely, it is relative to a gathering, not merely to an encounter, that the interactional facts will have to be considered,” Goffman wrote (1981: 136). Among the potentially consequential actors for any encounter is the “bystander”, a member of such a gathering who is an unratified, "adventitious participant" in the encounter by virtue of being within visual and aural range of its ratified participants (Goffman 1981: 132). Bystanders, Goffman pointed out, can catch the talk of an encounter in bits and pieces, and follow it partially and temporarily, becoming overhearers. Bystanders can even become “intended overhearers” (as distinct from addressees). Significantly for our purposes, Goffman suggests that even the most polite bystander will be able to glean information such as “the language spoken, and ‘who’ (in categorical or biographical terms) is in an encounter with whom” (1981: 132). The presence of bystanders, Goffman asserts, should be considered the rule rather than the exception. This rule, I suggest, has been observed mostly in the breach in research on ethnolinguistic identities-in-interaction. Certainly, analysis of the achievement and communication of identities-in-interaction has expanded beyond the two-person arrangement to multi-party encounters. Nonetheless, it has not strayed far from the imagery of engaged, participating speakers and addressees associated with the notion of “face-to-face” communication.1 However, identity is displayed not only to the active, immediate contributors to an interactional encounter, but also to non-participating (nonspeaking) witnesses or bystanders. Goffman’s fundamental insights allow us to consider one of the basic ways in which social identities can be simultaneously interactionallyestablished and yet transportable beyond a given interaction. That is, identities formulated or displayed in interactions are transported in part simply because these interactions occur not just in face-to-face encounters but in gatherings, where they are witnessed by bystanders. The bystander can function as a nexus for the portability of identity in several ways, among them the fact that the bystander to one encounter may play a significant active role in the next in many institutions and communities. In many gatherings, bystanders are not only future interactants but also future social arbiters or gate-keepers (Ehrlich 2002).
190
Kathryn A. Woolard
However, because bystanders’ access is partial, coming in bits and pieces, the formulation of identity they carry out may sometimes be systematically different from that done by participants in an encounter, even when they are invoking the same interpretive rules. Bystanders don’t see the same sequence of unfolding events, and from the same standpoint, that participants do.2 Because of this, sequential analysis even of the seeming entirety of an encounter (the bounding of an encounter already being a thorny issue in itself) cannot fully capture its consequences for identity assignment. Nonetheless, such consequences still can be seen as interactionally-given when we broaden our understanding of the relevant interactional field from the encounter to the gathering. I want to suggest that “face-to-side” and “face-to-back” communication to bystanders in the gathering can be at least as important as, and possibly even more consequential than, face-to-face communication within the encounter for the creation and management of transportable social identities. Between the proximal and the distal, to use the terms applied to the issue of identity by Zimmerman, we might say there lies a medial arena of identity, where portability materializes. The exploits of the first self-created individual in Western literature illustrate well the paradoxical pitfalls of interactive identity construction in a social world. Don Quixote’s radical project of self-transformation has been characterized as the “invention of individualism”, yet even this revolutionary of identity eventually found himself trapped in the identities he himself had created (Mariscal 2003). In the second volume of Cervantes’s tale, published in 1615, Don Quixote encounters several characters who have read the first volume, published in 1605. Quixote and his sidekick Sancho Panza “see their freedom slowly slip away” as former bystanders – readers – become participating characters who insist on staging interactions that realize the expectations they formed about these two based on their reading of the first book (Mariscal 2003: 4). As George Mariscal puts it, “no longer free to re-create themselves, Don Quixote and Sancho are in effect trapped inside both the preconceived ideas of others and their own fame” (2003: 4). What was true for our first early modern European hero is also true, I argue, for speakers in the late modern world of allegedly fluid identities. In the next sections, I will illustrate this simple claim, and hope to establish that it is non-trivial, with some examples from earlier research in Barcelona. All illustrations are taken from interviews with or interactions among adolescents, and most come from a group of 36 classmates in one school in the late 1980’s. (For a fuller report on this study, see Woolard 1997.)
Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication
3.
191
Ethnographic background
The social world in Barcelona is traditionally divided into ‘Catalans’ and ‘Castilians.’ Both referentially in discourse and pragmatically in interaction, these identity categories are fitted onto actual individuals with more or less difficulty (see Woolard 1989). While many criteria enter into play in different contexts, language choice is the fundamental shibboleth. For example, young people routinely and systematically transformed my interview questions about Castilian speakers or Catalan speakers into responses about Castilians and Catalans, as seen in the boldfaced segments of the exchange below: (1) KW:
M:
Tu notes diferències entre castellanoparlants i catalanoparlants?3 ‘Do you notice any differences between Castilianspeakers and Catalanspeakers?’ Els catalans i els castellans estan picats. ‘Catalans and Castilians are at odds’
In the system M draws on in this exchange, people who speak Catalan – catalanoparlants – are Catalans; people who speak Castilian – castellanoparlants – are Castilians.4 However, even the reduction of identity to the single criterion of language does not allow a simple, straightforward diagnosis. Given long-established bilingualism among Catalans and growing competence in Catalan among Castilians in Barcelona, the translation of actual patterns of language use into this dichotomy is a complicated linguistic ideological process. It involves what Irvine and Gal call iconicization and erasure, highlighting some aspects of linguistic behavior and ignoring others (Irvine and Gal 2000). For instance, only linguistic choices made in some interactions are relevant to claims to Catalan or Castilian identity. Home language and habitual language in informal solidary relations with others known to be Catalan are traditionally among these. The significance of the language spoken at home in diagnosing identity gives rise to the convoluted characterization of one student by another in example (2): (2)
yo tinc una amiga que parla el castellà i és catalana, bueno a casa seva són catalans, però ella sap parlar català però parla castellà perquè li agrada més.
192
Kathryn A. Woolard ‘I have a friend who speaks Castilian but she’s Catalan, well at her house they’re Catalans, but she knows how to speak Catalan but she speaks Castilian because she likes it better.’
The practice of assigning Catalan and Castilian identity on the basis of language use is also a particularly complex affair because linguistic choices can index the addressee’s identity as much as – or even more than – a speaker’s, as is well known. Overt acts of linguistic accommodation (Giles and Powesland 1975; Giles, Coupland and Coupland 1991) can index addressee-focused identity attributions rather than speaker-focused identity claims. A Catalan not only can but traditionally was expected to use Castilian even in informal domains to accommodate Castilians when directly addressing them. For this reason, speaking Castilian in interaction with a Castilian never has constituted a claim to Castilian identity for a Catalan speaker. Change in this traditional accommodation norm is in progress in autonomous Catalonia, but comes slowly. It still operated for many of the case study students, and for them language choice often indexed not speaker identity but addressee identity. For example, Frederic interacted with some classmates in Castilian, but they still classified him as Catalan because they understood him to be merely accommodating them. In turn, some students interacted with Sara or Tomàs in Catalan, and yet they categorized these two as Castilian (for reasons that will be discussed below). Young Catalans who routinely speak Catalan within their own ethnolinguistic group often volunteer that it is very difficult to use Catalan to someone who responds in Castilian, even when the response itself evidences full comprehension of Catalan. A passively bilingual conversation is felt by some speakers as a violation of some fundamental social norm, as seen in examples (3) and (4): (3)
És que és mania. Yo no li puc parlar a una persona que a mi em parla en castellà, yo, no em surt en català. ‘It’s an obsession. I can’t speak [Catalan] to a person who speaks to me in Castilian, me, it won’t come out in Catalan.’
(4)
És molt difícil parlar amb una persona que està parlant en castellà i tu li parles en català. Sembla que li parlin en xino, és molt difícil. ‘It’s very hard to speak to a person who’s speaking Castilian and you talk to them in Catalan. It’s like speaking to them in Chinese, it’s really hard.’
Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication
193
Such accommodation presents a challenge to LePage’s speaker-focused “Acts of Identity” model. In accommodation, a speaker’s language choice projects and adapts to the linguistic preferences, abilities or identity of the addressee, but does not constitute a claim about speaker’s own identity, except perhaps as a polite person who puts others before self. Such accommodation might indeed be said to assert a claim about affiliation in the sense of liking and solidarity with the addressee as an individual (Giles and Powesland 1975). That is precisely what Castilians in Barcelona have sometimes said. During this fieldwork in the 1980s, one sometimes heard it said of a Catalan who breaks the accommodation norm and maintains Catalan when Castilian is returned, “he cares more about his language than about me.” But the expression of sympathy (or lack thereof) in this sense is not the same as a claim about speaker’s (transportable) social identity or category membership. Like Giles and Powesland's, LePage’s model is built in part on an implicit equation of these two kinds of solidarity (social alignment and social/linguistic similarity), but the validity of this equation of liking to likeness is a question that has long been begged in sociolinguistic studies (see, e.g., Woolard 1985). The question is underlined when we consider that traditionally – and still – some Catalans accommodate not just to an interlocutor’s active projection of identity, as seen in that interlocutor’s choice of vehicular language for the conversation. Rather, they may accommodate to what they understand to be the interlocutor’s ‘underlying’ transportable identity as diagnosed from other conversations witnessed, even if such accommodation violates the interlocutor’s own language choice in the present interaction. Castilian-speaking students who were trying to master Catalan often found that Catalan classmates responded to them in Castilian, even when addressed in Catalan. Frustrated learners understood these Catalan classmates’ language choices with them to be indexical of an unchanging classification of their ethnolinguistic identity and linguistic skills as “Castilian”, as these two comments from Josep illustrate: (5)
amb alguns de la classe potser yo els parlo català, no?, perquè el vull aprendre, però molts em contesten en castellà ( ) no sé, creuen que yo els entenc millor. ‘with some of the people in the class maybe I speak Catalan to them, you know? because I want to learn it, but many of them answer me in Castilian, ( ) I don’t know, they think I’ll understand them better.’
194 (6)
4.
Kathryn A. Woolard El que passa es que jo soc castellà, no? Em tenen com un castellà pero jo parlo perfectament el català i les fa com cosa de parlar en castellà, no? Es creuen que jo no entenc el català, no? ‘What happens is that I’m Castilian, no? They see me as Castilian but I speak Catalan perfectly and it’s like a thing with them to speak [to me] in Castilian, you know? They think I don’t understand Catalan, you know?’
Adolescent language choice, identity, and classroom bystanders
Despite the general importance of the home language to the establishment of ethnolinguistic identity and to ensuing language choices in Catalonia, adolescents in my case study routinely acknowledged that they couldn’t tell which language their classmates spoke at home. Knowledge or inferences (founded or unfounded) about home language didn’t usually underlie their categorizations of classmates and resultant language choices with them. Rather, aspects of language choice witnessed at school provided the basis for them to distinguish Catalans and Castilians among their classmates. Most (though not all) of the students in the classroom I studied could be heard speaking Catalan and Castilian at some time in the school setting. So what language choices did students attend to in diagnosing the identity of their peers? The language used in the classroom was sometimes relevant, but only sometimes, given the dominance of obligatory Catalan-medium instruction. Attention to classroom language use in discussing peer identities depended on the interactional activity in which identity assignment occurred. When I asked in interviews about typical differences between Catalans and Castilians, students considered the language used by fellow students in official classroom activities relevant to placing them in one category or the other. Many students responding to these interview questions used the term “Castilian” to mean functional monolinguals who did not learn and use Catalan, even at moments officially mandated or suggested in the classroom. In this sense, active social or home use of Castilian didn’t necessarily make a person Castilian; rather, the failure to become actively bilingual in Catalan did. “Castilian” in this use was a monolingual identity based on an absence rather than a presence. The focus of the Castilian identity category in this sense was in those who didn’t use Catalan even
Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication
195
when it was officially required in classroom activities. To be Castilian was to refuse the use of Catalan. In talking about their actual friendship circles, however, and at the point of selecting a language to speak with individual classmates, use of Catalan in official classroom activities did not usually count as identity–relevant. Rather, it was taken as contextually mandated and therefore not a significant sign of identity that would affect interaction among peers and friends. This pattern suggests a paradoxical effect of a successful official language policy in relation to acts of identity. Thanks to Catalan-medium schooling, more people than in the past may be speaking Catalan – in school, casual interactions, and commercial exchanges – without this meaning that they either count themselves or are counted by others as Catalans. This is a parallel to the fact that under the former Castilianist sociolinguistic regime before Catalan autonomy, speaking Castilian in official domains didn’t signify a claim to Castilian identity or a loss of or challenge to Catalan identity. Within the frame of a new sociolinguistic policy, linguistic behavior once seen as identity-claiming can lose that meaning. Changes in patterns of linguistic choice do not then necessarily reflect or affect speakers’ personal sense of affiliation with social identity categories; i.e. they are no longer necessarily acts of identity. The language used in interaction with classmates in general also did not usually count in peers' identity assignments in the case study classroom. The language choice these students most often used as the key diagnostic of identity was that which an individual used with those peers recognized as his or her ‘friends’, in a more intimate and solidary relation than just “classmates”. Observed language choices in encounters in which the diagnostician was not a ratified participant were thus often central to decisions about identity. We can illustrate the effects of language choice with close friends on bystanders by looking at Sara and Eva, two girls in the case study classroom of 36 students. Nearly three-quarters of the girls in the class participated in homogeneous “best-friend” relationships or networks, in which all members not only used the same language, but came from the same linguistic background and shared the same ethnolinguistic identity. These networks were also relative closed and cliquish (see Woolard 1997).5 Sara and Eva were anomalous, and their presence accounted for almost all of the heterogeneous friendship networks among the girls in the class. Eva was from a Catalan-speaking home, and Sara was from a bilingual home, where she spoke Catalan with her father. Both girls used Catalan
196
Kathryn A. Woolard
fluently with teachers and Catalan peers. But both were associated with cliques of girls from exclusively Castilian-speaking homes, and these associations were recognized and commented on by classmates. Sara and Eva each adapted to, or accommodated, the language habits of her circle of friends. Sara used only Castilian with her principal friends. Eva codeswitched between Catalan and Castilian, in the manner of her group – something that native Catalan speakers do not usually do but is more common among second-language speakers of Catalan whom I call “New Catalans”.6 So, while they often spoke Catalan outside these circles, both girls used Castilian, either exclusively or intermixed with Catalan in a nontraditional way, in their close friendships on display in the classroom. These associations and accompanying linguistic behaviors stamped the girls’ identity for many of their classmates. Their ability to speak Catalan was recognized, but at the same time as it was often taken to be non-native or secondary to their identity. So, in answer to my question, “who are good speakers of Catalan among those who don’t speak it at home?”, five classmates named Eva and six named Sara, and most used terms that showed that they equated my category “those who don’t speak Catalan at home” to “Castilians”. Several more initially categorized Sara or Eva this way, although they then repaired their classification: (7)
El Rafael, el Víctor, i després no sé si l’Eva és castellana o catalana, però l’Eva. ‘Rafael, Víctor, and then I don’t know if Eva is Castilian or Catalan, but Eva.’
(8)
Eva. Bueno, l’Eva parla de les dues maneres, pero més bé, el castellà. ‘Eva. Well, Eva speaks both ways, but more often, Castilian.’
(9)
Son castellanes, Rosario, Margarita, Elena, Adela, Sara. Sara parla molt el català. El que passa es que parla castellà perquè va amb aquestes. ‘They’re Castilians, Rosario, Margarita, Elena, Adela, Sara. Sara speaks Catalan a lot. What happens is that she speaks Castilian because she goes around with these girls.’
To some classroom bystanders, Sara and Eva were not just girls who had Castilian friends, but themselves became Castilian speakers (castellanoparlants) and “Castilians”. To be seen as a traditional Catalan when speaking Castilian, one must be known to be accommodating, that is, moving from a
Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication
197
Catalan-speaking position to Castilian for the sake of an addressee. Because all their intimates were Castilian speakers, Sara and Eva could not routinely do this. Frederic describes exactly this pattern and its effect on his own response to Sara: (10)
El primer trimestre i aixins quan no ens coneixiem ningú, doncs, jo sentia parlar castellà amb les seves amigues, li parlava en castellà. Amb ella parlo en català, a vegades. ‘The first trimester and all when nobody knew each other, well, I heard [Sara] speak Castilian with her friends, so I spoke to to her in Castilian. [Now] I speak Catalan with her, sometimes.’
Despite the newer counter-evidence, however, the label stuck in Frederic’s social mapping of the classroom. Frederic still thought of Sara as “Castilian” and listed her as such in answer to my question. The social challenge in such a situation is the converse of the problem of “passing”. Those who attempt to “pass” performatively from one identity to another may avoid interaction with members of their group of origin, who might “out” or unmask them in interaction to bystanders. In cases like Sara’s and Eva’s, individuals unintentionally lose their legitimately claimable ascriptive identities of origin because they do not regularly interact closely with members of their group of origin, and they thus lose the chance to be observed and overheard using the “right” language. Although they are not attempting to pass as Castilian, this identity is imposed on them by bystanders as a result. In contrast to Sara and Eva, however, two Catalan-origin and Catalan self-identified boys, Emili and Frederic, were able to maintain close friendships with Castilians, accommodating them in Castilian, without losing their Catalan identities among their classmates. That was because, unlike Sara and Eva, these boys moved in a large bilingual circle of friends, which allowed them to display their Catalan ability in Catalan friendships. Frederic described himself as using both languages equally within his group, “because we’re a group of friends, half Catalan and the other half Castilian. So you have to speak mixing them”. As could be predicted from this quote, Frederic practiced the traditional Catalan accommodation norm of switching to the language he associated with the identity of his addressee; he rarely actually “mixed” the languages in a single utterance unless he was switching interlocutors. One of Frederic’s friends, Paco, was indeed just about as monolingual a Castilian speaker as could be found. But neither such a Castilian friendship, nor Frederic’s linguistic accommodation
198
Kathryn A. Woolard
to it, created any confusion or weakening of his Catalan identity among his classmates. The same was true for Emili. I suggested in an earlier analysis that this difference was evidence of the differential power of association for girls and boys (Woolard 1997). That is, not only did I observe gender differences in the structure and composition of social networks, but I also posited that there was bias in the ideological template students applied in interpreting observed relations and behaviors, depending on which sex exhibited these behaviors. Peer association seemed to count more, to be more constraining, in determining girls’ social identity than boys. This fit with the argument developed by Eckert that ‘persona’ or a set of identities is the source of girls’ status, while action is the basis for boys’ status in a peer hierarchy (Eckert 1989). Now taking into consideration the dynamics of interaction from the bystander’s point of view, I would reframe that interpretation. Gender enters the equation at the formation and shaping of the friendship circles in the case study classroom, but not necessarily at the reading of them by bystanders. Thus, the bilingual, bi-ethnic makeup of Frederic and Emili’s circle allowed them to display clearly to bystanders that in speaking Castilian, they were accommodating to addressee’s identity rather than expressing their own (i.e. to disambiguate participant-related codeswitches and signal that addressee rather than speaker was the participant to whom the decision is keyed). Within friendship interactions, they observably used sometimes monolingual Catalan and sometimes monolingual Castilian, and this fit within traditional norms of Catalan identity. In contrast, the more homogeneous circles of Sara and Eva did not enable them to display a Catalan identity to observers outside those circles through their language choice with friends. Even though they might have been drawing on the same linguistic norm as Fredric did – that of matching their interlocutors’ language choice – these girls did not have the opportunity to affirm a Catalan identity for themselves, since none of the close friends whose linguistic behaviors they matched were Catalan. The conversational participants whom they addressed in Castilian might themselves interpret this choice as linguistic accommodation to them rather than as an identity claim on the part of these speaker (and there is indeed evidence from interviews that the friends understood Eva and Sara to be Catalans). Observing from outside, however, bystanders were given no evidence that these girls’ social selves, as founded in close friendships, were Catalan, and they might easily arrive at a different interpretation of the meaning of the linguistic behavior.
Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication
199
The differentially confining character of friendships for these girls and boys can be accounted for by the fact that the composition of their friendship networks allowed different opportunities for display of identity to bystanders. There is probably a second point to be made here about focusing vs. diffusion, a concern that LePage associated with acts of identity. Certainly it is true that under norms of linguistic accommodation, the more the social network is focused in one ethnolinguistic group, then the more focusing the effect on the identity of the occasional anomalous member. Girls’ homogeneous networks are focused and focusing in this sense, in that there is significant matching of linguistic background, current linguistic habits, and identity assignment. But the opposite is not necessarily true. More heterogeneous networks that are not so highly focused in one ethnolinguistic group don’t necessarily lead to a diffusion of an individual’s linguistic habits or a breakdown of ethnolinguistic identity. In this classroom, ethnolinguistically diffuse networks and diffuse practices did not diffuse identity for the boys. Rather, they allowed members to keep strong but different identities. Mixed male friendship groups led to less identity crossing into an alternate identity than the girls did, not more. Nor did the mixed male groups seem to encourage a hybrid or mixed identity. Such a hybrid identity did exist in the bilingual “New Catalan” girls, but it did not derive from on-the-ground mixing between Catalans and Castilian girls as close friends (see Woolard 1997). For boys in this classroom whose friendship patterns were more similar to those of the girls – i.e. dyadic or homogeneous – the effects on their recognized identities were similar to those for girls. This occurred with Tomàs, a native Catalan speaker who routinely used Castilian with his single close friend, Jesús. Tomàs systematically accommodated Jesús in Castilian, and some of Tomàs’s classmates and even teachers mistakenly assumed he was himself “Castilian”, and not a habitual speaker of Catalan (though they could hear and admired his Catalan linguistic ability in the classroom). (11)
No em sé, no, perque no sé quina parl – si que parlen a casa seva. El Tomàs, creo que es castellano y habla muy bien el catalán. ‘I don’t know, because I don’t know who spea- if they speak at home. Tomàs, I think he’s Castilian and he speaks Catalan very well.’
200
Kathryn A. Woolard
For another boy whose network was large but ethnolinguistically nearly homogeneous, the same kind of effect was achieved as for Sara and Eva, but in the reverse direction. Several classmates were taken by surprise to learn through a teacher’s classroom comment that Rafael, who moved in an exclusively Catalan-speaking circle, was of Castilian background: (12)
En Rafael diu que és castellanoparlant i yo no ho hauria dit mai, perquè és clar, per a mi sembla que sigui català. ‘Rafael says he’s a Castilian speaker, but I never would have guessed that, because, obviously, to me he appears Catalan.’
(13)
És que jo, vull dir, el Rafael, no m'ho pensava jo que parlava en castellà,...el Rafael, no m'ho pensava. I quan ho va dir, (laugh) em va sorprendre, no? ‘It’s just that I, I mean, Rafael, I never thought that he spoke Castilian... Rafael, I never would have thought it. And when he said so, (laugh) it surprised me, no?’
Rafael in fact had sought the Catalan identity that classmates like this one ascribed to him, and he had gone to some trouble to transform his identity when he entered this high school, as shown in example (14): (14)
Vaig (decidir) si jo m’ho proposo parlar català, parlaré en català desde principi…el primer dia de curs, no, vaig dir, bueno, aquí al [school name] parlo català I ja està. I parlo català. ‘I decided that if I wanted to speak Catalan, I would speak Catalan from the start…The first day of school, you know, I said, ok, here at [school name] I speak Catalan, and that’s it, I speak Catalan.’
When I asked what language he spoke with classmates, Rafael distanced himself from Castilian speakers among them: (15)
Als que parlen més en castellà, doncs en castellà, però normalment els meus amics casi tots son catalans, parlo en català sempre. ‘To those who speak more in Castilian, then in Castilian, but normally my friends are almost all Catalan, I always speak Catalan.’
Prompted to list Castilian speakers in class, Rafael again took pains to make clear that they were not his friends:
Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication
(16)
5.
201
Adela, Rosario, Sara..el José Luis, el José, algú més, no me’n recordo ara. Però aquests no son tant amics, jo vaig amb els altres, normalment. ‘Adela, Rosario, Sara, ..José Luis, José, some others, I don’t remember now. But these aren’t really friends, I go around with the others, normally.’
Exploiting bystanders, bystanders exploiting
As Rafael’s case suggests, any sociolinguistically indexical fact can be, and often is, creatively exploited and manipulated by speakers (Silverstein 1995). So, in language choice as in so many other interactional phenomena, we find speakers playing to the crowd. Singh refers to this as the theatrical and game-playing aspects of code identification, and views these as overlooked in the traditional analyses of codeswitching and ingroup identity (Singh 1983). Language choice can be intended to be overheard, and thus potentially mobilized for identity claiming. For example, one of the students who showed the most identity entrepreneurship was Laura, one of the ‘New Catalans’. She was one of Eva’s friends, Castilian-origin bilinguals and codeswitchers. On a science field trip, Laura became separated from her clique. Looking for them, she ran across a large field where most of the other kids were occupied in various ways. As she ran, Laura shouted loudly On sou? On sou? (‘Where are you?’) in Catalan. It is particularly noteworthy that Laura used a public call in Catalan to locate her intimates among a field of far more Catalanidentified potential addressees. All her classmates could thus “overhear” her using Catalan not just to address her ingroup of friends, but apparently to single them out. Such a language strategy seems more designed for public display to intended overhearers than for these particular intended addressees. Since these girls most often used Castilian or a mixed-language strategy among themselves, they in fact were not especially likely to recognize themselves as the intended addressees, out of all the young people present, of a Catalan-medium cry. If speakers such as Laura can manipulate their interactional displays of language choice to bystanders, bystanders can in turn define or redefine displays, whether intended or unintended to be overheard. (In fact, by orienting to overheard language choice publicly, bystanders can imply that the choice was intended to be overheard and throw doubt on its authenticity.)
202
Kathryn A. Woolard
Josep, the boy who complained in example (6) that classmates ignored his use of Catalan and pegged him as Castilian, provides an example. As an incipient Catalan-speaker trying to restructure his identity in this Catalanoriented high school, he preferred to use the Catalan form of his name, Josep, rather than the Castilian form, José. In general, the linguistic form of one’s name was taken as a sign of the ethnolinguistic identity one claimed and was accorded (see also Bierbach and Birken-Silverman, this volume). Thus, teachers discussed this boy with me using the Castilian form of his name in otherwise monolingual Catalan discourse, signaling that they identified him as Castilian. His closest friend in the bi-ethnic boys’ network, Emili, was helping Josep by making occasional moves to address him in Catalan. This began with naming; Emili was among the first to use the Catalan form that Josep himself preferred, rather than the Castilian, which teachers used. On the science field trip, a female Catalan student who was not a ratified participant overheard Emili address this boy as “Josep”, the Catalan form. She laughingly and loudly commented in Catalan to her girlfriends, ‘Josep’, li diu ‘Josep’ (‘’Josep’, he calls him ‘Josep’’), ridiculing the language choice and orienting other bystanders to it as dispreferred. Such ridicule presumably not only was based on her perception of Josep as Castilian but also reinforced this perception among her classmates.
6.
Conclusion
We find that people’s ethnic identities are formulated and oriented-to in interaction not only by participants in encounters but also by bystanders. As members of recurring gatherings, bystanders are one of the constraints on the autonomy of the speaker in constructing identity, which I posed as an issue in the beginning of this article. Bystanders can do much to focus, transport and institutionalize the interactionally-inscribed social identities of others. Moreover, individuals in different social circles can find that their identities are differently ‘diagnosed’ and reinforced by bystanders not because they enact different language choice principles in interaction, but rather because of the structural particulars of the overheard interactions in which they frequently participate. We can say that ‘face-to-side’ and ‘faceto-back’ communication is as relevant to the establishment of social identity as is the ‘face-to-face’.
Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication
203
Young people often are all too acutely aware of the effect of interactional displays to bystanders and the gathering on their social identities, not only ethnic identities as displayed in linguistic choices, but more generally. As seen in both the Catalan study and in Eckert’s (2000) work on high school identities in Detroit, transitions to large middle or high schools create just the kind of gatherings in which bystanders can quite rapidly and powerfully institutionalize individuals’ social identities on the basis of partially observed and overheard encounters. In recurring and institutionalized social gatherings such as the classroom and playground, bystanders’ categorizations of interactants’ identity create conditions that constrain further identity displays in future interactions. Like Don Quixote and his friend Sancho, schoolchildren often learn to their chagrin that they quickly get locked into identities they didn’t necessarily intend to claim, in the children’s case through the company they keep. In exchange, some find that the artful staging of displays to bystanders in peer interactions is a good route to successfully claiming new, sought-after identities.
Notes * Earlier versions of this paper were presented at the Colloquy on “Acts of Identity” organized by Peter Auer and Christian Mair, Freiburg, Germany, February 2002, and in the session, “What’s Left of Ethnolinguistic Identity?”, organized by Jan Blommaert and Robert Moore, at the annual meetings of the American Anthropological Association, Chicago, IL, November 2003. 1. For other topics there have been expansions beyond the face-to-face, most notably in the work of Charles Goodwin and Marjorie Goodwin. M. Goodwin’s (1990) analysis of “he-said-she-said” routines, for example, traces the way interactions ramify through relationships in a social network. Studies of gossip (e.g., Haviland 1977) also show such social ramification of communication. 2. By extension, analysts have the same limitations, and it would be useful to consider analysts as bystanders. Unfortunately, this is an issue beyond the scope of this article. 3. In all transcribed examples, italic script represents Catalan. Castilian speech is represented in underlined text. Text is rendered in boldface to make an analytic point. Poorly intelligible stretches are represented by ( ). 4. Readers will rightly ask whether young people categorized their peers ethnically or ethnolinguistically when not prompted to do so by an interviewer. Although this is an important question, I cannot explore the complexities here. Students in this study varied in how likely they were to introduce ethnic and
204
Kathryn A. Woolard
ethnolinguistic labels spontaneously to discuss their social world. Classroom observations and evidence from naturally-occurring talk showed that the ethnolinguistic distinction was relevant in peer and friendly interactions, but in varying degrees for different students. For some evidence, see Woolard 1997. 5. Network data was collected through both interview and observation of interactions and classroom seating. 6. Classifying the linguistic habits of Eva’s friends, for example, one student from outside Laura’s network said: “Laura and those girls, they talk to each other in Castilian. But if you talk to them, they answer in Catalan. It depends on the moment you catch them.”
References Auer, Peter and Christian Mair 2001 Acts of identity. Unpublished manuscript. Eckert, Penelope 1989 The whole woman: Sex and gender differences in variation. Language Variation and Change 1, 245–267. 2000 Linguistic Variation as Social Practice; The Linguistic Construction of Identity in Belten High. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. Ehrlich, Susan 2002 Legal institutions, nonspeaking recipiency and participants’ orientations. Discourse & Society 13(6), 731–747. Giles, Howard and Peter F. Powesland 1975 Speech Style and Social Evaluation. London: Academic Press. Giles, Howard, Justine Coupland, and Niklas Coupland (eds.) 1991 Contexts of Accommodation: Developments in Applied Sociolinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Goffman, Erving 1981 Forms of Talk. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Goodwin, Marjorie 1990 He-Said-She-Said: Talk as Social Organization Among Black Children. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. Haviland, John B. 1977 Gossip, Reputation, and Knowledge in Zincantan. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Irvine, Judith T. and Susan Gal 2000 Language ideology and linguistic differentiation. In: Kroskrity, P. V. (ed.), Regimes of Language. Santa Fe: School of American Research, 35–84.
Bystanders and the linguistic construction of identity in face-to-back communication
205
Le Page, Robert B. 1988 Some premises concerning the standardization of languages, with special reference to Caribbean Creole English. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 71, 25–36. Mariscal, George 2003 Tilt. Review of Don Quixote. Miguel de Cervantes, Edith Grossman (trans.). Union Tribune. San Diego, Books 1–4. Silverstein, Michael 1995 Indexical order and the dialectics of sociolinguistic life. In: Ide, R., R. Parker, and Y. Sunaoshi (eds.), Third Annual Symposium About Language and Society-Austin. Austin, TX: University of Texas, Dept. of Linguistics, 266–295. Singh, Rajendra 1983 We, they, and us: A note on code-switching and stratification in North India. Language in Society 12, 71–73. Woolard, K. A. 1985 Language variation and cultural hegemony: Towards an integration of sociolinguistic and social theory. American Ethnologist 12(4), 738–748. 1989 Double Talk: Bilingualism and the Politics of Ethnicity in Catalonia. Stanford: Stanford University Press. 1997 Between friends: Gender, peer group structure, and bilingualism in urban Catalonia. Language in Society 26, 533–560. Zimmerman, Don H. 1998 Identity, context and interaction. In: Antaki, C. and S. Widdicombe (eds.), Identities in Talk. London: Sage, 87–106.
Part 2. Monolingual styles and social identities – From local to global
Introduction to Part 2 Peter Auer While Part 1 of this volume deals with variation and linguistic choices out of a multilingual repertoire and the way in which these choices, being part of certain social styles, may become relevant for participants’ identities, the core of sociolinguistic research on variation has been concerned with a different type of choices: those within a linguistic system, i.e. by a monolingual speaker. The chapters in this part of the book address questions of variability within a language on the level of the vocabulary, phonology or grammar of ‘a language’. However, these chapters are united with the papers in the first part in that the same methodologies are followed. In particular, linguistic choices are not investigated as single parameter variation (as is mostly done in ‘variation studies’), but are rather embedded into a network of features which is socially interpreted as a style. These styles can be seen as the basis of performances on a stage, be it a political stage or that of the World Wide Web. As the chapters in this section show, styles therefore include more than grammatical or phonological variation: some of their most salient properties are on the rhetorical level. The communities in which speakers act may be those of a traditional sociolinguistic study: a regional speech community into which the overarching standard variety is imported. However, they may also be part of global youth culture, or a migrant community in which old (established) and new (adapted from the receiving society) linguistic resources coexist and interact. In Ch. 8, Nikolas Coupland combines the investigation of speech styles, political rhetorics, and linguistic choices by looking at one political performer in Wales, Aneurin (‘Nye’) Bevan, the founder of the British National Health Service. Nye remains an icon of British socialism and was a politician of considerable stature in the British political arena in the 1940s and 1950s. He was also renowned, however, for setting his South Wales Valleys working class values against those of a dominant Conservative government based in Westminster. The data source is a newly discovered audio-recording of Nye speaking at a political rally in 1959, one year before his death. Of sociolinguistic interest is Nye’s shifting between phonological features redolent of the British middle-class establishment of the
210
Peter Auer
period and features that powerfully evoke a South Wales Valleys vernacular ideology. The rhetorical effect of the performance more accurately lies in the interplay between Nye’s selection from his dialect-style repertoire and the subject positions he establishes in the non-phonological dimensions of his discourse. Nye’s public political rhetoric has often been described as ‘sneering at the establishment’, but as Coupland points out, it can be better accounted for in terms of his vocal stylizing of Wales-Westminster political antagonisms. While the relevant identities in Coupland’s study are the personae of a ‘national politician’ and of a ‘spokesman of Wales’, both of which are activated by Nye in his speeches, Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain investigate a particular kind of ‘migrant identity’ in Ch. 9. The migrants here are people who lived in the Federal Republic of Germany before the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989 and moved to former East Germany at various times between 1990 and 1998; the relevant identity category is ‘West German’ (and, by co-activation in the membership categorization device, ‘East German’). These migrants share the personal experience of moving to the east at a time when East and West Germans were still coming to terms with their different pasts and negative stereotypes of each other. Due to their ‘in-between’ status, they have potential ties to both previous and new identities, places and experiences. In many interactions, they are challenged to position themselves in relation to these past and current affiliations. The paper aims at identifying features of a communicative style through which a ‘migrant identity’ emerges in such interactions. Four main features are identified: (i) an additional conversational effort, (ii) a delay in categorizing, (iii) a metalinguistic deconstruction of the categories ‘East’/’West‘ and (iv) the presentation of oneself as an animator rather than an author in relating factual matters of the new place. In the third paper in this section (Ch. 10), Jannis Androutsopoulos investigates the Web as a social space in which like-minded individuals use the resources of the medium (interactivity, multimodality, unlimited access to media production, etc.) for purposes of self-presentation and community formation. In constructing and negotiating their online identities through text and talk as well as other semiotic resources, participants may choose to reproduce an aspect of their offline social identity, or construct alternative versions of self. As Androutsopoulos shows, sociolinguistic style is a crucial part of identity formation in CMC. It is a boundary marker through which participants position themselves vis-à-vis other participants. Online styles are hybrid, however, drawing on resources of face-to-face discourse
Introduction to Part 2
211
as well as specific CMC resources. As the social identities negotiated on the Web are often framed in global popular culture, sociolinguistic styles on the Web are immanently related to processes of cultural globalization. This particularly holds for hip-hop culture in Germany from which Androutsopoulos draws his examples. The hip-hop field of activities on the Web is structured along participation formats and discourse activities (e.g. discussion board member; homepage author; semi-professional website writer). Its concomitant style displays certain core features which occur across formats and activities and are recognized by members as typical for the field. Among them is an orientation to spoken language features (“conceptual orality”), technical terms and categorizations, distinctive routines and phrases, slang items of African-American English origin, codeswitching into English, spelling variants, naming patterns, and preferences in typography and visual imagery. Style differences along these features construct social, activity-related and ideological diversity in the field.
Chapter 8 Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism Nikolas Coupland 1.
Aneurin Bevan
Aneurin Bevan, affectionately known as ‘Nye’, is an iconic figure in the history of British politics.1 He is the icon of British democratic socialism in the twentieth century.2 His reputation conventionally centres on his role in founding the British National Health Service. The NHS, for all the politicking around it over successive decades, remains the greatest achievement of the 1945 Labour government and is without doubt the single most socially transformative institution introduced by any administration on the British political left. But even beyond this achievement, Bevan’s career symbolises successful working-class resistance, through mainstream political processes, to large-scale and destructive capitalist exploitation of working communities. Aneurin Bevan was a class warrior, a socialist hero of modernity. The roots of Bevan’s life and politics were in South Wales. Steel fabrication, but even more powerfully coal mining, defined the South Wales Valleys’ position as the hotbed of nineteenth and twentieth century industrialisation. Rapid deindustrialisation, mass unemployment and waves of extreme social deprivation from the 1920s through to the 1980s, whose legacy is far from eradicated today, decimated the Valleys communities. But it also further galvanised working-class consciousness and radical political action in South Wales, documented in the historical commentaries of Gwyn Alf Williams and Dai Smith (G. A. Williams 1985; Smith 1999). Industrial fervour and post-industrial turmoil have come to constitute one of, in Raymond Williams’ expression, the “two truths of Wales” (R. Williams 2003), the other in his view being the romanticised, Welsh-language based, Celtic and Cymric Wales. Aneurin Bevan epitomises this first truth, implementing the radical politics of the left as it developed in South Wales and impacted on Britain
214
Nikolas Coupland
through the depression years of the 1920s and 1930s and between the wars. Born in 1897 in Tredegar, on the eastern extremity of the South Wales coal field, a colliery worker from age 13, Bevan quickly engaged with and moved up through local political groups, becoming a member of parliament for Ebbw Vale in 1929. He went on to become a cabinet minister with responsibilities for health and housing in the post 1945 Labour government, and deputy leader of his party. It is widely thought, nevertheless, that Bevan failed to fulfil his political potential at the very highest level. He is reputedly the greatest British politician never to have led his party or become prime minister. Current British prime minister Tony Blair writes that, since Bevan’s death, “his ideas and personality and rhetoric passed on from generation to generation … and the remaining soundtracks of his speeches give people born after he died the chance to hear the power of his oratory” (Blair 1997: 11). Bevan’s biographers, political inheritors and admirers represent him as a complex individual, in his political, personal and social dimensions.3 There is a debate about whether Bevan construed himself more as a rebellious agitator than as a mainstreamer, even though he was in fact highly influential in the political mainstream at Westminster. At the same time there is strong support for the view that he could be a pragmatist and a political accommodator, as well as being, overridingly, a socialist ideologue fervently committed to principles of social justice through public ownership and redistributive taxation, and enraged by capitalist exploitation. He was capable of utterly ruthless actions, decisions and verbal attacks, notwithstanding that he was generally companionable and sociable, and even tender and shy, in dealings with friends and non-political acquaintances. He was fiercely proud of his Welsh social origins in Tredegar and appealed passionately to his childhood experiences as a basis for his political philosophy: democratic socialism was the only means of redressing the social injustices he had experienced at first hand. All the same, he developed a wide social circle of friends, including the newspaper proprietor Lord Beaverbrook. He read and often quoted poetry and philosophy. He had a reputation for sharp dressing and enjoying good food and drink. Kenneth O. Morgan calls him “an aristocrat in outlook and taste” (1989: 13). On this count, as Morgan explains, he was criticised as “a Bollinger Bolshevik, a ritzy Robespierre, a lounge-lizard Lenin”. A mythology around what is elsewhere called his “champagne socialism” was recycled by some who wanted to undermine his otherwise impeccable working-class credentials. His Welshness provided the foundation for his politics, yet he was a com-
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
215
mitted internationalist and analysed ‘the Welsh problem’ essentially in terms of class conflict and repression, not nationalism. Dai Smith writes: The question of Wales, per se, was not one to which he gave much thought in isolation. It was what had taken place in Wales throughout its industrial history which concerned him and, by extension, how the people who had undergone that experience might make dynamic social capital out of their material exploitation (1997: 71).
The facet of Aneurin Bevan’s political career that is relevant to the present book’s concerns is his standing as a political speaker (mentioned in the Blair quote) and how he carried his social identity through his oratory. Michael Foot, a former leader of the Labour Party, places Bevan among the three most impressive and effective political speakers of the last century, but with Bevan far ahead of the other two, Winston Churchill and David Lloyd George (Foot 1997: 193). His rhetorical range in public speaking, at public political rallies and in the House of Commons, was considerable. His fulminating and corruscating attacks on the Conservative Party (the Tories, a term Bevan used almost as an insult), but sometimes also on members of his own party, were notorious. He famously once said in public that he considered the Tories to be “lower than vermin”, a remark that appears to have damaged him more than his opposition (Smith 1993: 254). But it is not an overstatement to talk of his hatred for the Tories, and we get a clear sense of this in some of the data extracts to be considered below. There is good support for the view that Bevan either would not or could not speak from prepared notes or a script, extemporising his speeches, though on the basis of extensive rehearsal.4 He often made capital from aggressive rejoinders to hecklers and spontaneous attacks on opponents who were present in the debating chamber. Prosodically and paralinguistically, Bevan’s voice was highly distinctive. It was generally high-pitched and thin, but he could infuse a gruffness and power that carried aggressive and furious affect. Foot calls it “sweet and strong at the same time” (1997: 202). He would often segment utterances into short, matched strings, punctuated by even-length pauses, in the grandiose manner of public political oratory, partly no doubt to allow his words to reach large and noisy audiences through the rudimentary public address systems of the day. He would manage speech rhythm and pausing, amplitude and intonation to produce swooping rhetorical sequences that often culminated in key political points. At other times his speeches would
216
Nikolas Coupland
create a surprising intimacy and audiences would be drawn into quasidialogic debate with him, or even actual dialogue. Biographers talk of his ‘speech impediment’, though this seems to have amounted to a recurrent but relatively non-intrusive block rather than an iterative stammer. He commonly had uvular /r/ in all linguistic contexts (for example in Tories, every, result, sincere in), and although this feature has a marginal status as a selectively regional characteristic of South Wales speech, it might be part of the lay perception of an ‘impediment’. In recordings made late in his life, he seems to have had badly fitting dentures, blurring his sibilants. His phonological habitus was decidedly South Wales Valleys, which is a major sub-variety of Welsh English (Garrett, Coupland, and Williams 1999, 2003), but not without frequent and sometimes extreme variation between Valleys variants and RP-like variants of certain speech variables. His speech also included occasional idiosyncrasies of word stress which might be the result of bookish but extensive self-education rather than formally institutionalised education. These features were overlaid on a remarkably confident, inventive and powerful oratorical competence which was the cornerstone of his political success. What, then, can the sociolinguistics of style offer to an appreciation of Aneurin Bevan’s public speaking, and vice versa? I shall introduce the data after some brief overview comments about the history of style in sociolinguistics, to situate the approach I will take in the analysis.
2.
A sociolinguistics of style
Variationist sociolinguistics at first treated style as an interesting but relatively minor and potentially distracting dimension of language variation. It was minor in that it was considered theoretically or in explanatory terms secondary; it was distracting in that it pulled away from a commitment to finding structured regularities in speech communities, which was held to be the discipline’s main obligation. This is Jack Chambers’ view when he writes that, for his integrative theoretical overview of variationist sociolinguistics, “style is an important independent variable but it is never the focal point” (Chambers 2003: 6). Style has been rehabilitated to a large extent through Rickford and Eckert’s integrative (2001) volume. That book establishes a dialogue between (i) William Labov’s original conception of style (1972) as a contextual plane of language variation; (ii) later relationallyoriented models, most notably Allan Bell’s audience design framework
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
217
(e.g. Bell 1984, 1999, 2001) and the communication accommodation framework of Howard Giles and others (e.g. Giles and Powesland 1975; Giles, Coupland and Coupland 1991), and my own studies of style as persona management (Coupland 1985, 2001a, 2001b); (iii) established perspectives on register (e.g. Biber and Finegan 1995); and (iv) a variety of other sociolinguistic perspectives, many of them originating in linguistic anthropology and organised around concepts of genre, performance and language ideology. Style research was also substantially revitalised from the late 1990s by Ben Rampton’s research on sociolinguistic crossing and stylistic creativity in relation to ethnicity and social class (e.g. Rampton 1995, 1999, 2001) and the steady flow of studies influenced by this important initiative, theoretically linked to the perspectives of Mikhail Bakhtin and Erving Goffman (Bakhtin 1981, 1986; Goffman 1981) – e.g. Bucholtz (1999), Cutler (1999), Schilling-Estes (2004). Although this is not the place for detailed discussion, there are at least the following arguments in favour of extending sociolinguistic engagement with style: – Style research builds bridges between variationist analysis of accent/ dialect and the many other distinct or partially distinct traditions of sociolinguistics, many of them grounded in ethnography and in the analysis of discourse (cf. Eckert 2000; Jaffe 1999; Johnstone 1996; Johnstone and Bean 1997); that is, it breaks out of autonomous variationism. – Style opens qualitative perspectives on social identity, for example by clarifying and extending the ‘acts of identity’ perspective (LePage and Tabouret-Keller 1985), seeing identities as interactionally negotiated and accomplished (Auer 1998). The language and social identity nexus is of course at the heart of all traditions of sociolinguistics. Styled identities are not (only) ‘who social actors are’ but how they can be represented to be, for specific purposes. – Style convincingly breaks the chain of assumptions that ensnares speakers as naive but perfect and authentic members of speech communities (Coupland 2003; Rampton 2000), and points up the limitations of treating speakers as social prototypes or as full owners of their speech. – Style leads us towards richer models of the social contextualisation of language (cf. Auer and Di Luzio 1991); indeed, it was the need for better accounts of language and social context that gave much of the original stimulus for sociolinguistics; style is the contextualising of social meaning.
218
Nikolas Coupland
– Style invites sociolinguistics to engage with aesthetic qualities of talk and its metalinguistic design, requiring critical sensitivities in analysis. – Style challenges us to take a wider theoretical stance on the relationship between social structure or “systems of distinctiveness” (Irvine 2001) and social agency in situated linguistic performance, without trivialising either part of this relationship (cf. Coupland 2001c, 2007). Following from these points, a sociolinguistics of style generally aligns with a constructionist view of social life. In this view, social meanings, identities, relationships and language ideologies are both invoked and actively created and styled in the dynamics of social interaction (cf. Antaki and Widdicombe 1998). Meaning resources are not so much sociolinguistic ‘features’ available for ‘selection’, although these concepts have predominated in the analysis of style variation. Rather, semiotic resources are historically and culturally imbued potentials for making meaning (Hasan 1996) and for achieving material and symbolic outcomes (Bourdieu 1977, 1991). They are indexical relationships between linguistic/discursive forms and clusters of social values, practices, norms and assumptions, but not in a simple, one-to-one mapping arrangement. These resources are available for deployment (creative, strategic use and placement) by speakers in evolving discursive contexts which renew and potentially reshape their value loadings. Because semiotic resources are generalised associations between linguistic forms and social meanings as part of cultural ideologies, speakers must necessarily (in Bakhtin’s term) ‘reaccentuate’ them or revalorise them in their unique discursive contexts. To that extent, stylistic operations are distinctively innovative, and certainly agentive and contextualising. But they are only possible because of the socially structured indexicalities that link ways of speaking to social groups or situations, and to the ideological conditions that define these at particular times and places. Relative to this broad agenda for a sociolinguistics of style, it is interesting to note that the dominant traditions of style research so far have actually had rather specific qualities. William Labov’s seminal observation on style – we can think of it as the first wave of sociolinguistic research on style – was that speakers are not stylistically uniform, and that there are readable associations between social situations and stylistic arrays for social groups. Labov’s insight has mainly been developed, in a second wave of sociolinguistic style research, as the search for generalisable statistical patterns and transcontextual explanations for style shifting. It was the uniformity of the directional tendency for speakers to shift their pronunciation
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
219
towards prestige variants during more formal or monitored speech activities during interviews that mattered to Labov’s account of sociolinguistic patterning. While Giles’ accommodation theory and Bell’s audience design models (frameworks in which my own early research on style was developed; see Coupland 1980, 1984) posit different and more resolutely social bases for regular style shifting (‘accommodating a listener’ and ‘designing one’s speech for an audience’), their priority is still to generalize about the conditions under which and the extent to which ‘speech convergence’, ‘speech divergence’ and so on will happen. Motivational and contextual accounts are more explicit in accommodation theory, while audience design is more sociolinguistically precise and entertains the idea of converging to non-present ‘audiences’ in ‘initiative’ style-shifting (Bell 1999). Style is treated mainly as a linear concept in both approaches (see Coupland 2001b for further discussion and critique). Both models give us a rather sparse account of the semiotic resources that are deployed – what their specific histories, values and affordances are – and very little detail on the social identification processes that inevitably mediate ‘audience effects’. But style points us above all to the local dynamics of sociolinguistic variation, and abstractions to ‘what many people stylistically do as general tendencies’ must necessarily sacrifice the detail of those dynamics. The mindset to establish generalisations about stylistic patterning in the service of predictive theory (if this is taken to mean establishing statistical regularities about different speakers’ style-shifting across different codable social situations) is an inheritance from survey research in sociolinguistics, where the essential claims have to be distributional and correlational. Generalisation in the domain of style is not necessarily, and arguably not naturally, of this order. It can aspire to more abstract and less predictive generalisations about the role of language variation in macro-micro sociological dimensions, processes in the articulation of social identities and relationships through discourse, and to interpretive frameworks within which myriad local acts of social construction operate. In this third wave approach to style, we need to be more ambitious in analysing stylistic constructions which do not reduce into consolidated general ‘strategies’ tied to linear sociolinguistic continua (such as increasing formality or reducing interpersonal differences and distance) and which do not fit neatly into simple theoretical/motivational architectures such as ‘a drive for social approval’ or ‘designing talk for listeners’. (For more on the three waves of style research, cf. Eckert and Mendoza-Denton 2002.)
220
Nikolas Coupland
Another tacit assumption in the sociolinguistics of style has been that what we can grossly call ‘dialect’ (sociolinguistic variables, often phonological, which function semiotically in the demarcation of speech communities as well as in the demarcation of social groups within them on the basis of frequencies) provides the necessary linguistic focus for style research. This restriction to a rather autonomous stylistics of dialect variation has certainly produced valuable coherence across studies, but it has also implied that the role of stylistics is secondary to the role of ‘social’ (or intergroup) variation, where dialect does necessarily define the territory. In fact, in the domain of style, dialect semiosis needs to be seen as only a part of the complex of discursive constructions that style achieves. It is a productive part, and largely what I will be concerned with in the Aneurin Bevan data. But even so it is specifically in the interplay across semiotic dimensions that social meaning is created and interpreted. The implication is that a sociolinguistics of style needs to be a committedly holistic analysis, even though dialect stylistics or ‘dialect in use’ (Coupland 1988) might continue to be its principal empirical focus. Finally, we should recognise that the sociolinguistics of style, in both first and second waves, has been the study of style-shifting. Often it is our awareness of ‘shifted’ variants that draws attention to stylistic meaning, and the idea of studying linguistic deviation from some presumed norm has a long pedigree in general stylistics. But understanding ‘how style means’ in discursive operations need not be focused around patterned shifts, qualitative or quantitative, within speakers’ speech across contexts. The basis unit of analysis for a sociolinguistics of style is a single semiotic unit, and the analytic demand is to explain how its activation contributes to speakers’ negotiation of social meaning in a discourse. Abstraction to general tendencies may of course follow from this, but these tendencies need not be where style is recognised. When shifting is in question, there is also the risk of adopting mechanistic and realist criteria for ‘context’. As an alternative to this survey-type, contrastive orientation, a holistic and discursively richer approach will need to recognise differential framing of semiotic resources. In the Bevan data, with multiple dimensions of rhetorical control at his disposal, dialect resources are invoked in subtle ways; their impact is far more dependent on the discursive frame that is in place (and which might come to be in place through dialect constructions themselves) than on some putative disjunction between ‘how that speech variable is realised at other moments’. Contrast is a powerful sensitising factor, but style analysis need not be the documentation of contrast per se.
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
221
The meaning potential of sociolinguistic style is realised in at least the two overlapping arenas of social identity and social relationships. Styling is part of the construction and deployment of a speaker’s and others’ social identities, which might be to invoke and to consolidate the values and attributes associated with a ‘speech community’ (for example interpreted as a social class and its associated political ideologies). On the other hand it might involve establishing a particular stance vis-à-vis those community norms, where ownership of voice becomes less or more clear – e.g. the voicing of a class position one wants to subvert. In turn, the styling and performance of social personas and stances achieves different relational orientations and effects, such as purporting to accredit an opponent’s ideological position or stealing/appropriating their own value system. The interplay between dialect styling and other dimensions of discursive construction can achieve highly nuanced meanings and effects, of the sort I hope to demonstrate in the analysis of Aneurin Bevan’s oratory. Behind this is the puzzle that Bevan is ideologically rooted on one side of the key discursive conflict of high modernity, class war, while we might think that his propensity to manipulate voice and social identification gives him impressive latemodern credentials. (I return briefly to this puzzle at the end of the chapter.)
3.
The Nye data
A small set of fragments of audio- and video-recordings of Aneurin Bevan’s speeches has been in circulation since his death, mainly embedded in political journalism features about Bevan on radio and television.5 The same sequences tend to recur in these recordings, and they have thereby become something of a standard repertoire (referred to by Tony Blair, above). Critical biographical commentaries on Bevan’s career often dwell on moments illustrated by these famous fragments, although, as far as I am aware, they have not been analysed stylistically before. Regrettably, contextual details of the speeches are limited, although it is possible to infer some general characteristics of the social settings of the recordings from the general acoustics and audience involvement. In one case, the historical context makes the precise setting obvious (see below). For reasons to do with developing recording technologies and practices, all the extracts available are from the later years of Bevan’s life. We know that one recording in the set was made in 1959, one year before Bevan’s death from cancer.6
222
4.
Nikolas Coupland
A performance space
The cultural distance between Aneurin Bevan’s Tredegar upbringing and the dominant mores of the House of Commons between 1929 and 1959 was inevitably stark, and was marked dialectally in varieties of English. Senior Labour Party politicians on the political Left as well as the Right, including Clement Atlee who was Labour Party leader between 1935 and 1955 (that is, for much of Bevan’s time in politics) presented a decidedly patrician, upper-class demeanour and speech style, which was clearly considered to be generally appropriate for figures in public life. Nye brought to Parliament a phonological habitus that was quite different from this conservative norm. Over time, and certainly by the time of the recordings available to this study, Nye had acquired the facility to deploy RP-like and ‘establishment sounding’ variants (and sometimes even conservative RP variants) of certain sociolinguistic variables, variables whose vernacular variants would have been (and still are) strongly diagnostic of Valleys English (or South Wales English generally, in many people’s perceptions). Table 1 lists the most salient sociolinguistic variables and variant forms that can carry this opposition. Lexical examples are taken from the data extracts. Table 1. Phonemic/phonetic variables distinguishing Valleys and RP speech Variable name
Valleys variant
RP variant
Conservative RP variant
Examples and comments
(ou)
o:
NT
?T
[o:] – [NT\ contrast is available in one lexical set, e.g. coal, although, so, over; [NT\ is normative in a second set, e.g. told, knows, ownership; Z?T\ is the conservative RP realisation for both sets
(ei)
e:
eH
Thiscontrast is available in one lexical set, e.g. educating, Ebbw Vale, misbehaving, nation; [eH\ is normative in a second set, e.g. train, day, neighbour
(au)
?T
`T
e.g. pounds, out, now
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
223
(ai)
?H
`H
e.g. might, private, why
(a)
a
æ
This contrast is available in stressed ‘short a’ contexts, e.g. fact, national, established, including in words that have a short vowel in South Wales but a long vowel in RP, e.g. circumstance, where lengthening is another option
(a:)
a:
@9
(y)
i
H/Hd D?
These contrasts are available in orthographic final ‘y’ words and in ‘-ly’ adverbials, e.g. finally, city, integrity
(2:)
8:
59
e.g. heard, perfectly, years
(?(
?
U
This contrast is available in stressed syllables, e.g. dull, must, come
(iw)
Hw
ju
e.g. opportunity, constituency, you
(h)
Ø
h
Ø (zero) in word-initial orthographic ‘h’ contexts hands, e.g. house, and favoured in have, had, he, his, etc.
(ing )
n
M
[n] in verbal ‘-ing’ contexts and lexical compounds with ‘-thing’, e.g. educating, going, nothing
This contrast is available in ‘long a’ contexts, e.g. argue, laugh, tarnished
It is impossible to hear the extracts without being aware of how Bevan’s socio-phonology interfaces with his various other rhetorical designs and ploys to create social meanings and effects. Socio-phonetic forms and meanings constitute rich semiotic resources, in the sense I outlined earlier, for Nye’s performances. Above all, the social class resonances of specific variants are highly salient in his speeches, evocative of (in gross terms) ‘vernacular’, ‘working class’ and ‘Valleys’ meanings versus ‘establishment’ or ‘middle class’ meanings, but also in this context evoking political ideologies of public ownership versus capitalist profit. These meanings and
224
Nikolas Coupland
the phonetic variants that they potentially index can be seen as opening up a performance space in which Bevan is free to operate discursively, to embed sociolinguistic values within his rhetorical designs. It would be wrong to ‘fix’ the meanings of phonological variants (as the column labels in Table 1 might appear to do) before we meet them in Nye’s discourse. A crucial point is that macro-level value systems can not be read directly or unambiguously from the phonetic shape of variants themselves. The crucial mediating consideration is Bevan’s styling of his own and others’ identities and relationships, how his speeches dynamically position culturally significant voices, and how Bevan builds implications about his own and his opponents’ orientations to these voices. We will see that Bevan appears to project some establishment speech characteristics, on some occasions, as qualities of his own preferred political persona. On other occasions he constructs them as objects of derision and hatred. This is why the ‘situational’ account of the data needs to be developed in terms of discursive frames and not in more materially or temporally realist ways. Discourse analyses of these meaning creations are necessarily post hoc and interpretive, open to alternative readings; this is the essence of a local stylistic analysis. All the same, valuable support for some lines of interpretation is available from the sequential organisation of the talk, particularly in the evidence of where and how Nye places certain segmental phonetic realisations in the rhetorical structure of his talk, and how they sometimes mesh or conflict with other social meanings. How audiences react by inferably reading humour or other affective qualities from elements of Nye’s performance is again revealing for its interpretation.
5.
Framing, footing, keying
A clear characteristic of Bevan’s public oratory is his propensity to engage different oratorical modes. He is most famous for his scathing wit and sneering derogation of political opponents. This often entails sarcastic humour, where his strategy is often to build fellow-feeling with an audience, designing his utterances dialogically, while ridiculing opponents and their arguments. We can take a discursive frame (Goffman 1974; Tannen 1993) to be a mutually achieved set of understandings of what goals and affordances are operative at any one moment of interaction. Bevan was recurrently able to establish a particular frame of expectations (let us call it the dialogic sneering frame) where Left-inclined audience members would
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
225
expect to have Tory Party actions or characteristics paraded before them for ridicule. Audiences would be on an interactional footing where they were under a demand to be judgemental, and where their own derisive laughter or applause was expected as an audible element of the discourse at moments which Bevan would orchestrate. This is a mode of political speech-making sometimes loosely referred to as ‘knock-about’, entailing non-serious and often mock-serious key: playful but nevertheless damaging characterisations of opponents’ arguments and stances. A strongly contrasting frame in Bevan’s repertoire is what we can call the denunciation frame. In this frame an audience’s footing would be much less participative and there would be less of a demand to respond or indeed to anticipate Bevan’s lines of argument and punch-lines. The audience could expect to be engaged affectively in a serious key, on topics close to the ideological centre of Bevan’s politics, and to be invited to follow and echo Bevan’s outrage at the unfairness or incompetence of the Tories. Whereas Bevan’s humour, often cruel, was definitely entertaining, constructing opponents as ludicrous, his denunciation sequences tore into them, on-record, from a position of the moral high ground. Extract (1), a famous sequence, demonstrates these two particular frames. This speech is at an outdoor venue to a large crowd. Almost certainly this is Bevan’s speech at the ‘Law not War’ Labour Party rally in Trafalgar Square in London at the time of the Suez crisis, November 1956.7 Tory prime minister Sir Anthony Eden had called on Egypt and Israel to withdraw their forces ten miles from the Canal. When they did not comply, Britain and France attacked Egyptian airfields the very next day, in defiance of the United Nations’ Charter. (1)
Get out get out get out
1
Sir Anthony Eden has been pretending (2.0)
2
that he has he is now invading Egypt (.)
3
in order (.) to strengthen the United Nations
M
Ø h
Ø
e: M
ai
((3.0 laughter; audience: rubbish)) 4
er every every um (.)
ei
226
Nikolas Coupland
5
every burglar of course (.) could say the same thing
8:
e:
((2.0 laughter)) Ø
6
a:
M
Ø
h ?T
he could argue that he was entering the house in order to > train the police
((4.0 loud laughter)) o:
Ø
M
7
so if Mis- Sir Anthony Eden (.) is sincere in what he is saying (.)
8
and he may be
Ø
((2.0 laughter; audience: ah with low-fall intonation)) Ø
9
he may be
((1.0 slight laughter; ‘yeah’)) Ø
M
Ø
10
then (.) if he is sincere in what he is saying (.)
11
then he is > too stupid to be a Prime Minister
Hw
Ø
?i
[ ((wild laughter)) (There is a break in the recording here, and the extract may therefore not be continuous, although it appears to be.) a:
ae
12
we are in fact in the position today (1.0)
13
of having appealed to force in the case of a small nation (1.0)
14
where if it is appealed to against us (1.0)
15
it would result in the destruction (.) of Great Britain-
16
-not only as a nation
ha M
e:
ei
?
?
ou
?
ei
ei
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism M
17
227
M
but as an island con/ taining living men and women
((2.0 audience: hear hear)) 18
therefore I say to Anthony Eden (1.0)
19
I say to the British / government (2.0)
20
there is no / count at all upon which they can be defended (2.0)
21
they have besmirched the name of Britain (3.0)
22
they have made us ashamed of the things (.) of which
?
?T
o:
8:
h
Ø
e:
e:
?
e:
?T
formerly we were proud ((4.0 loud cheers)) Ø
D?
ou
23
they have (.) offended against every principle of decency (2.0)
24
and there is only one way in which they can (.) even begin to restore their
ou
a:
?
u ei
>tarnished reputation ((audience: get out)) ?T?T?T
25
and that is to get out (.) get out get out
[ ((loud cheers))
In the first 11 lines Bevan represents Eden’s policy of military intervention in Egypt as motivated by a desire to strengthen the United Nations. This is a curious proposition, presumably reworking some specific comment by Eden that the policy of attacking Egypt would show the power of the United Nations. Bevan parallels the invasion with burglars breaking into a house, and ‘strengthening the United Nations’ with ‘training the police’ (lines 5 and 6). This of course imputes criminality to the attack and criminal behaviour to the Tories, as well as exposing the ludicrous nature of the
228
Nikolas Coupland
supposed motivational reasoning. Then we have the mock-serious concession that Eden may be being sincere in his (ludicrous) beliefs (lines 7–9), before the abusive put-down that Eden’s being sincere would leave stupidity as the only available explanation. Bevan has invited the audience to reflect on the illogical motivation of to strengthen the United Nations up to line 3, and a supportive audience is already audibly ‘rubbishing’ this motive in the pause between lines at that point. Bevan’s apparent fumbling for a comment (the disfluencies in line 4 – and some commentators have suggested he sometimes feigned disfluency to increase the appearance of spontaneity – see note 4) sets up an anticipation of a derisory remark to come. He then tantalises his audience with his as-if accepting of Eden’s sincerity, presenting the opportunity for them to reach their own denial of this sincerity before he articulates it himself. Some audience members duly give him laughter and a prolonged lowfall ah after line 8 (to the effect, ‘ah, we don’t believe that’) and slight laughter and an ironic ‘yeah’ after line 9. But then, Bevan’s debunking of Eden in line 11 turns out not to be simply a refutation of sincerity, but the more damaging suggestion that Eden cannot be sincere without also being too stupid to be a prime minister. This is a true Bevanite discursive ploy – eliciting an audience evaluation then transcending or trumping that evaluation in his own punch-line comment. From line 12 Bevan drops out of the mock-moral evaluation of Eden’s sincerity and moves into the denunciation frame. He loads up his censure with direct and extreme morally evaluative expressions, especially over lines 20 to 24. He asserts his own authority in the highly formal metapragmatic design I say to Anthony Eden… (lines 18, 19). The frame is not participative in the way the earlier frame was. Bevan moves out of personal attack into attacking Tory policy, from sarcasm to direct evaluation, and from light-hearted play to heavy condemnation.
6.
Socio-phonetic imaging
How does dialect semiosis work as a resource in these framings in Extract (1) and elsewhere? Bevan’s delivery over lines 1–5 is slow and deliberate, with a small non-fluency in line 2 possibly resulting from his wanting to ‘correct’ an aitchless realisation of he.8 There is no consistent patterning of variants. There is variation between [e:] (the prototypically South Wales value) in invading (2) and same (5) and RP-like [ei] in Nations (3). (Nation
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
229
in the political entity the United Nations as a lexeme is less amenable to phonetic vernacularisation than non-proper-noun items, although Bevan seems to favour RP-like [ei] in the word nation generally.) But when Bevan draws the ‘burglar’ parallel, the complete line (5) has a strong vernacular quality. This is partly because Bevan has lexically relegated the status of the grammatical subject from Sir Anthony Eden to every burglar (5), but the semiotic associations of vernacular [8:] in burglar add to the effect. Bevan constructs a second parallel contrast between Eden’s grandiose purpose (to strengthen the United Nations) and his feeling that anyone could say the same thing, with vernacular [e:] in same. We begin to see that, in an abstract sense but with some evidencing in phonetic substance, there might be two voices in Bevan’s account. The first is not Eden’s own voice, but it is a voice which superficially endorses Eden’s stances and claims. The second is a critical and sceptical voice, drawn from and evoking a vernacular culture of unreliable burglars. These phonetic images are fleeting and not fully consistent, although features in sentence-stress position, and probably vocalic more than consonantal features, have most salience. They cannot in isolation carry the values I am attributing to them (thankfully, because there is no suggestion that all South Walians are burglars), although I am sure we can say that they are part of the ideological contrasts that Bevan is constructing. The ‘sinceritystupidity’ sequence has something similar about it. The ideational/ pragmatic thrust of his talk at that point is to offer respect to Eden’s being sincere in what he is saying. But the too stupid to be a prime minister phrase (line 11) is the targeted punch-line, and is delivered on a markedly higher pitch, with increased amplitude, and with a resoundingly South Walian [Hw] in stupid. This is the critical voice returning, speaking abrasively and personally, but from a vernacular social base, willing to drop out of polite and respectful parliamentary protocols of ‘trusting the sincerity of opponents’. As a personal judgement, too stupid to be a prime minister is uncompromisingly disrespectful, direct and unhedged. Lexically, stupid sets up a self-consciously unsophisticated and perhaps even puerile basis for personal judgement; socio-phonetic imagery adds support, implying a specifically South Walian, ‘no nonsense’ intolerance. But the utterance is also voiced from a position of superiority – Bevan’s reputed intellectual elitism, added to the moral basis of his critique of invading Egypt. Intellectual and moral elitism with vernacular authority is very much Bevan’s distinctive political persona, and indeed it is a key part of the mythology of workingclass Welshness.
230
Nikolas Coupland
There are contrasting socio-phonetic nuances in the powerful, censorious sequence in the second frame, down to and including line 24. Some keywords have resonant South Wales Valleys pronunciations: no count at all in line 20, ashamed in 22, tarnished in 24, and of course the final get out get out get out (where three flapped intervocalic [t] sounds support the Valley’s voice with marked informality and non-institutionality), where Bevan voices the communal rhythmic chant that the crowd itself might adopt. Yet there are also several RP-like realisations of (h), (ng) and (ei), and the phonetically striking line, they have offended against every principle of decency (23). Within this line we have the unusual, hypercorrect spellingpronunciation of (ou) (realised as /ou/) in the first syllable of offended, and the highly conservative RP realisation of ‘y’, [İԥ] + as the final syllable of decency. Bevan produces this feature on some other occasions too. To my ears it is evidence of him fleetingly accommodating Westminster’s patrician phonological mores, before, on this occasion, the sequence ends with a rousing return to vernacular values and class action, in get out get out get out. The moral high ground he is claiming in this sequence in some ways naturalises the conservative variant, reversing the polarity of vernacular authority in the earlier sequence. ‘Principles of decency’, Bevan is implying, ought to be values shared by all people in government. At the same time, [İԥ] undoubtedly also contributes to the impression of Bevan’s ‘champagne socialism’ that his left-wing critics disapproved of. We can look in less detail (for lack of space) at two other extracts. Extract (2) is from a speech at an unspecified indoor venue. The acoustic quality of the recording suggests the speech is made in a large and full hall, once again with a generally supportive audience. Bevan again establishes the dialogic frame to sneer at the Tory government. Either by chance or through some planned device, an audience member heckles him, and receives a verbal assault quite similar to the too stupid sequence. (2)
Dull going out
1
> even the Tories themselves have been rather surprised at what’s
ha
h ae
been 2:
2
M
happening (2.0) ?i
?i ?i ai
it’s the first time in my lifetime (2.0)
@:
?i
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism Ø
231
?d9
h ae
3
that we have had a Tory government in Great Britain (.)
4
without having had mass unemployment at the same time
h a M ha
a
?i
e:
((3.0 audience murmuring)) ae
5
Ø
?i
e:
ai
and this s- circumstance has taken even the Tories by surprise
((4.0 some laughter)) Hw
?
6
and you (can’t) wonder at it (2.0)
7
because they don’t know why it’s happened
?i
ou
Ø ae
((5.0 growing laughter and applause)) ?i
h ae
8
?u
and they want to have a general election before they find out
((7.0 bursts of loud laughter and strong applause)) ?h
9
59
D?
> (now) I want to be perfectly fair to them
((4.0 growing laughter, probably responding to a non-verbal gesture from Bevan)) ou
10
?i
look (th)ere’s no reason why we shouldn’t because we can beat them that Ø?u
way anyhow ((4.0 laughter, an apparently hostile audience member calls out repeatedly))
[ ?u
11
?u
?u
now (.) now (.) listen now (1.0) this is a very serious
[ ((more calling out, above laughter)) Hw ou
12
Hw
?
o: M ?u
Hw
l-listen carefully because if you don’t you’ll be as dull going out as you ?
M
were coming in ((loud laughter and cheering))
232
Nikolas Coupland
Bevan opens this sequence with controlled, measured prosody. In line 1 he preserves some aitches, has RP-like open back long [a] in rather and the ‘posh’ close variant, short [a] in happening is prominent. In line 2 he ‘corrects’ the final realization of the (ai) variable to an open-onset RP-type variant. The effect continues through RP-like had in line 3 and [h] and velar [M\ in having had in line 4, plus the close short [æ] in the very formal phrase this circumstance in the following line (even though a full RP realisation would be with long open [@9\). Bevan is imputing naivety to the Tories, who, he says, can’t account for why there isn’t mass unemployment while they are in office. It has often been said of Bevan’s rhetoric that he liked to attack the strong points more than the weak points in his opponents’ positions, and here he appears to be conceding that unemployment levels under a Tory government are not too high. But of course he undermines the Tories’ position by claiming that they are not in control of the employment situation: they don’t know why it’s happened (line 7). We have the incongruous pairing of aitchlessness and close short [æ] in the single word happened in the utterance that triggers the laughter, and the effect is of an insincere RP affectation, usefully creating a supercilious persona. The two voices we met in the earlier extract pull apart from each other rather more clearly in lines that follow, particularly in the socio-phonetic contrast between lines 9 and 10. 9 has RP-like [5:] and [İԥ] in perfectly fair, which is again a heavily stylised establishment voice. It is similar to the every principle of decency sequence in Extract (1), but this time metaparodic (parodying its own stance). Bevan is affecting an honourable, House of Commons-type demeanour of fairness (again similar to he may be sincere in what he is saying in Extract (1)), although the dialogic frame is obviously and successfully bringing in the audience’s judgements to the contrary (after lines 5, 7 and 8). The audience laugh at Bevan’s mock commitment (in this local instance) to fairness, and he reacts to their laughter with the disjunctive look (as if he needs to correct their cynicism), and a strongly vernacular line 10. Its vernacular quality is partly in the phonetics of some familiar phonological variables (especially the aitch-dropped and centralised onset to (au) of anyhow) and partly its lexical reference to ‘beating them’ (another studiedly unsophisticated way to represent political success). Another facet is the syllable-timed rhythm of this line (not marked in the transcript) which contrasts with Bevan’s more typical RP-type stresstiming. In line 10 Bevan acknowledges that his mission is of course to beat the Tories and not at all to be fair to them, retrospectively confirming the as-if design the earlier line, but also trumping the assumption that we need
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
233
not be fair to the Tories, with the idea that ‘even if we are fair to them, we will still win’. It is Bevan’s attack on the heckler that elicits the strongest audience support (in the last two lines). Bevan rounds on the heckler, with the patronising fall-rise tone on the last syllable of listen now, and warns him that he will remain as dull going out as he was coming in if he does not listen. Just like stupid, dull is a personally offensive and self-assertive insult, though perhaps more directly so here because Bevan also constructs the implication that the heckler could have been illuminated by his (Bevan’s) truth, if only he had listened. Just as [Hw] in stupid contrasts with RP [ju]:, dull has the vernacular Valleys imprint of schwa rather than RP [U\, with dull perhaps being lexically one of those socially self-contextualising words that have different connotations and perhaps denotations in RP versus vernacular phonology. The final extract is an extended sequence from a 1959 recording, made a few months before the general election (which Labour were again to lose). Bevan is campaigning at a public meeting. (3)
Complete poppycock
1
> at last we might have an opportunity of educating the British
?i
a:
h
ju
i
e: n
?u
people about the truth (3.0) ?u ai
o:
Hw
H
2
now I represent a (.) a steel and coal constituency (2.0)
3
we are still nationalised (3.0)
4
the Ebbw Vale er the (.) the R T B’s (.) Richard Thomas and / Baldwin’s (.)
ae
ai
e:
ei
still belong (.) / to the nation (.)
Ø
a
?i M
5
they haven’t succeeded i- in denationalising it > yet (1.0)
6
al / though they’ve been at it for seven years
o:
a
8:
234
Nikolas Coupland
((3.0 slight laugher)) U
H?
7
that is still public property (3.0)
8
and it’s a very prosperous concern (3.0)
9
in fact (.) it’s so prosperous and efficient (.) that even Mr Macmillan (.) told
2:
a
o:
h ?u
?
the House of Commons the other day (.) Hw
o: n
?u ?i
a
Hv
10
that the new strip mill that’s going to be established outside Newport is
n9m
?
2:
a
going to be entrusted to the national(ist) > concern (4.0) ?u
11
Hw
Hw
Ø
now you would have thought wouldn’t you (.) that er er if Mr Macmillan U
really believed (.) that the public ownership of > steel (1.0) was a
ei
Ø
h
?
Hw
er disadvantageous to the nation (.) then he ought to have entrusted the new ?i
2:
steelworks to (.) a > private concern (2.0) ?i Ø a
Ø
12
well why hasn’t he? (3.0)
13
why is he so misbehaving himself?
?i
Ø
e: M h
o:
((3.0 laugher)) ?i
14
Ø
o:
a
why is he so neglectful of the national interest?
((2.0 laugher)) Ø ha
15
o:
2:
o: M
?u
that he hands over a concern that’s going to cost about sixty million ?u
?h
>/ pounds before it’s finished (1.0) to a (.) a publicly
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
235
owned steelworks (4.0) ?
16
Ø
h
> doesn’t he believe his own æ
@9
propaganda? ((3.0 strong laughter)) ?i Øm
17
why is he doing it? (2.0) Ø
18
M
Ø
a
he’s doing it of course because he knows (.) that the propaganda that they’re n ?u
putting out is complete poppycock ((2.0 slight laughter; audience: of course)) 19
com>plete poppycock (3.0)
This is another mixed-frame sequence, although it is overall a more orderly speech event, with less audience participation and better acoustics, than the previous extracts. Bevan creates another opportunity to accuse the Tories and Harold Macmillan in particular of acting inconsistently and ‘not believing his own propaganda’ (lines 1–19). We see his typically mixed sociophonetic profile, with several RP-like variants early on (in opportunity [but not its final syllable], nationalised, nation, property, etc.). For most of the extract Bevan is speaking as a ‘serious Westminster politician’ and there is some sustained phonetic support from the RP-like features for this construction, bolstering the grand initial claim of educating the British people, the credentialising self-reference (line 2) and the generally expert formulations of the social policy topic. The sixth line, which elicits slight laughter, is heard by the audience as an aside or a quip, briefly breaking the established serious key, and its slight disfluency and three vernacular variants help to mark the momentary incursion into Bevanite mockery. The original frame is quickly reinstated in line 7 with its RP-like realisation of the phrase public property, which is a concept dignified by an establishment pronunciation of both words. At line 11 Bevan begins his querying of Macmillan’s integrity, on the evidence of his decision to make substantial new investment in a public steelworks. The frame becomes overtly dialogic. The question-tagged now
236
Nikolas Coupland
you would have thought wouldn’t you (11), and the follow up, more insistent question well why hasn’t he? (12), and the sarcastic why is he so neglectful of the national interest? (14), all have uniformly vernacular variants; Bevan is now styling himself as agitator and critic. A further question is voiced in the same style: why is he doing it? (17), although it is preceded by a candidate explanation in line 16 that draws strong audience laughter: doesn’t he believe his own propaganda? This utterance begins with the same vernacular design, but the long rise in pitch across the whole utterance results in the extremely high-pitched final word propaganda. The third syllable of the word is a very close, RP-like variant of short [a] (unusual in Bevan’s repertoire) and the final syllable is another hyper-correct spelling-pronunciation, the stylised Z@9\. As the line develops Bevan voices first the vernacular critic, but then blends into an abstracted propagandaising Tory persona. The stylisation itself evokes the disjunction between truth and propaganda, which is after all precisely how Bevan thematises the speech at its outset. In the final two lines of the extract, Bevan consolidates his critique of Tory propaganda as complete poppycock, which some audience members have evidently already conceded (see of course after line 18). The critical vernacular voice is a requirement for this consolidating line and it is unsurprising that we find a string of vernacular variants (except for the velar nasal in doing) in line 18. Notice that this second, vernacular-voiced realisation of the word propaganda has regained its Valleys-like open ‘short a’ form [a] and lost its hyper-correct final syllable: [prPp?'gznd@9\ has been recast as [prPp?'gand?\- This socio-phonetic contrast across a few seconds of speech performance vividly symbolises the cultural and ideological divide at the heart of the policy dispute, and the two conflicting points of view: of propagandist (Macmillan) and critic (Bevan and, he intends, his audience).
7.
Discussion
Sociolinguistic theories of style have claimed to find unitary principles or ‘dimensions’ through which to model the variation they address. As a reaction against unitarianism, and hoping to open a door to a more complex and more closely contextualised reading of stylistic constructions of social meaning, I have previously suggested ‘the relational self’ as a two-headed theoretical formulation of what style potentially achieves (Coupland
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
237
2001b). By this I mean to imply that the work of style is to construct personal and social personas or identity images, for self and others, but also that constructed personas are then positioned and framed by speakers to achieve a wide range of relational orientations and effects. Diffuse as it may be, this line of theoretical interpretation again seems to me to provide the best fit to the data I have been considering in this chapter. Aneurin Bevan marshals semiotic resources to construct compellingly diverse social personas in and through the different interactional frames that are characteristic of his public speech-making. This is not to say that his political identity is in some way deeply compromised – it could hardly be more resolute. But his enduring commitment to what he often called ‘his own people’ on the working-class Welsh side of the class war of 1950s Britain required him to fight and win political battles, and this called for complex tactical operations, not least in that primary domain of political confrontation, public debate. On the basis of the evidence available in the audio-recordings, many of Bevan’s debating successes hinged on establishing ludicrous or foolish or inconsistent personas for his Tory opponents, and in building identities and stances for himself in relation to them and working people which listeners could adopt as their own. Style could in this case be defined as the discursive work that achieves these identities and relationships, where images of competence/ incompetence, coherence/ incoherence, integrity/ profligacy and truth/ falsity are some of the qualities at stake. Bevan’s constructions were potentially transformative, which is why they merit attention. Socio-phonetic features and their cultural resonances are what I have chosen to focus on here, although they are just some of the discursive resources through which Bevan achieves these constructions. The battle-lines of a class war are evident enough in Bevan’s speeches, and that is how he intended it to appear. But the discursive class war, in the data, is not a simple antagonism of ‘us and them’, Labour and Tories, workers and owners, even though these are words that pepper the language of the debates. Bevan does more than simply recycle the social and sociolinguistic mores of Tredegar mining communities as his own persona, although he can and does appeal to these values repeatedly. His stylistic constructions allow him to make capital out of working-class semiosis when it is rhetorically most useful to do so. Personal derogation of the Tory establishment through words like stupid and dull, and moral outrage through words like ashamed and besmirched, are stances where the vernacular symbolism of a Tredegar mining community voice adds social depth and
238
Nikolas Coupland
power. But Bevan was aware that power is multi-faceted and is located in different places. One key source of political authority was the House of Commons, and we should remember that Bevan believed wholeheartedly in constitutional rather than revolutionary routes to social change. It is not surprising, then, that he was prepared to adopt some of the semiotic trappings of parliamentary authority, despite the fact that this semiotic would potentially conflict with the Tredegar mining semiotic. This is the best explanation, I think, for Bevan’s sometimes perceived ‘champagne socialism’, at least as it is evident in his occasional and striking deployment of conservative RP features in his speech. His left-wing critics might have asked, ‘how can Bevan, the boy from the Tredegar colliery, collude with the enemy and talk posh?’, which is how pronouncing the word decency as ['dis?nsD?\ was probably heard. But it would be entirely consistent with Bevan’s politics to argue that the working classes should claim authority whenever they could, and not eschew the trappings of privilege. Fine wine and sharp suits, so why not the occasional syllablefinal [İԥ]? Conservative RP certainly defined the establishment in 1950s Britain, but it was also (one sort of) authority. When Bevan could claim that moral authority was on his side of the debate, why not occasionally cross into the voice of the Tory establishment and bring its weight to bear down upon itself? This was at least an alternative to voicing moral authority as resistance, style-marked as Tredegar voice. These resources, like others, needed to be carefully deployed in felicitous and politically useful contexts. Sometimes it was most useful to ‘leave traces of the artist’s brushstrokes’, to cue an audience to realise that this was parody not appropriation, a stylised, as-if voice rather than fully-owned voice. In those moments Bevan provides a nice case study of the discursive performance of authenticity, and of how tactics of self de-authentication as well as other de-authentication can work in the service of self-authentication at other levels (paradoxes I discuss in Coupland 2001). It is interesting to speculate on the significance of the Bevan data being about fifty years old, and how the data might or might not reflect ‘modern’ versus ‘late-modern’ tendencies (the puzzle I mentioned earlier). The contemporary trope is to claim that social class is recessive or less structurally dominating today (see Rampton 2003 for a critique), and if we agree, we might be tempted to see Bevan’s class wars discourse as an interesting cultural relic of high modernity. A contrary and I think more accurate line is that Bevan’s playful and not-so-playful styling of social class relations has a decidedly late-modern quality. In British political history his rhetoric was
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
239
in fact a powerful force acting to destabilise the established class order. As Dai Smith writes: Public speech, as interpreted and practised by Bevan, was an indictment of the suffocating wisdom of established superiority in its settled forms… Bevan understood that it was language alone which allowed his listeners to comprehend reality as something which could be fashioned in its plasticity not just endured in its materiality. (Smith 1993: 184–185)
Bevan’s rhetoric was a force towards a more polyvocal modernity, undermining the foundations of rigid class structures in 1950s Britain. We have seen how Bevan could bring vernacular authority to bear on political elitism. Putting the Tredegar mining community’s voice on-stage in the institutional genres and settings of British political debate in the 1950s was itself a move towards a more open and plastic social reality. But plasticity is above all the quality of discourse achieved through multiple voicing, which is able to destabilise the voice of privilege from within. The overtly dialogic frame in which most of Bevan’s voice-play happened was a radical departure from British political norms. We can invoke Bakhtin’s distinction between “authoritative discourse” and “internally persuasive discourse” to describe the distinction. Authoritative discourse is essentially monologic; it is, in Bakhtin’s expression, “the word of the fathers” and it “demands that we make it our own” (1981: 342). It is not unreasonable to call on-record, politically consolidated discourses of both left and right – ‘authoritative’ in this sense. The discourse of the political establishment was certainly “the word of the fathers”, and one which also happened to be a Tory voice in the late 1950s, giving it a an even more unmoving quality of patronymy, with RP as its socio-phonetic image. But Valleys voice was another “word of the fathers” – different fathers, of course, iconically hooked into a history of class struggle and deprivation in the coalmines of South Wales. One of Bevan’s sociolinguistic achievements was to bring these two authoritative discourses into conflict with each other, symbolically as well as rhetorically confronting establishment authenticities with vernacular authenticities. Establishment authority was thereby challenged as a universal doxa. For Bakhtin, internally persuasive discourse is relativised and developmental, and repositions authoritative discourse “in a new situation in order to expose its weak sides” (1981: 348; see also Tsitsipis 2004). The spontaneity of Bevan’s speaking (see note 4) encouraged listeners to work through arguments – with him but
240
Nikolas Coupland
for themselves – and to become critics of received Tory stances. It is in Bevan’s socio-phonetic stylisations that his ideological critique was most persuasive and where class strictures began to seem less inevitable. His styling was both locally diverting and ideologically destabilising.
Notes 1. I am grateful to Theo van Leeuwen, Justine Coupland, Ben Rampton, Peter Garrett, Adam Jaworski and the editor of this volume for very helpful comments on an earlier version. Deficiencies in the chapter are my own. I am also grateful to the BBC Archive in Llandaff, Cardiff and to Rachel Muntz for assistance in identifying and obtaining these sources. 2. Aneurin Bevan was recently voted top of a list of ‘100 Welsh Heroes’ (see http://www.100welshheroes.com/), although the results have proved controversial. A short, photo-illustrated biography of Aneurin Bevan is available at http://www.worldwidewales.tv/index2.php?mid=259. 3. Michael Foot’s is the definitive biography (Foot 1997a); see also Brome (1953), Cambell (1987), Morgan (1989), Smith (1993), and the collection of senior politicians and journalists who contributed to Geoffrey Goodman’s (1997b) edited volume on Bevan’s legacy. 4. “[The] quality of spontaneity and immediacy is the very kernel of effective speech. It induces in the audience a disposition to give themselves to the speaker because their surrender has not been obviously and carefully prepared beforehand” (Aneurin Bevan, quoted in Smith 1993: 183). 5. The broadcast sources I originally had access to are: Nye, BBC Radio 4, 21st June 1978; What Became of Nye, BBC Radio Wales, 6 parts, 7th November 1997 to 11th December 1997; and A Class Apart: Aneurin Bevan and the Labour Party, BBC television, 1998. All were presented by the political journalist Patrick Hannan. A further, unique source is: The Past Master, presenter Phil Carradice, BBC Radio Wales 25th May 2003. This programme was devoted to a then newly discovered complete recording of one of Aneurin Bevan’s speeches, with extensive extracts of the speech included in the broadcast. 6. Transcribed extracts, below, are segmented into intonational units (to which I have allocated ‘line’ numbers), usually but not inevitably bounded by pauses. A typical pattern in Bevan’s public speeches is a sequence of short units divided by short pauses, followed by more substantial pauses which may include audible audience response. Parts of some extracts show a less formal oratorical mode, with some longer intonation units and less predictable pause boundaries. I mark short, untimed pauses as (.) and Bevan’s occasional (stammering) blocks with /. Particularly heavy syllable emphasis is shown through underlining, but
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
241
this is of course only a rudimentary convention for representing the rhythmical properties of the speeches. I do not give a formal representation of intonation either, but I note some salient pitch movements and some paralinguistic characteristics, as necessary to the analysis, in comments enclosed by > within the line of transcript. I give representations of salient socio-phonetic variants in small font, above the line of transcript. 7. See Foot 1997a: 519ff, 1997b: 202ff, for a personal account of this speech and more details of its historical context. 8. “Bevan’s natural gifts were perhaps unpromising for someone who wanted to persuade from a platform or the benches of the House of Commons. A rather high voice, a stammer, and a substantial dropper of aitches too. But none of it mattered, I think, because these characteristics were for his audience a kind of guarantee of authenticity. That and his skill at making ideas accessible” (Patrick Hannan, What Became of Nye, Programme 5).
References Antaki, Charles and Sue Widdicombe (eds.) 1998 Identities in Talk. London: Sage. Auer, Peter (ed.) 1998 Code-Switching in Conversation. New York: Routledge. Auer, Peter and Aldo di Luzio (eds.) 1991 The Contextualization of Language. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Bakhtin, Mikhail M. 1981 The Dialogic Imagination, M. Holquist (ed.), C. Emerson and M. Holquist (trans.). Austin: University of Texas Press. 1986 Speech Genres and Other Late Essays, Caryl Emerson and Michael Holquist (ed.), Vern W. McGee (trans.). Austin: University of Texas Press. Bell, Allan 1984 Language style as audience design. Language in Society 13, 145–204. 1999 Styling the other to define the self: A study in New Zealand identity making. Journal of Sociolinguistics 3(4), 523–541. 2001 Back in style: Reworking audience design. In Eckert, Penelope and John R. Rickford (eds.), Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press, 139–169. Biber, Douglas and Edward Finegan (eds.) 1995 Sociolinguistic Perspectives on Register. New York: Oxford University Press.
242
Nikolas Coupland
Blair, Tony 1997 Foreword to Goodman (ed.), 11–13. Bourdieu, Pierre 1977 Outline of a Theory of Practice, Richard Nice (trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1991 Language and Symbolic Power [Ce que parler veut dire], John B. Thompson (ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Brome, Vincent 1953 Aneurin Bevan: A Biography. London: Longmans and Green. Bucholtz, Mary 1999 ‘You da man’: Narrating the racial other in the production of white masculinity. Journal of Sociolinguistics 3(4), 443–460. Campbell, John 1987 Nye Bevan and the Mirage of British Socialism. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson. Chambers, J. K. 2003 Sociolinguistic Theory: Linguistic Variation and its Social Significance, 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell. Coupland, Nikolas 1980 Style-shifting in a Cardiff work setting. Language in Society 9(1), 1– 12. 1984 Accommodation at work: Some phonological data and their implications. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 46, 49–70. 1985 ‘Hark, hark the lark’: Social motivations for phonological styleshifting. Language and Communication 5(3), 153–172. 1988 Dialect in Use: Sociolinguistic Variation in Cardiff English. Cardiff: University of Wales Press. 2001a Dialect stylisation in radio talk. Language in Society 30, 345–375. 2001b Language, situation and the relational self: Theorising dialect style in sociolinguistics. In: Eckert, Penelope and John Rickford (eds.), Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press, 185–210. 2001c Introduction. In Coupland, Nikolas, Srikant Sarangi, and Christopher N. Candlin (eds.), Sociolinguistics and Social Theory. London: Longman/Pearson Education, 1–26. 2003 Sociolinguistic authenticities. Journal of Sociolinguistics 7(3), 417– 431. 2007 Style: Language Variation and Identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cutler, Cecilia A. 1999 Yorkville Crossing: White teens, hip hop, and African American English. Journal of Sociolinguistics 3(4), 428–442.
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
243
Eckert, Penelope 2000 Linguistic Variation as Social Practice. Maldon, MA/Oxford: Blackwell Publishers. Eckert, Penelope and Norma Mendoza-Denton 2002 Language and identity. In Chambers J. K., Peter Trudgill, and Natalie Schilling-Estes (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change. Oxford: Blackwell, 475–499. Foot, Michael 1977 Aneurin Bevan 1897–1960, Brian Brivati (ed.). (Originally in two volumes, 1962 and 1973.) London: Victor Gollancz. Garrett, Peter, Nikolas Coupland and Angie Williams 1999 Evaluating dialect in discourse: Teachers’ and teenagers’ responses to young English speakers in Wales. Language in Society 28(3), 321–354. 2003 Researching Language Attitudes: Dialect, Ethnicity and Performance. Cardiff: University of Wales Press. Giles, Howard, Justine Coupland, and Nikolas Coupland (eds.) 1991 Contexts of Accommodation: Developments in Applied Sociolinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Giles, Howard and Peter Powesland 1975 Speech Style and Social Evaluation. London: Academic Press. Goffman, Erving 1974 Frame Analysis. New York, NY: Harper. 1981 Forms of Talk. Oxford: Blackwell. Hebdige, Dick 1979 Subculture: The Meaning of Style. London: Methuen. Goodman, Geoffrey 1997a The soul of socialism. In Goodman, Geoffrey (ed.), The State of the Nation: The Political Legacy of Aneurin Bevan. London: Victor Gallancz, 15–35. Goodman, Geoffrey (ed.) 1997b The State of the Nation: The Political Legacy of Aneurin Bevan. London: Victor Gallancz. Hasan, Ruqaia 1996 Ways of Saying: Ways of Meaning. London: Cassell. Irvine, Judith 2001 ‘Style’ as distinctiveness: The culture and ideology of linguistic differentiation. In Eckert, Penelope and John R. Rickford (eds.), Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press, 21–43. Jaffe, Alexandra 1999 Ideologies in Action. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
244
Nikolas Coupland
Johnstone, Barbara 1996 The Linguistic Individual: Self-Expression in Language and Linguistics. New York: Oxford University Press. Johnstone, Barbara and Judith M. Bean 1997 Self-expression and linguistic variation. Language in Society 26, 221–246. Labov, William 1972 Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. LePage, Robert and Andrée Tabouret-Keller 1985 Acts of Identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Morgan, Kenneth O. 1989 The Red Dragon and the Red Flag: The Cases of James Griffiths and Aneurin Bevan. Aberystwyth: The National Library of Wales. Rampton, Ben 1995 Crossing: Language and Ethnicity among Adolescents. London: Longman. 2000 Speech community. In Verschueren, J., J.-O. Ostman, J. Blommaert, and C. Bulcaen (eds.), Handbook of Pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 1–34. http://www.kcl.ac.uk/depsta/education/ULL/wpull.html 2001 Language crossing, crosstalk and cross-disciplinarity in sociolinguistics. In Coupland, N., C. Candlin, and S. Sarangi (eds.), Sociolinguistics and Social Theory. London: Longman, 261–296. 2003 Hegemony, social class and stylisation. Pragmatics 13(1), 49–84. Rampton, Ben (ed.) 1999 Styling the other. Thematic issue of Journal of Sociolinguistics 3(4). Rickford, John and Penelope Eckert (eds.) 2001 Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Schilling-Estes, Natalie 2004 Constructing ethnicity in interaction. Journal of Sociolinguistics 8, 163–195. Smith, David 1993 Aneurin Bevan and the World of South Wales. Cardiff: University of Wales Press. 1999 Wales: A Question for History. Bridgend: Seren. Tannen, Deborah (ed.) 1993 Framing in Discourse. New York: Oxford University Press.
Aneurin Bevan, class wars and the styling of political antagonism
245
Tsitsipis, Lukas 2004 On Bakhtin and sociolinguistics: Linguistic ideology and the tension between authoritative discourse and minority language communities. Journal of Sociolinguistics. Williams, Gwyn A. 1985 When was Wales? London: Black Raven Books. Williams, Raymond 2003 Who Speaks for Wales? Nation, Culture, Identity, D. Williams (ed.). Cardiff: University of Wales Press.
Chapter 9 Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays* Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain 1.
Introduction and theoretical background
In his article on the asymmetries of knowledge in conversational interactions, Drew (1991) points out that “the kinds of ways in which asymmetries are manifest need to be more carefully specified in the details of talk” (1991: 22). These asymmetries concern the ways in which speakers claim authority to knowledge through the use of interactional and linguistic resources. These resources are, for example, mitigations by a patient in doctor-patient-interaction with the use of a medical term in order to “treat such terminology as normatively ‘belonging’ to the other” (1991: 39), in this case the doctor. Thus, Drew shows that “an asymmetry of knowledge is not equivalent to ‘not knowing’”(1991: 39). Furthermore, Drew argues for a link between identity and these knowledge asymmetries: Entitlements to knowledge are attached to, or belong to, categories – and not to persons.... This has the consequence that a speaker may possess some knowledge, but nevertheless have an asymmetrical position with respect to that knowledge. The connection between categories or speaker identities and normative entitlements to knowledge begins to separate the notion of asymmetry from actual states of knowledge and ignorance. The interactional consequences of this shows up in cases where speakers use some piece of knowledge, but simultaneously display their asymmetrical position of non-entitlement to – or of not being an authoritative source of – that knowledge. (1991: 37–38)
Identity, as conceived by Drew above, as well as by Antaki and Widdicombe (1998), is something that is co-constructed by participants in an interaction. In our interest in identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays, we concern ourselves with emergent identities within the context of Eastern and Western Germany1 after the fall of the wall in 1989
248
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
and German unification in 1990.2 We distinguish between three types of identity: personal, social, and discursive. Personal identity refers to the particular life history of each of our interactants, which results in a biographical accomplishment of an individual person (cf. Ricker 2000: 9). Part of this history is the accumulation of life experience, which then becomes first-hand knowledge. With it comes a psychological awareness of one’s own as well as others’ experiences, and this awareness is part of the contextual knowledge participants bring to an interaction, whether or not such knowledge is made relevant in the interaction. In the analysis for this paper, this knowledge is tied to social categories such as those of Eastern German and Western German. While identity is discursively constructed, the cultural context in which the conversational participants live makes them aware of such social categories, and this awareness allows them to use these categories as “construction materials” for social identities for themselves and others, by making such information relevant in the interaction (cf. McKinlay and Dunnett 1998: 46). In this sense, “Eastern German” and “Western German” as social identities are not pre-analytic concepts which determine the outcome of the interaction, but rather concepts which evolve in the interaction and present themselves through linguistic variability. Finally, discursive identity is that position which interactants momentarily take up at a particular point in the interaction. For example, a person may take up the position of the “speaker”, while another is the “recipient”. The interactants can then construct themselves and/or each other as the “knowing recipient” or the “non-knowing recipient.” The process by which interactants make these identities relevant is called positioning (Harré and van Langenhove 1991; van Langenhove and Harré 1993; Wolf 1999), which we define here as the ways in which interactants index their own and others’ relationships to roles and social categories by means of interactive resources. Positioning can be seen as “a dynamic alternative to the more static concept of role” (Harré and van Langenhove 1991: 393), and far from being permanent category memberships, these positionings are always highly context-dependent and often quite temporary. In our analysis, these have to do with respective identity assignment in the discursive co-construction of a particular personal story in which cultural knowlege plays a role. Positioning aids in “mak[ing] a person’s actions intelligible and relatively determinate as social acts” (van Langenhove and Harré 1993: 83) and is a natural and unavoidable process of conversation.
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
249
By using this approach, this paper tries to make a contribution to the understanding of the link between identity, positioning and knowledge displays. While this research may be of interest to linguists investigating the relationship between epistemic phenomena and grammar in the tradition of Chafe and Nichols (1986), it also addresses questions which interactional researchers are increasingly interested in. Such questions are: Since knowledge may be acquired on the basis of experience (first-hand) or through hearsay (second-hand), how do interactants deal with these differences? Contrary to institutional settings where authority may be based on an understanding of roles (e.g. doctor vs. client), how is authority through knowledge displays negotiated in everyday conversation? How do epistemic stances contribute to the construction of social identities? Finally, is there a link between first- and second-hand knowledge and communicative styles in particular local contexts? Heritage and Raymond (2005: 16) address the connection between knowledge and style as “organized practices of speaking” when claiming that “the distribution of rights and responsibilities regarding what participants can accountably know, how they know it, whether they have rights to describe it, and in what terms is directly implicated in organized practices of speaking.” In their analysis of first and second assessments in everyday conversations, they describe the linguistic features that index “relative epistemic rights to evaluate states of affairs” (ibid.) and discuss the use of these features in relation to the management of face and identity in form of social categories such as “dog owner” and “grandmother”. In this discussion, they point out that this relationship is rather ambivalent at times in that, e.g. “[s]peakers may assert rights that are (or could be) contested; in some cases they may defer to a recipient with putatively subordinate rights” (Heritage and Raymond 2005: 31). In searching for reasons why speakers do that, we suggest to consider how personal knowledge links with social stereotyping and the social discourses about identities at large such as the discourses about Eastern and Western Germany after 1989. The conversations analyzed for this paper all involve people who have migrated from Western to Eastern Germany after German unification in 1990. We refer to these individuals as “migrants”3 due to the fact that they moved from one geographic area to another, each with a distinct recent cultural and political past. One outcome of these distinct pasts are the different experiences of people living in the East and in the West, resulting on the individual level in different biographies, i.e. different personal identities. Whether or not these differences are made salient in the interaction has
250
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
an effect on the construction of different German social identities and on the construction of the identity of Germany as a whole. In our analysis, we focus on the varying ways in which the migrants construct these different kinds of culturally specific knowledge, and in particular, how they use specific linguistic resources to index one social group or the other in the context of this knowledge management. While we are interested in the ways in which speakers in our data use linguistic resources to “upgrade” and “downgrade” their claims to authority, building on Heritage and Raymond’s (2005) analysis of knowledge displays in first and second assessments, we are further interested in the use of these resources within a context in which claims to knowledge relate to ways in which contested or stigmatized Eastern and Western social identities are constructed. Communicative style plays a role to the extent that it can be considered an outcome of the interaction, in which local and social contexts are as important as who is participating in the interaction. Our understanding of style is that of a “strategic, proactive use of available linguistic resources to construct social meaning” (Milroy and Gordon 2003: 199). Kamio (1995, 1997) in his theory of the territory of information seems to link style to the management of knowledge, in particular the display of authority through the use of particular forms. Though Kamio’s theory is somewhat deterministic in that linguistic forms are linked to social meaning a-priori, we can also take his theory to inform our understanding about why certain forms are possibly used in particular local contexts and what such use implies in regard to the social identities of speakers. This theory distinguishes two basic types of sentential forms: the direct form and the non-direct form. The direct form consists of bare forms such as “That lady is your mother,” while non-direct forms contain “hedging” elements such as “I think”, “probably”, or rhetorical questions. These forms correspond, in turn, to the notion of locating the information inside or outside a particular territory, which is analogous to a physical space that can be claimed by a person or animal. The form of the utterance that is used indicates the interactant’s relationship to the information, as well as that of the other conversational participants; i.e., when a given piece of information falls into the speaker’s territory but not the hearer’s, the speaker will tend to use the direct form; when a given piece of information falls into the hearer’s territory but not the speaker’s, the speaker will tend to use an indirect form (Kamio 1997: 6). We do not, however, claim that there is a particular Eastern German or Western German style. Rather, as these social categories emerge as topic or through vocabulary, speakers use stylistic means such as hedging
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
251
to create epistemic or affective stances to position themselves in relation to these categories. This notion comprises style shifts where an individual speaker “creatively uses language resources often from beyond the immediate speech community, such as distant dialects, or stretches those resources in novel directions,” which Bell (2001: 147) refers to as “referee design”. In that sense, a Western German may present Eastern German knowledge, including Eastern German-specific vocabulary. According to Kamio, however, in using a direct form when presenting this knowledge, the speaker would construct an Eastern German identity for himself. In contrast, by presenting Eastern German knowledge using a non-direct form, the speaker would construct a non-Eastern German identity for himself. Since Eastern German and Western German are complementary categories (in that indexing one category necessarily means simultaneously invoking the other category; Wolf 1999: 15), positioning as a non-Eastern German may evoke a Western German identity. In other words, “linguistic choices are interpreted as part of a larger set of strategies and practices whereby speakers not only associate themselves with particular social groups and index distinctiveness from others, but also construct by means of these practices social categories such as ‘whiteness,’ ‘masculinity,’ ‘jock,’ ‘burnout,’ and so forth” (Milroy and Gordon 2003: 199–200), or, we may add, ‘Eastern German’ and ‘Western German’.
2.
Data and methodology
The data on which this paper is based was collected by the two authors over three summers between 2000 and 2003 in Eastern Germany. The data are audiotaped and videotaped, mostly non-directed conversations which took place during cooking, while playing with children, while sitting together in the migrants’ homes, or during meals. The participants in these conversations are one or more migrants from Western to Eastern Germany and sometimes their partners and children, along with one or both of the researchers, who are also the authors of this paper. All of the migrants in this study moved from Western to Eastern Germany for personal or professional reasons after the unification of East and West Germany in 1990 and the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989.4 Specific circumstances distinguish the migration from Western to Eastern Germany from other migrant situations. Eastern Germany and Western Germany are not politically neutral grounds, but places that each carry the
252
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
associations of the formerly opposed political systems of the West vs. the East. These associations such as social evaluations, emotions and attitudes are tied not only to the respective places but also to the people, and to the social categories of Eastern German and Western German (cf. Wolf 1999: 15). Since the conversations take place in Eastern Germany, where the pejorative picture of the Western German as the “other” is strongly present, the Western German migrants may feel compelled to address this pejorative image. While they can do this through the ways in which they use linguistic resources to position themselves within the Eastern and Western German categories, the direction of the move itself may also be indicative of a favourable view on Eastern Germany.5 Simply by moving from Western to Eastern Germany during the time when important parts of the former East German infrastructure were still intact, the migrants already position themselves as favourable towards Eastern Germany. As in any situation of migration, however, they are, based on their personal histories, outsiders. As such, they may have an interest in integrating as soon as possible, i.e. in positioning themselves as Eastern Germans. Positioning, as one way to display integration discursively, is made more difficult through the sociopolitical contrast between Eastern German and Western German and the baggage these categories carry. The ways in which knowledge about Eastern Germany is presented becomes intrinsically linked to the evaluation of Eastern and Western German as social categories. The migrants in this study have acquired their knowledge of former East German and present-day Eastern German life through a combination of first-hand and second-hand experience. Those varying kinds of experience are relevant to whether they perceive a piece of information as falling inside or outside of their territories, and therefore with what degree of authority they speak about that information. As Sacks (1995: 468) points out: People in this world in any event are built to be the custodians of just about only their own experiences. And a lot of things that people are built to be the custodians of, i.e. that they can be made to care about keeping, taking care of, defending, and the like, are more or less whatever it is that the world has them conceive of as ‘their own’. Their experiences are but one class of such things.
Among the questions that we address involve how these migrants relate their knowledge, how this knowledge display is part of identity construction, and to what extent different styles can be said to be emerging through the use of different features that are instrinsically tied to particular social groups.
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
253
For this paper, we collected excerpts from the data in which our migrants present their knowledge on Eastern and Western Germany. From these excerpts, we selected segments to be analyzed and presented in this paper. Our analysis is based on a combination of qualitative tools developed within interactional sociolinguistics, conversation analysis and the ethnography of speaking. While we discuss individual utterances within their conversational context and consider the sequential nature of each interaction, ethnographic information in form of the cultural and historical backgrounds of and the relationship between Eastern and Western Germany also become part of the analysis. This background includes the relationship between language and culture, e.g. through the ways in which lexical items carry certain social connotations.
3.
Analysis
The focus in the segments analyzed below is always on the presentation of knowledge about Eastern Germany, which necessitates choices for positioning, links the migrants’ selection of linguistic forms to social identities, and makes these links contestable. The segments are organized into different analytic sections in order to discuss different aspects of knowledge presentation. In section 3.1, we discuss the use of Eastern German-specific vocabulary as part of knowledge management. Our focus in section 3.2 is the variable use of direct and non-direct forms by the same speaker in one and the same interaction and its consequences for self- and other-positioning. Finally, in section 3.3, we examine the ways in which migrants’ presentation of knowledge is challenged and how the migrants strategically position themselves to address these challenges. 3.1. Variability and positioning involving Eastern German-specific vocabulary Presenting knowledge about Eastern Germany in an interaction often involves the use of lexical items which carry the connotations of and knowledge about Eastern German culture and society, and which we call Eastern German-specific vocabulary. Such vocabulary in itself can be considered a stylistic feature which calls upon Eastern German as a social category. For a migrant from Western Germany, using such vocabulary is no easy task
254
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
because it implies knowledge about Eastern Germany and, perhaps, positioning as an Eastern German. In other words, its use implies knowledge that does not fall into the territory of information of a Western German who has lived all his life in Western Germany, especially if this vocabulary is tied to former East Germany rather than to present-day Eastern Germany. The use of Eastern German-specific vocabulary occurs in several segments discussed in this paper, but it takes a central role in the two segments discussed in this section. The following segment is from a breakfast table conversation which took place in a town in Eastern Germany in 2003. The interactants are the Western German migrants, Diana and Fred5, with their pre-school and primary school aged daughter and son who are present but do not talk during this excerpt, and the two researchers GL and JD. They migrated to Eastern Germany after Fred, who is in his fourties, was hired by a local government ministry. Segment (1)7 1
GL:
geht die tochter in den kindergarten eigentlich? ‘is your daughter in preschool?’
2
Fred:
ja ‘yes’
3
Diana: ja ‘yes’
4
(2.0)
5
GL:
hier in (.) wachau? ‘here in wachau?’
6
Fred:
ja ‘yes’
7
(1.0)
8
Fred:
9
GL:
in den volkssolidaritätskindergarten ‘at the volkssolidarität preschool’ ((laughs, looks up from the table at GL))
10 Fred:
ja? heißt der noch so ja? ‘yeah? is it still called that yeah?’ ja- na ja- der heißt kinderland ‘yeah well it's called child-land’
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
255
[(0.1) aber wird von= [(0.1) but is run’= [(du deswegen ) [(‘hey therefore’ ) ((separate conversation involving JD))
11 Diana:
12 Fred:
=der volkssolidarität betrieben =‘by the volkssolidarität organization’
13 GL:
ach so? ‘oh?’
14 Fred:
aber ist nicht schlecht. die haben so ne montessori gruppe ‘but it's not bad. they have a montessori group’
15
und auch ne nette kinder [gärtnerin‘and also a really nice [preschool teacher’[ [hm=hm? [‘hm=hm?’
16 GL:
After GL’s initial question in line 1 about the daughter’s preschool and a follow-up question in line 5, Fred in line 8 volunteers more information about the preschool, namely specifying it as a Volkssolidarität preschool. Volkssolidarität is an Eastern German-specific term referring to a charitable organization that used to do work in former East Germany, where it still exists today, and has never done work in the West. When he uses this term, Fred starts to laugh, indicating that he finds this amusing, and looks up from the breakfast table to make eye contact with GL.8 By doing this without giving any additional information about the term, he positions GL as the knowing recipient, and moreover, indicates through the use of a direct form that knowledge about the Volkssolidarität organization falls within his territory of information. Through these linguistic and interactional resources, he calls upon their shared knowledge about the significance of the term as something to do with Eastern Germany, though also checking through eye-contact whether GL has knowledge about that term and/or anticipating her reaction to his use of the term. In using a direct form rather than a non-direct form, he presents this knowledge as first-hand knowledge, as part of his current experience in Eastern Germany rather than tied to former East Germany. In fact, GL in her question in line 9 also ties this term to present-day Eastern Germany in formulating a question with the
256
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
underlying assumption that this term is still used today. This assumption may arise from the fact that Fred uses the direct form suggesting that this is first-hand knowledge from his experience living in present-day Eastern Germany. It is not clear whether Fred actually knows that the term originated in former East Germany, but through his laughter he indicates that he finds the use of the term amusing for some reason or another, i.e. takes an affective stance toward the term. In looking at GL in line 8, he positions her as somebody who may be able to share in the amusement value surrounding the term. Through her response in line 9, GL indicates that she is familiar with the term, i.e. shares that knowledge. She does not, however, join in the laughter with him, but seems to want to locate the reason for the amusement in asking whether this term is really still used in present-day Eastern Germany. The ‘noch’ (still) is used to index the relationship between the present (Eastern Germany) and the past (East Germany). Similarly to the use of ‘früher’ (in the past) in the first segment, the reference is given through GL’s personal life history as a former East German who may be interested in the changes between East Germany and present-day Eastern Germany. In other words, GL assumes that Fred’s laughter may result from the fact that the term is still used today as it was in former East Germany. While GL positions Fred as somebody who has more knowledge than herself about Eastern Germany, she is also building on shared knowledge about East Germany, i.e. she locates the information about the Volkssolidarität organization within both her territory of information and his. In Fred’s answer starting in line 10, the several restarts and perturbation makers are indicative of a dispreferred response (i.e. a ‘no’-answer rather than a ‘yes’-answer presupposed by GL’s question). Since the name of the kindergarden, as it turns out, is Kinderland, Fred adds an explanation why it is called Volkssolidaritätskindergarten (presumably as an alternate name): it is run by the Volkssolidarität organization. Since this is an Eastern German organization and since he therefore could possibly be seen as mocking things East German, the positive information about the preschool he gives in lines 14 and 15 can be seen as asserting his position as being positive about Eastern German things (at least as far as the kindergarten is concerned). This could indicate that he may have understood GL’s reaction as doubtful about his positive attitude towards Eastern Germany by her not joining in a joke. By displaying a positive attitude through current knowledge about something with ties to East Germany, Fred asserts his position as integrating into his place of migration in a positive way. While Fred did
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
257
not succeed in displaying this positive attitude through the use of vocabulary, the Western German migrant in the following segment does. Segment (2) is similar to the previous segment in that it involves the use of Eastern German-specific vocabulary, a term that calls upon Eastern German as a category and by which the Western German migrant, in using this term, displays knowledge about former East Germany. What is different from segment (1), however, is how the interactants in the following segment (2) treat this use of such vocabulary, namely the term Westbesuch. This term was typically used by East Germans to refer to people from West Germany who visited relatives or friends in pre-unification East Germany. It had a pejorative meaning, especially when used by East German state officials, with a connotation of “making allies with the enemy”. The use of the term was commonly associated with an East German person, and would fall into the territory of information of someone with an East German identity. In a Bakhtinian sense, the person using it speaks with an East German “voice” (Bakhtin 1981). If a migrant from Western Germany uses this term, as in the segment below, it becomes marked and has multiple implications, as the discussion following the segment will show. The participants in segment (2) are the two migrants, Silke and Bernd, along with their 4-yearold daughter Sabine, as well as the researchers GL and JD. Silke and Bernd, both in their late thirties, came to Eastern Germany shortly after unification to start their own businesses. Segment (2) 1
JD:
2
so sabine meinte ihr hattet (.) gaanz lange beSU:CH ‘so sabine said that you guys had (.) visitors for a loong time’ (2.0)
3
Silke: wann (.) GEStern ‘when (.) YESterday’
4
JD:
5 6
weiß ich nicht ‘I don't know’ (1.0)
Bernd: von VORgestern abend bis gestern v- vormittag ‘from the day before YESterday until yesterday m- morning’
258 7
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain Silke: ( ) und das- wenn dann auch (.) äh vieler besu:ch ist (.) ( ) ‘and therefore- if then (.) uh many of our visitors are’(.)
LA:Nge auch da ‘here for a LONG time such’
8
[also so (.) WESTbesuch [I mean like (.) WESTbe-
GL:
[mhm [‘mhm’
10 JD:
[mhm [‘mhm’
9
11 GL:
[((laughter))
12 JD:
[((laughter))
13
(1.0)
14 JD:
((laughter)) WESTbesuch
15
((laughter))
In this segment, the researcher JD initiates a topic about visitors at Silke and Bernd’s home, since their daughter had mentioned them. Silke, in her turn in lines 7 and 8, confirms their daughter’s description that the visitors stayed for a long time, and adds that their visitors are usually from Western Germany, using the term WESTbesuch. As the term displays an East German social identity, in using this term, Silke evokes a voice she is not typically associated with. By using the discourse marker also (I mean),9 she marks this term as a repair10 and as a reformulation of her description of the visitors. In doing so, she compares her own experience of receiving visitors in Eastern Germany with receiving Westbesuch in former East Germany. As part of the general nature of this comparison, Silke associates the Eastern and Western German territories with the former states of East and West Germany. While the term was outdated at the time of interaction because East and West Germany no longer existed as states, Silke, in using the term, recalls the East German experience through the territorial correspondence. In alluding to an East German identity, Silke displays her East German knowledge and “transports” that knowledge into the present day, invoking the connotations this term used to have, such as visitors who came from the wealthier part of Germany with West German goods (or hand-me-
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
259
downs), possibly staying for a longer time after going through the trouble of registering as visitors in East Germany and of paying a fee to be able to visit. These associations are the reason why Silke’s use of the term triggers laughter, first by the two researchers and later, in line 15, by all interactants. Silke’s use of the term can thus be seen as a funny comment about her visitors. Secondly, and more importantly for our discussion, Silke’s use of the term is a comment about herself as a Western German who would have been the person visiting rather than receiving the visit in preunification East Germany. In using the term, she not only displays her East German knowledge but is able to shift identities from a Western German to an Eastern German. Nonetheless, because of her personal identity as a migrant, this shift is recognized by the present parties through the laughter. Silke gets credit for her knowledge about the East German term, drawing on the shared knowledge among all interactants, and the way in which her use of the term is a twist on her own identity. While the interactants seem to recognize this twist through the laughter, it is GL and JD who position Silke as a Western German by initiating the laughter, by which they express that Silke is somebody for whom the use of the term ‘Westbesuch’ is unusual. 3.2. The contrast between direct and non-direct forms in the presentation of knowledge There are also subtler ways than Eastern German-specific vocabulary by which the social categories Eastern and Western German emerge in the interaction, and in the course of which positioning plays a role. One of these ways is the focus of discussion in this section, where the speaker constructs a migrant identity for herself through the use of the direct and nondirect forms and simultaneously constructs the interlocutor’s identity. The first of two segments in this section, segment (3), is an excerpt from an interaction between the migrant Nina, who is in her mid-thirties and came to Eastern Germany in 1995 to pursue a doctoral degree, and the researcher GL. The conversation took place over dinner in a restaurant, and the main topic is the differences between Eastern German products before unification and at the time of conversation.
260
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
Segment (3) 1
Nina:
[(2.0) angeboten [(2.0) ‘packaging’
2 3
bestimmte produkte werden wieder mit der originalverpackung ‘certain products are being offered again in their original’
GL:
4
[m=hm: [‘m=hm:’ (2.0)
5
GL:
zum beispiel? ‘for example?’
6
Nina:
die schlager süßtafel 11 ‘the schlager süßtafel’
7
GL:
ach so? ((laughter)) ‘oh really?’
8
Nina:
ich weiß nich ob wir da neulich schon drüber gesprochen ‘I don't know if we already talked about that last time?’
9
hatten? aber das is so KÖSTlich ‘but it's so GREAT’
10 GL:
hmm doch [schon ‘hmm yeah [I think we did’
11 Nina:
[die schmeckt ja nicht mehr sooo [‘it doesn't taste aaaas good as it used to’ [(1.0) sagn (1.0) die einheimischen [(1.0) ‘as (1.0) the locals say’
12 13 GL:
[hm:=m [‘hm:=m’
14
(3.0)
15 Nina:
nja: die ZETTi knusperflocken (1.0) die sehen wohl nich mehr ‘well: the ZETTi knusperflocken (1.0)I guess they don't’
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
261
16
so aus wie früher (1.0) die sin nämlich jetz in so ner ‘look like they used to (1.0) now they're wrapped’
17
hochkants [(2.0) folie‘in a horizontal [(2.0) foil-
18 GL:
[ach soo [‘ohh
[verpackung [packaging’ [aha [I see’
Nina’s formulation at the beginning of Originalpackung (‘original packaging’) contains an implicit comparison between the state of affairs in former East Germany with that of current-day Eastern Germany, through which the categories of East German and West German are made relevant. Since Nina is using the direct form in this comparison, she claims to have knowledge about current-day Eastern Germany just as she does about former East Germany, without indicating that any of this is second-hand knowledge. Even though GL does not challenge her directly, she asks Nina in line 5 for the name, possibly in anticipation of recognizing the East German product herself, but may be perceived as probing into Nina’s more specific knowledge about things East German, which she presents again in a direct form in line 6. By using these direct forms, Nina locates this piece of knowledge within her territory of information and not GL’s. This has the effect of evoking GL’s own migrant status as someone not currently living in Eastern Germany, and positioning GL as someone for whom this knowledge is new information. The presentation of knowledge at the beginning of the segment and the positioning of both herself and GL contrast with the ways in which Nina formulates similar knowledge about past and present Eastern German products in lines 11 and 12. In line 11, she again uses the direct form when giving comparative information about such products. In line 12, however, she locates that information outside of her territory by using sagn die einheimischen (‘as...the locals say’), which identifies it as hearsay by making the form indirect and as outside her voice (in a Bakhtinian sense). Even though the term ‘locals’ could potentially refer to any group of people in the town or region in which Nina lives, in this context it indexes Eastern Germans as a category bound by the conversational context in which it appears, since the talk has been about Eastern German products. In talking about the locals in third-person reference, Nina positions herself as well as GL outside this group, i.e. she defines ‘locals’ as those that are different
262
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
from both herself and GL. The difference between herself and this group is that she has just recently moved to this area, which allows her migrant status to appear. The difference she may perceive between GL and this group is that GL is not ‘local’ anymore, since she does not now live in Eastern Germany. In line with positioning herself as an outsider to the ‘locals’, i.e. the Eastern Germans, Nina now uses a non-direct form in line 15, where she employs the hedging particle wohl (Helbig 1988; see also Brausse 1992) when comparing former East Germany with current-day Eastern Germany. Nina makes this comparison by using früher (translated here as ‘as they used to’, literally ‘earlier’), a temporal deictic commonly used to index preunification East Germany in conversations about Eastern Germany (cf. Liebscher 1997; Auer and Kern 2001). Through the hedge which marks the knowledge as hearsay, she locates this knowledge as outside her territory of information by reporting on this knowledge as second-hand. The use of this hedge, however, does not necessarily locate this information outside GL’s territory of information, as was the case with the demonstrative pronoun die (‘they’). The information immediately following in line 16 ‘now they are wrapped...’, is presented in a direct form and therefore as new information to GL, without marking this information as shared knowledge. GL further confirms that this was new information through her backchannel cues of ach soo ‘oh’ and aha ‘I see’ in line 17. Interestingly, however, Nina, in this comparison between former East Germany and the current state of affairs in lines 15 through 17, only reports about present-day Eastern Germany but not on what used to be in East Germany, thus positioning GL as knowledgeable about pre-unification East Germany. For her own positioning, however, the hedge wohl is a strong indicator that she shares this knowledge neither with GL nor with the ‘locals’. In marking this past knowledge as outside her territory of information, i.e. positioning herself as a Western German, and marking current knowledge as inside this territory, her identity as a migrant (rather than e.g. a Western German nor an Eastern German) emerges. The temporal deictic früher plays an important part in this identity construction, as this time indexed through früher for her is linked to Western Germany rather than Eastern Germany (see also discussion of segment 4 below). The following segment, part of the same conversation as segment (3), provides evidence of how Nina presents herself as outside of the social category of Eastern German by means of pronoun use but without using a hedging device. This self-presentation coincides with her changing percep-
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
263
tion of GL as an outsider to former East Germany. The conversation in this segment is still about East German products, here specifically about wine. Segment (4) 1
Nina: is ja letztendlich egal (.) da wird eh alles zusammengekippt ‘in the end it doesn't matter (.) it all gets mixed together anyway’
2
GL:
3 4
hmm=m ‘hmm=m’ (3.0)
Nina: un die hatten selbst in ddr zeiten schon auch französischen ‘and they even had French wine back in East Germany’
5
(1.0)
hmm=m ‘hmm=m’
6
GL:
7
Nina: also gegen [(..) TEUre devisen (..) einge [kauft ‘it was [(..) bought with (..) exPEN [sive hard currency’
8
GL:
9 10
[doch [‘yeah you're right (1.0)
GL:
11
un der MEIßner wein wurde dagegen (.) verKAUFT ‘and the wine from MEIßen12 was SOLD (.) going in the other direction’ (2.0)
12
Nina: als bückware ‘under the table’
13
GL:
14 15
[hja:. [yeah’
nja ‘yeah’ (1.0)
Nina: saale unstrut geNAUso ‘just like saale unstrut’
264
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
16
GL:
hm (.) wo? hm (.) where?’
17
Nina: saale unstru:t ‘saale unstru:t
[(..) aber (..) der= [(..) but (..) the=’
18
GL:
[aha (.) so genau (.) [‘I see (.) right okay’ (.)
hm:=m hm:=m’ 19
Nina: =der [(...) weiß nich wie hieß des denn =‘the [(...) I don't know what did that’
20
GL:
[hm:=m. [‘hm:=m.’
19a Nina: FRÜher= ‘used to be called back THEN’= 21
Nina: =JETZT (.) sagt man ja sachsen anhaltinische =’NOW (.) you call it Saxony-Anhaltinish’
After a general comment in line 1 about the mixing of wines for sale and a relatively long pause, Nina in line 4 volunteers information on the subject of wine in former East Germany. Here, she uses the direct form but positions herself outside the group of people indexed by die (‘they’) through the use of the third person pronoun. Through the conversational context, and especially due to the use of the expression in DDR Zeiten (‘back in East Germany’ or ‘during East German times’), the referent of die can be located as the people in East Germany at the time. It makes sense, therefore, that Nina positions herself outside of this group. Interestingly, however, she presents knowledge about this group as direct knowledge, i.e. locating this information within her territory. This is noteworthy because, in doing so, she also positions GL as outside this group and as the non-knowing recipient, even though she knows about GL’s personal identity as a former East German. This construction of GL as an outsider just like her may be the reason why she may now, in contrast to segment (3), present information about East Germany as the knowledgeable author, using direct forms, rather than presenting the information as second-hand. In perceiving GL as an outsider rather than a former East German, there is no reason for Nina to engage in any additional effort to hedge any objection to her authority on
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
265
East German knowledge. In fact, GLs doch (‘yes’) in line 8 confirms the information Nina gave in line 4 as correct. In the turns that follow, Nina and GL co-construct knowledge about former East Germany. In contrast to Nina’s positioning both of them as outsiders using die, GL, in line 10, resorts to the passive through wurde verkauft (‘was sold’), through which she avoids both her own and Nina’s positioning in relation to a particular German social identity, as she would have using a pronoun. In line 12, Nina presents a further piece of information about East Germany through the use of the term Bückware (‘products sold under the table’) which strongly evokes the East German cultural context. Nina’s use of früher (‘back then’), as in segment (3), indexes former East Germany, and again she positions herself as an Eastern German, tracking her past in a linear fashion from current times to pre-unification times in the same place, despite the fact that she used to live in a different place before unification. Through this direct line, and by asking the question in which früher is used in a direct rather than non-direct form, Nina does not mark the shift that has occurred in her personal identity from Western to Eastern German. 3.3. Challenging migrants’ presentation of knowledge in narrated knowledge displays The use of Eastern German-specific terms and the references to Eastern German products discussed in the previous two sections are one way of displaying knowledge about Eastern Germany. The segments in this section deal with narrated knowledge display of Eastern German cultural life. As already suggested in the discussion of earlier segments, this knowledge can be challenged or disputed by other conversational participants. This is what Harré and van Langenhove (1991: 396) call “second order positioning”, which is a re-positioning after a challenge to the “first-order positioning”, i.e. the initial positioning. Through such second order positionings interlocutors may call into question migrants’ rights to their expressed knowledge, and through their very self-positionings. These challenges have repercussions for the speaker’s territory of information, since, as we have previously stated, information is considered less close to an interactant if he or she is deemed not to have an adequate basis for asserting it (Kamio 1997: 17–20). In segment (5) below, we analyze a stretch of conversation where one of the other participants in the conversation challenges a piece of
266
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
knowledge about East Germany as introduced by a migrant. In segment (6), we analyze a similar stretch of conversation in which the migrant reacts as though his knowledge is being challenged; however, the actual challenge is not to the validity of the knowledge expressed, but to the migrant’s right to claim that information as falling within his territory. Segment (5) is a part of a conversation between the migrant Erika, who is in her late thirties and came to to Eastern Germany in 1993 to take a staff position at a university, her Eastern German partner Lars, whom she met after her migration, and the two researchers, JD and GL. Directly preceding the segment shown here, they have been discussing possible reasons why the Saxon dialect is so unpopular, and Lars has presented the former East German politician Walter Ulbricht’s Saxon origin as part of the reason for this unpopularity, which stretched back to pre-unification East Germany. Segment (5) un das is ‘and that's
[wahrscheinlich MIT?= [probably PART?=’
1
Lars:
2
JD:
3
Lars:
=KÖNNT ich mir vorstellen als GRUND? ja? =‘I COULD imagine part of the REason? yeah?’
4
JD:
ja ‘of
5
GL:
6
JD:
wo kam der eigentlich her? ‘where was he from?’
7
Lars:
ach des ‘oh I
8
Erika:
9
JD:
10 ??:
[wo KAM der? [‘where did he COME?’
[klar. [course.’ [mh=m. [‘mh=m.’
[weiß ich nich. [don't know.’ [aus LEIPzig. [‘from LEIPzig.’
aus LEIPZIG? ‘from LEIPZIG?
[direkt LEIPZIG? NEE. [directly from LEIPZIG? no WAY.’ [ja ja. [‘oh yeah.’
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
267
11 Lars:
jaa. ich glaube das is (.) des war n leipziger gewesen. ‘yeeah. I think he's (.) he was a Leipziger.’
12 JD:
WAAS? ‘WHAAT?’
13 Lars:
ja: ich glaube ja:. ‘yeah: I think so:.’
14 JD:
der sprach aber so KOmisch? ‘but he talked so FUNny?’
15 Lars:
jaa. der hat so die (.) die hohe? so ne ganz ‘yeeah. he had that (.) that high? such a really’
16
hohe stim [me hatte der gehabt? so das KLANG so=n ‘high voi [ce was what he had? and it SOUNDED like’ [hmm. [‘hmm.’
17 GL: 18 Lars:
[bisschen komisch. so jo=o=a? [‘a little bit weird. so 'jo=o=a?' ((imitating))
19 JD:
[ach soo. [‘ohhh.’
20 JD:
((laughter)) aber der KAM aus leipzig? also das HÄTT ich (laughter)) ‘but he was FROM leipzig? I mean I WOULDN'T have’
21
nich gedacht. [ich dachte irgendwie VOGTland ‘thought so. [I thought maybe the VOGT region’
22 Lars:
[(doch.) [(‘yeah he was’.)
23 JD:
[oder so? [‘or something like that?’
24 Lars:
[nein des is n LEIPziger glaub ich. [‘no he was a LEIPziger I think.’
25 Erika: ich GLAUbe jaa. ‘I THINK soo.’
268
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
26 Lars:
m=hm. ‘m=hm.’
27 JD:
hmm. ‘hmm.’
28 Erika: =wurde mal gesacht. =‘or so I've been told.’ 29 GL:
hmm. ‘hmm.’
In lines 4 and 5, both JD and GL indicate their agreement with Lars’s assertion that part of the reason why Saxon German was so unpopular in preunification East Germany resulted from the unpopularity of East German politician Walter Ulbricht. In line 6, JD asks a question, presumably of Lars, as he is not only the interactant who introduced the subject of Ulbricht, but also an Eastern German who himself lived in East Germany prior to unification. Lars responds to this question of JD’s in line 7 by stating that he doesn’t know the answer, but in overlap with Lars’s response in line 8, the migrant Erika also answers, stating that Ulbricht was from Leipzig. This statement is phrased in a direct form, locating this East Germanspecific piece of information within her territory. The researcher JD, however, challenges Erika’s assertion in line 9, stating very strongly through both repetition and increased volume that she does not believe that Ulbricht was actually from Leipzig. Even after Lars supports Erika’s assertion in line 11, JD maintains her strong doubt in line 12, possibly in part because of Lars’s hedge through ich glaub ‘I think’, and Lars repeats his assertion of belief with the same hedge in line 13. After JD and Lars further describe the ways Ulbricht talked to support their argument in lines 14 through 23, and JD acknowledges through her ach so in line 19 that Erika and Lars may in fact be correct about Ulbricht’s origin, Lars asserts again in line 24 that he believes that Ulbricht is from Leipzig. It is only here, in line 25, that Erika enters the discussion again in support of her original assertion that Ulbricht was from Leipzig, but this time she uses the same hedge Lars has been using. This is a non-direct form that pushes her original direct assertion about Ulbricht outside of her own territory of information. Erika here downgrades her knowledge, possibly because of JD’s previous strong disagreement and in order not to challenge JD.
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
269
This happens after JD and Lars have both presented this East Germanspecific knowledge, while Erika has been silent, letting her Eastern German partner Lars step in and make a case for an argument he originally said he had no knowledge about. Through this silence, Erika defers to Klaus’s authority on matters Eastern German, despite the fact that the information about Ulbricht’s origin was introduced into the conversation by Erika in the first place. She then provides unrequested evidence for her assertion in line 28, indicating that she acquired this knowledge from other people, and thereby treating JD’s questioning as doubting Erika’s own authority on Eastern German matters. Even though she uses the passive wurde mal gesacht (literally: it was said) and thus does not explicitly state the source of her knowledge, at this point in the discussion where she, as a migrant, is trying to find support for her assertion, this support can only have come from a source within Eastern Germany, i.e. she must have heard it from locals. In other words, given Erika’s status as a migrant, providing an Eastern German source strengthens the validity of her knowledge. Through having been told this information about Ulbricht by Eastern Germans, she may locate this knowledge within her territory of information. Through this series of conversational strategies, she positions herself as a migrant who has some right to this knowledge, though, in contrast to Lars, she has to support this right by locating the source of knowledge outside herself. Taken together, this is therefore neither an Eastern German nor a Western German positioning, but one of “migrant Westerner in the East”. In the next and final segment it now is the migrant’s degree of closeness to the information which is challenged, even though the migrant himself initially perceives it as a challenge to the validity of his personal experience. This segment is a part of a conversation in which the migrant Walter recounts the story of trying to get a telephone installed in his Dresden apartment after his migration in 1994. The participants in the conversation are Walter, who is in his forties and migrated to Eastern Germany to be part of a research team at an institute, his partner Claudia (an Eastern German whom he met after his migration), and the two researchers, GL and JD.
270
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
Segment (6) 1
Walter: [nachdem ich ne dresdnerin kennengelernt hatte (.) die= [‘after I had met a woman from dresden (.) who had’=
2
GL:
3
Walter: =ganz gut- ganz gute beziehungen zur telekom hatte (..) =‘really good- really good connections to the phone company’ (..)
[mhm [‘mhm’
[und d[‘and th-’
4 5
JD:
[aha? [‘oh yeah?’
6
GL:
aha ‘I see’
7
Walter: und drei monate später hatten wir [telefon ‘and three months later we had a [telephone’
8
JD:
[da gings [‘you still
9
noch über beziehungen had to have connections’
10 GL:
ja ja ‘yeah yeah’
11 GL&JD:
((laughter))
[echt 12 Walter: das war wirklich so ‘it was really that way [really’ [aha [‘uh-huh’
13 GL: 14 GL&JD:
((laughter))
15 GL:
is ja13 wie im osten ‘just like in the East’
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
271
16 Walter: das [war ja auch osten ‘it [was the East’ [ 17 JD: [ja wir sind ja im osten [‘well we are in the East’ 18 Walter: das war ja noch der kluge osten (..) zumindest [für mich= ‘it was still the wise old East (..) at lEast [for me’= 19 GL: 20
[vierund[‘ninety-’ [neunzig ja? [‘four yeah?’
21 Walter: = [als zuge- für mich- zumindest für mich als zugereister jaja = [‘as a m- for me- at least for me as a migrant14 oh yeah’
In lines 1, 3, and 7, Walter is relating his personal experiences. By using direct forms to convey these experiences, he locates that information within his own territory and outside the territories of the other conversation participants, indicating that only he himself has that knowledge. Walter’s use of the word Beziehungen (‘connections’) in line 3, however, evokes a context of secrecy and insider knowledge typical of former East Germany. Since both GL and JD had personal experience living in former East Germany that Walter did not have, what would otherwise be a simple case of someone relating his own personal experience becomes a matter of positioning them, since he calls upon shared knowledge here. They react to this display of East German knowledge through their aha’s in lines 5 and 6. The actual story of the telephone installation reaches a satisfactory conclusion in line 7. What follows in lines 8–21, however, is a series of subtle challenges to Walter’s closeness to the information he is relating. In lines 8 and 9, JD comments on the notion that connections were ‘still’ (noch) a part of Eastern German society even four years after unification. By doing this, she makes explicit Walter’s evoking of former East Germany through the use of the word Beziehungen. In line 10, GL aligns herself with JD by appealing to their shared knowledge arising from their mutual experiences in former East Germany, and they join together further through laughter in
272
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
line 11. While Walter is the only custodian of his own personal experiences, he does not have the direct experience with former East Germany that both GL and JD have. Any information making reference to former East Germany therefore falls further inside GL’s and JD’s territories than Walter’s, since he can only have knowledge of that information through hearsay. Walter, in fact, does not join in GL’s and JD’s laughter, and through his reaffirmation of the truth of his experiences in line 12, seems to show that he interprets that laughter as casting doubt on the validity of his experiences. Without reaffirming the validity of Walter’s experience, GL comments in line 15 on his experience as typical of East Germany through the expression is ja wie im Osten15 (‘just (as you know) like in the East’). This seems to be an attempt to correct Walter’s assumption that the two researchers are challenging the validity of his experiences, but could also be interpreted as providing Walter with the reason for their laughter: Beziehungen reminds her of East Germany. She makes specific reference to shared knowledge through the use of the particle ja, which can be loosely translated as ‘as you know.’ Walter, in line 16, stresses that it is not ‘just like’ the East but that it ‘was’ the East, thus contradicting GL and making himself an authority on East German knowledge. In doing this, he not only employs a direct form, but also uses the particle ja. Through this particle, he indicates not only that his statement is valid, but that GL should in fact share in the knowledge of its validity, locating that validity within her territory of information as well as his. In line 18, Walter again presents himself as someone with knowledge of pre-unification East Germany by specifying directly that he views East Germany and Eastern Germany in 1994 as similar enough to be equated, again accompanied by the particle ja. However, he follows this direct form with a non-direct form modified by zumindest für mich als Zugereister (‘at least for me as a migrant’) in lines 18 and 21. By making specific reference to his migrant identity as part of this hedge, he is not calling into doubt the veracity of his own experiences, but he is casting doubt on the validity of his perception that Eastern Germany in 1994 still resembled former East Germany in certain important ways. By doing this, he moves the information he has about former East Germany to a point outside of his territory. This results in a re-positioning of himself as someone who does not have an adequate basis for asserting knowledge about former East Germany, at least as compared with two people who actually lived there before unification.
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
273
As the discussion in this section has shown, when challenged about their presentation of knowledge related to Eastern Germany, the migrants in both cases look for conditions under which the challenged knowledge becomes valid, and they do this by appealing to their migrant identities as nonEastern Germans. In segment (5), the migrant appears as non-Eastern German only implicitly by locating the source of knowledge with Eastern Germans rather than herself. In segment (6), the migrant explicitly formulates his identity as ‘somebody who moved here’, i.e. somebody with no direct experience in East Germany. In both cases, the use of direct forms as resources indexing the social category of Eastern German preceded the challenge, and in both cases, the challenge was only resolved when the speakers strategically formulated their identities as different from that of the Eastern Germans.
4.
Conclusion
In this paper, we have shown several different ways in which knowledge display about Eastern Germany can occur in conversations involving migrants from Western to Eastern Germany. First, there are Eastern Germanspecific vocabulary items in which the vocabulary item itself is connected with the social category of Eastern German. Second, there are instances where the display of knowledge about Eastern Germany is part of a narrated event, and makes up part of the content of the conversation. In both cases, the knowledge display is tied to the social category of Eastern German, although as we have shown, this effect can be reduced in several ways, including through the use of non-direct forms. Direct and non-direct forms are not connected to the social categories of Western and Eastern German in a one-to-one correspondence, or to state this differently, it is not the case that Eastern Germans always use direct forms when discussing Eastern German topics and Western Germans always use non-direct ones. Instead, these forms emerge as connected to social categories only when the conversational context in which they are used is taken into account. When the talk is about Eastern German knowledge and the forms used to express that knowledge are direct, the person expressing that knowledge positions himself as someone with an Eastern German social identity. If, however, he uses a non-direct form to do so, he marks that knowledge as outside of his territory of information, thereby positioning himself as someone with a non-Eastern German identity. Mi-
274
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
grants may be consistent about these self-positionings throughout a given conversation, but they may also vary them as a result of changes in the situational context or in response to their interactants. These kinds of shifts are not unique to these interactions, but in the case of our migrants, they indicate the complex nature of the migrants’ identities as Western Germans living in Eastern Germany for whom strategic positioning may become a means to address the relationship between Eastern Germany and Western Germany. We have also shown that migrants position not only themselves through their choices between direct and non-direct forms, but also their conversation partners. In turn, these conversation partners also position both themselves and the migrants. At any point, any conversation participant may either accept the first-order positionings offered or contest them by offering a second-order positioning. As shown in the analysis, interactants do this not only with reference to what has been said, but also with reference to their own and others’ biographies, their knowledge of the information in question, and their own positionings of themselves as Western German, Eastern German, or something else altogether. A second-order positioning, if accepted by the interactant being positioned, has the effect of relocating the piece of knowledge in question to a different place in that person’s territory, and thereby producing a different relationship between that person and the piece of information. Positioning is therefore not only relevant to the validity of the knowledge presented, but it can also be used strategically in response to a second-order positioning. Positioning involving the use of Eastern German-specific vocabulary is less open to challenge from the interlocutors than positionings involving the display of knowledge as part of a narrated event. These latter positionings are more easily challenged if former East Germans or others who experienced former East German culture are present. Our analysis has shown that migrants may use knowledge displays and variability in the form of vocabulary resources and direct and non-direct forms to validate their decisions to migrate. As such, they may transmit positive attitudes towards Eastern Germany, as well as present themselves as discursively integrating without conflicting with the territories of their interlocutors who may share different attitudes about in- and out-groups. These resources allow the migrants in our data to make connections between former East Germany and Eastern Germany without positioning themselves as former East German. The analysis presented here has also shown that implicit or explicit positioning can be strategically placed at a
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
275
point in the interactional organization where the knowledge display has been challenged.
Notes *
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6. 7.
We would like to thank Peter Auer, Werner Kallmeyer and two anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments on earlier versions of this paper. In this paper, the terms ‘East Germany’ and ‘West Germany’ refer to the two separate countries prior to October 3, 1990. The terms ‘Eastern Germany’ and ‘Western Germany’ refer to the same geographic territory subsequent to unification. ‘Eastern German’ and ‘Western German’ are analytic categories for anything associated with one or the other territory. These terms are also used in reference to people who position themselves as Eastern or Western German. An additional distinction may be made between ‘(former) East German’ vs. ‘Eastern German’ and ‘(former) West German’ vs. ‘Western German’, whereby the first of each refers to the time before unification and the latter to the time after. This paper is a part of a larger project entitled ‘(Inter)acting identities in dialect and discourse: migrant western Germans in Eastern Germany’, carried out by the authors and funded in 2003 by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (File number: 410-2003-0378). We are also grateful for funding from the University of Alberta and the University of Waterloo. We use the term ‘migrant’ throughout, but in each case we mean a migrant from Western to Eastern Germany rather than migrants to or from different places, or people who have relocated in general. While the migrants were still living in Eastern Germany in 2003, it is unknown to the authors whether they moved elsewhere since then or whether they stayed in Eastern Germany. While migration from Eastern to Western Germany was and continues to be quite common, there has been far less migration from Western to Eastern Germany, the direction in which the migrants in this paper moved. Names and some other pieces of information have been changed in order to protect participants’ identities. Transcription conventions are as follows: German utterances are in italics and English translations are directly beneath in normal type and single quotations. The transcript differs from usual orthographic spelling, e.g. capitalization in the transcript is used to mark loudness. Conversational overlap is indicated with square brackets. Pauses lasting a beat (.) or two (..) are indicated as shown; longer pauses are indicated in seconds. Laughter and parts deleted to
276
8.
9. 10. 11. 12. 13.
14.
15.
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain save space are written in ((double brackets)), and =equals signs= are used to indicate a continuation between previous and following lines of talk. The reason for his eye contact with GL and not with Diana or JD may have to do with GL’s East German origin, but it may also have to do with the fact that it was GL who asked the initial question about the preschool. It is also possible it is due in part to a separate conversation developing between Diana and JD, as noted in the transcript in line 11. For a discussion of also as a discourse marker, see Dittmar (2002). ‘Repair’, a conversation analytic term, refers to a mechanism used to deal with trouble in speaking, hearing, and understanding (Schegloff et al. 1977). Schlager Süßtafel and Zetti Knusperflocken are types of Eastern German chocolate snacks. Meißen, Saale Unstrut, and Saxony-Anhalt are Eastern German place names. Ja is not translated in the transcript in GL’s turn here or in the turns that follow, but it is a particle serving as a marker of shared knowledge that can be glossed approximately as ‘as you know’. Zugereister means ‘migrant’ in the sense of someone who has moved from one place to another place, and is not specific to the western-to-Eastern German context. For a discussion of the way Osten (‘the East’) indexes different time periods as used by the different speakers in this conversation, please see Liebscher and Dailey-O'Cain (2005).
References Antaki, Charles and Sue Widdicombe (eds). 1998 Identities in Talk. London: Sage. Auer, Peter and Friederike Kern 2001 Three ways of analysing communication between East and West Germans as intercultural communication. In: di Luzio, A., S. Günthner, and F. Orletti (eds.), Culture in Communication: Analyses of Intercultural Communication. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 89–116. Bakhtin, Michael M. 1981a The Dialogic Imagination. Four Essays, Michael Holquist (ed.). Austin: University of Texas Press. 1981b In: Holquist, Michael (ed.), The Dialogic Imagination. Four Essays. Austin: University of Texas Press. Bell, Allan 2001 Back in style: Reworking audience design. In: Eckert, P. and J. R. Rickford (eds.), Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 139–169.
Identity and positioning in interactive knowledge displays
277
Brausse, Ursula 1992 Wohl – The lexical variation of adverbs. Deutsche Sprache 20, 219– 243. Chafe, Wallace and Johanna Nichols 1986 Evidentiality: The Linguistic Coding of Epistemology. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Dittmar, Norbert 2002 Lakmustest für funktionale Beschreibungen am Beispiel von auch (Fokuspartikel, FP), eigentlich (Modalpartikel, MP) und also (Diskursmarker, DM). In: Fabricius-Hansen, C., O. Leirbukt, and O. Letues (eds.), Modus, Modalverben, Modalpartikeln. Trier: Wissenschaftlicher Verlag, 142–177. Drew, Paul 1991 Asymmetries of knowledge in conversational interactions. In: Marková, I. and K. Foppa (eds.), Asymmetries in Dialogue. Hertfordshire: Harvester Wheatsheaf, 21–48. Harré, Rom and Luk van Langenhove 1991 Varieties of positioning. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 21, 393–407. Helbig, Gerhard 1988 Lexikon deutscher Partikeln. Leipzig: VEB Verlag Enzyklopädie Leipzig. Heritage, John and Geoffrey Raymond 2005 The terms of agreement: Indexing epistemic authority and subordination in talk-in-interaction. Social Psychology Quarterly 68(1), 15–38. Kamio, Aki 1997 Territory of Information. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 1995 Territory of information in English and Japanese and psychological utterances. Journal of Pragmatics 21, 67–100. Liebscher, Grit 1997 Unified Germany (?): Processes of identifying, redefining and negotiating in interactions between East and West Germans, In: Chy, A., A.-M. Guerra, and C. Tetreault (eds.), Salsa IV. Proceedings of the Fourth Annual Symposium about Language and Society Austin. Texas Linguistic Forum 37. Austin: Dept. of Linguistics, The University of Texas at Austin, 78–87. Liebscher, Grit and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain 2005 West Germans moving East: Place, political space, and positioning in conversational narratives. In: Baynham, M. and A. De Fina (eds.), Dislocations/Relocations: Narrative of Displacement. Manchester: St. Jerome, 61–85.
278
Grit Liebscher and Jennifer Dailey-O’Cain
McKinlay, Andrew and Anne Dunnett 1998 How gun-owners accomplish being deadly average. In: Antaki, C. and S. Widdicombe (eds.), Identities in Talk. London: Sage, 34–51. Milroy, Lesley and Matthew Gordon 2003 Sociolinguistics. Method and Interpretation. Oxford: Blackwell. Ricker, Kirsten 2000 Migration, Sprache, und Identität: Eine biographieanalytische Studie zu Migrationsprozessen von Französinnen in Deutschland. Bremen: Donat Verlag. Sacks, Harvey 1995 Lectures on Conversation, vol. 1, Gail Jefferson (ed.). Oxford: Blackwell. Schegloff, Emanuel A., Gail Jefferson, and Harvey Sacks 1977 The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language 53(2), 361–382. van Langenhove, Luk and Rom Harré 1993 Positioning and autobiography: Telling your life. In: Coupland, N. and J. F. Nussbaum (eds), Discourse and Lifespan Identity. London: Sage, 81–99. Wolf, Ricarda 1999 Soziale Positionierung im Gespräch. Deutsche Sprache 1, 69–94.
Chapter 10 Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web Jannis Androutsopoulos 1.
Introduction
Drawing on sociolinguistics, discourse analysis, and research on computermediated communication, this chapter develops a sociolinguistic approach to language style on the Web. Based on findings of a study on Germanspeaking hip-hop websites and discussion boards, it aims at demonstrating how the notion of sociolinguistic style could deal with the complexities of linguistic variability in computer-mediated communication (CMC). In particular, this chapter proposes a framework of style analysis which attempts to bridge the gap between the social contexts of CMC and micro-linguistic processes in online text and talk, thereby using genre as a crucial mediating factor between the social and the linguistic. I start by contextualizing my approach in a brief discussion of style, computer-mediated communication, and the discourse arena of interest here, i.e. hip-hop. The notion of style adopted here is inspired by recent “speaker design approaches” (Schilling-Estes 2002: 388–394) as well as by the framework of the “social style of communication” by Kallmeyer and Keim (e.g. 2003). Notwithstanding their particular differences, these fairly new developments are influenced by interactional sociolinguistics and the ethnography of communication, and react to previous notions of style in sociolinguistics, in particular those by Labov and Bell (cf. Schilling Estes 2002; Bell 2001). In these latter frameworks, style is discussed in terms of stylistic variation along a one-dimensional axis between standard and non-standard/vernacular speech. By contrast, the approaches of interest here view style as a holistic pattern, in which linguistic structures on several levels work together with non-linguistic resources to index social positioning (cf. Bucholtz 1998, 2004; Coupland 2001; Eckert 2002; Eckert and Rickford (eds.) 2001; Keim and Schütte (eds.) 2002). Rather than understanding style as a reaction to formality or addressee, the active and constructivist nature of style is now
280
Jannis Androutsopoulos
foregrounded. In speaker design approaches, style is viewed “as a resource in the active creation, presentation, and re-creation of speaker identity” (Schilling Estes 2002: 388). Likewise, in the “social style of communication” framework, style is organically related “to a group’s culture and its social identity” (Kallmeyer and Keim 2003: 29). Moreover, rather than focusing on a single linguistic variable, a whole range of linguistic features is now examined, to the extent they are shown to be situatively relevant, even if they do not lend themselves to quantification. Researchers of social style thus aim at understanding how clusters of linguistic (and non-verbal, semiotic) resources gain local meaning as indices of particular social orientations. However, style in these approaches is mainly investigated in settings of face to face interaction. While Bell’s language style framework (Bell 1999, 2001) has repeatedly been applied to media discourse, new approaches to sociolinguistic style have hardly been taken out of the realm of the interpersonal and unmediated (but see Coupland 2001; Holly 2002). In this chapter, I argue that the sociolinguistic style approach outlined above can also provide a framework for addressing the complexities of language use in CMC. Computer-mediated communication has been an increasingly popular topic in empirical and applied linguistics in the last years, but relatively few studies have examined it from a sociolinguistic point of view. Much linguistic research on CMC has focused on media-related determinants of language use, and on the linguistic innovations that emerge as a response to media constraints. While this research has made significant contributions to our understanding of language use in online communication, it tends to downplay the social diversity of language use on the Web. A case in point is Crystal’s popular notion of “netspeak”, which is defined as “a type of language displaying features that are unique to the Internet, […] arising out of its character as a medium which is electronic, global, and interactive” (Crystal 2001: 18). Crystal conceives of “netspeak” as a language variety which is subdivided in the “language of e-mails” and the “language of chatgroups”. However, it is empirically questionable whether anything like a “language of e-mails” exists, simply because the vast diversity of users, settings and purposes of e-mail communication outweigh any common linguistic features. Similarly, while emoticons – e.g. :-) – and acronyms – e.g. lol ‘laughing out loud’ – are characteristic features of e-chat in general, it is the particular type of chat and the social profile of its users that are decisive in whether and to what extent these resources are used. Crystal
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
281
(2001: 155) does note the “variety of group practices” in CMC, without accounting for it in a systematic way. By contrast, this paper is situated within a growing body of research inspired by sociolinguistics and discourse analysis which aims at exploring the social and contextual diversity of language use in CMC (cf. collections by Androutsopoulos (ed.) 2006 and Danet and Herring (eds.) 2003; research overviews by Georgakopoulou 2003 and Herring 2003; as well as papers by e.g. Paolillo 2001, Sebba 2003b and this volume, Androutsopoulos and Ziegler 2004). Common denominators of these studies are the shift of focus from medium to user related patterns of language use, and the increasing emphasis on the varying instantiations of online genres in their particular social contexts. Georgakopoulou (2003: 2) points out the need for “contextual and particularistic analyses that shed light on how different contextual parameters shape and are evoked in the discourse of various types of CMC”. She identifies four main agendas of linguistic studies of CMC with a discourse-pragmatic focus: language use between writing and speaking, play and performance, self-presentation and identity, and the formation of online communities. In the following section, I draw on these agendas in sketching the framework of this paper. Hip-hop culture offers an ideal setting for the study of sociolinguistic style in computer-mediated communication. Over the last 20 years, hip-hop has developed from an African-American street culture into a globally acknowledged form of youth-cultural expression. A growing number of studies theorize the global spread of hip-hop as a process of cultural appropriation, in which forms of cultural expression – rapping, graffiti, dancing, djing – acquire local features and invite local interpretations that no longer rely exclusively on their African-American origins (cf. collections by Mitchell (ed.) 2001 and Androutsopoulos (ed.) 2003). However, ‘local’ hiphop does not completely separate from, but emerges in a constant dialogue with its ‘mother culture’. In Germany, for instance, hip-hop culture develops its own discourse around ‘local’ events and productions, all by drawing on U.S. American hip-hop as a source for new trends and a frame for the interpretation of local productions. Local hip-hop identities are immanently framed as part of globally distributed popular culture. The constant dialogue between the global and the local is manifested both in discourse and in language style. This is particularly salient in the language of German hip-hop fans with its vast amount of English, which extends beyond technical jargon and slang to communicative routines and emblematic codeswitching (cf. Androutsopoulos 2004).
282
Jannis Androutsopoulos
From the point of view of sociolinguistics and discourse analysis, hiphop foregrounds issues of linguistic creativity, performance, the relation between globalization and language (Pennycook 2003), and the reflex of language variation in popular culture (Morgan 2001). Rap has been repeatedly celebrated as a creative form of expression, in which all linguistic resources of a speech community are poetically intermingled. Several studies have documented how this credo translates in local appropriations of rap (cf. Androutsopoulos and Scholz 2002; Berns and Schlobinski 2003, as well as papers in Mitchell (ed.) 2001 and Androutsopoulos (ed.) 2003). As Streeck (2002) argues, rap artists engage in (lay) sociolinguistics as they draw on a multitude of linguistic varieties and styles in order to stage various, often conflicting social voices in their lyrics. Much the same could be said of the adolescents and young adults who use the Web “as a means of representing, critiquing and contradicting the images and issues of hip-hop culture” (Richardson and Lewis 2000: 251).
2.
Locating style on the Web: fields, communities, and genres
My sociolinguistic outlook on computer-mediated communication views the Web as a social space in which like-minded individuals use the resources of the medium, such as interactivity, multimodality, and easy access to media production, to construct identity and community (cf. Baym 2000; Döring 2003). However, these familiar notions must be understood in terms of the new communicative potentials that are provided by the New Media. With respect to identity, I follow here Mendoza-Denton’s definition of social identity as the active negotiation of an individual’s relationship with larger social constructs, in so far as this negotiation is signaled through language and other semiotic means. Identity, then, is neither attribute nor possession, but an individual and collective-level process of semiosis (Mendoza-Denton 2001: 475).
Identities on the Web may, then, be conceived of as processes in which individual relationships to larger social constructs are constructed and negotiated through text and talk. Internet users do not necessarily reproduce offline (or real-life) identities in their Web literacy practices, but may choose to foreground alternative aspects of self. They do so by drawing on
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
283
symbolic resources that are both restricted and extended, i.e. through image, animation, and sound, vis-à-vis direct interaction (cf. Chandler 1998; Hine 2000; Hawisher and Selfe (eds.) 2000; Snyder (ed.) 2002). Just as in ‘real life’, identities on the Web are formed in a continuous dialogue with larger social formations, which are usually termed ‘online’ or ‘virtual communities’ in CMC research. Following Castells, A virtual community ... is generally understood as a self-defined electronic network of interactive communication organized around a shared interest or purpose, although sometimes communication becomes the goal in itself. Such communities may be relatively formalized ... or be spontaneously formed by social networks which keep logging into the network to send and receive messages in a chosen time pattern (either delayed or in real time) (Castells 2000: 386).
Online communities are thus networks of interconnected individuals who engage in regular communication in a virtual space, such as a chat channel, a discussion board or a mailing list. Their members share a common interest or purpose; they develop social relationships and a set of shared interactional and linguistic norms. Virtual communities, as Castells points out, “are communities, but not physical ones, and they do not follow the same patterns of communication and interaction as physical communities do. But they are not ‘unreal’, they work in a different plane of reality” (2000: 389). The notion of virtual community links linguistic research on CMC with sociolinguistic theory, in which community – originally in the sense of speech community, more recently as a community of practice – has always played a crucial role (Paolillo 2001). However, in addition to the virtual community, a broader concept is needed. If we conceive of, say, the regulars on the discussion board of a football club as an online community, we also need a notion for the “meta-community” formed by several footballrelated discussion boards and websites, be it in a particular country or on a transnational scale. In other words, we need to acknowledge that online communities are embedded in larger patterns of online discourse and literacy practice. For these larger patterns, I propose the notion of ‘computer-mediated discourse field’. It is based on the concept of field originally developed by Bourdieu (1991) and extended by Fairclough (1995), i.e. as a structured space of positions for the articulation of a social discourse. In my working definition, a computer-mediated discourse field is a set of discursively and
284
Jannis Androutsopoulos
hypertextually interconnected websites, which represent a social discourse on the Web. While the notion of online community foregrounds networks of users, the notion of discourse field foregrounds semiotic artefacts, i.e. websites, rather than the individuals behind them. These websites and their users (authors and readers) share a common discourse, including, as will become clear in this paper, a set of common symbolic resources for the display of affiliation; however, member categories, production formats (Goffman 1981) and individual patterns of engagement within a discourse field on the Web may vary considerably. Online communities and discourse fields draw on a multitude of genres or text-types.1 The choice of genre is part of the situated management of identities that emerges from online activity. Some genres of CMC extend traditions of media discourse in a more or less straightforward way, e.g. editorials, reviews, reports or newsletters. However, genre blending and the emergence of hybrid genres are particularly salient in CMC, as is the case with personal homepages that draw on the private diary and the photo album (Chandler 1998; Karlsson 2002a, 2002b). The most important innovations of CMC are interactive multi-user formats such as chats, boards and newsgroups. Provided that they are regularly used by an online community, these sites of online talk host sequences that share at least some features of verbal interaction, i.e. dialogicity, sequentiality, and indexicality (cf. Herring 1999; Storrer 2000). Although sites of one-way communication (e.g. homepages) and sites of online interaction (e.g. discussion boards) are densely interrelated in practice, they need to be analytically separated in the study of online style. In personal homepages and other website genres, language style is designed in advance, in a process of composition and editing, and then displayed to an audience. The style of websites typically evokes issues of multimodality, as it involves an interrelation of verbal and visual resources. Within the verbal mode, it involves the linguistic design of names and categories (e.g. navigation bars) as well as issues of text layout (e.g. text blocks as opposed to lists; cf. Karlsson 2002b; Kress 2003). Within the visual mode, style involves the selection and composition of resources in typography, image, and color. In genres of online talk, on the other hand, communicative resources are essentially restricted to the verbal mode. In e-chats, newsgroups or discussion boards, each participant’s linguistic style is subject to immediate negotiation in the online community. The positions articulated and the linguistic forms used by individual members may be ratified or challenged, aligned to or contrasted by other participants.
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
285
All genres of online text and talk can be sites of performance, in the sense of “a display of communicative virtuosity” for an audience (Bauman 2004: 9). However, they differ with regard to, among other things, the available semiotic resources and in the immediacy of audience reaction. It is in genres of online interaction that “performance particularly flourishes” (Danet 2001: 100). This coexistence of communicative modes makes clear the need for complementary methodologies in the sociolinguistic study of CMC, in particular variation, genre and conversation analysis. How does the notion of style tie in with computer-mediated discourse fields, virtual communities, and genres? Social style emphasizes individual agency and the social embeddedness of online language use, as well as the interrelation of language with other signifying systems in constructions of social identity on the Web. It provides an interface between linguistic selections, interactional processes, communicative goals, and their larger social contexts. Typical dimensions of linguistic variability in CMC – e.g. between spoken and written language, standard and dialect, monolingual and multilingual speech – are resources for the construction of sociolinguistic style, their capital value depending on the ideologies and genre conventions of each particular community and field. Individual styles of online writing draw on these resources to varying degrees, depending on community norms, genre traditions, and individual goals. A task of the sociolinguistic style analysis proposed in this chapter is to disentangle style features related to a computer-mediated discourse field as a whole from those related to a particular genre or to individual users. Fields are constituted by a variety of genres, and any particular realization of a genre will carry the traces of its social context (Kress 2003: 100). Text and talk within a field can therefore be expected to contain ‘global’ style markers that occur across user networks and genres within the same field. On the other hand, genres rely on relatively stable patterns and conventions that operate across social contexts. We can therefore expect that genres within a field will be clearly distinguishable from one another, with indicators of genre differences operating on one or several linguistic levels. However, there is a considerable degree of freedom in the individual realization of (at least some) online genres. Users may shape and transform genres in order to construct individuality and originality or to express a particular stance vis-à-vis other users. Therefore, genre is the level on which style in a collective and individual sense is manifest. Different users have various degrees of exposure to and experience with CMC, and they may represent different orientations within a virtual community. We can
286
Jannis Androutsopoulos
expect these orientations to be indexed through linguistic choices, which, in turn, reflect the social-symbolic value of language styles in a particular community or field. The distribution of linguistic and interactional resources across genres and individual participants within a field of computer-mediated discourse is a central issue in the approach to online style outlined in this chapter.
3.
Research context and aims of this paper
The findings reported in this paper are drawn from a research project on ‘youth-cultural media styles’ which examined the language and discourse of German websites related to hip-hop culture.2 In consonance with other studies of media discourse (Fairclough 1995; Scollon 1998), the project’s aim was to explore connections between media texts and the practices of their production and consumption, as well as participants’ awareness of language style and variation in media discourse. For this purpose, a combination of linguistic analysis and online ethnography was developed. Ethnography is a meeting point of sociolinguistics and CMC studies. For sociolinguistics, it is an essential resource for understanding the social categories of a community and the meaning of sociolinguistic variation from the members’ point of view (cf. Eckert 1997; Kallmeyer and Keim 2003). CMC studies, on the other hand, draw on ethnography as a point of access to the formation of online communities, the patterning of online literacy practices, and the dynamic unfolding of online activities in relation to offline events (cf. Danet 2001; Döring 2003; Hine 2000; papers in Hawisher and Selfe (eds.) 2000; Snyder (ed.) 2002). Data collection included systematic observation of online activities as well as participant interviews. The reconstruction of the hip-hop field (cf. 4.1) was based on systematic observation, which started off from key nodes (portals, link directories), and browsed its way through to smaller sites. Several websites and discussion boards were repeatedly visited in order to develop a ‘feel’ for topics, trends and emblems in hip-hop discourse. A second set of ethnographic data consists of approximately 25 interviews with webmasters, homepage authors and community members. These interview partners were selected in a non-random manner, taking into account both the ‘richness’ of individual cases and practical issues such as regional location. The initial contact was established with an informal e-mail, followed by a semi-structured, face-to-face or email interview. The interviews
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
287
covered a broad range of topics, including the website’s aims and target group, production practices, semiotic resources, aspects of language use, personal opinion on hip-hop on the Web, and the relationship between online and offline cultural engagement. I confronted members with sample texts from their own website in order to elicit their awareness of style features and their evaluation of language variation. For purposes of linguistic analysis, samples from various genres were collected partly through systematic sampling on a particular website, and partly on the basis of specific topical triggers. In a strategy familiar from Kallmeyer and Keim’s framework (2003), board discussions were repeatedly scanned for controversial issues, based on the assumption that conflict interactions prompt condensations of social style and thus reveal constructions of identity in a particularly salient way. In hip-hop discourse, such issues include the representation of hip-hop in mass media, controversial artists and their new releases, the tension between regional hip-hop scenes (e.g. Berlin vs. Hamburg), and issues of sell-out and cultural authenticity. Four dimensions of linguistic variability were examined in the project: (a) variation on the continuum of typically spoken vs. written mode,3 (b) the use of non-standard language, especially regional varieties’ (c) spelling variation without a correspondence to phonetic features, and (d) language contact between German and English as well as between German and migrant languages. The analysis draws on both qualitative and quantitative methods, thereby joining approaches that were separately used in previous linguistic research on CMC. As suggested by Bell (1999), the multi-level structure of language style entails that some of its features will be relevant in their relative frequency and distribution over large data sets, while other features will have a highly contextual significance. In CMC research, a quantitative approach has been proposed by Paolillo (2001: 181), who focuses on relations of “micro-linguistic variation to the social mechanisms by which virtual communities are structured and maintained”. This approach demonstrates correlations of linguistic variables with social roles within an online community. An example for the interactional approach is Georgakopoulou (1997), who examines code- and style-shifting as contextualization cues in e-mail exchange, showing how participants draw on various linguistic resources in accomplishing interactional tasks. It seems to me that a combination of quantitative and qualitative methodologies is an advantage given the complexity of online language use (cf. also Siebenhaar 2006).
288
Jannis Androutsopoulos
The remainder of this chapter will discuss hip-hop on the Germanspeaking Web as a field of discourse that consists of various types of websites, genres, and participation formats (cf. 4.1). Style features that are typical for the field as a whole will be shown to exist in various lexical and discourse-pragmatic categories as well as on the nonverbal level (cf. 4.2). Based on findings of a case study, I then focus on language style in different genres of a particular website (cf. 5). I will provide evidence for the relationship between online genres and the orientation to a spoken or written language style, and present an interactional analysis of a board discussion to illustrate how participants’ strategic style choices reflect the socialsymbolic value of linguistic resources within the German hip-hop community.
4.
Hip-hop on the German-speaking web
Hip-hop’s popularity in Germany is reflected in the amount of computermediated activity of its fans who use the Internet as one additional means of articulating cultural affiliation and involvement. Reading online magazines dedicated to hip-hop, posting on dedicated discussion boards, chatting with other fans or making a personal homepage are practices of ‘vernacular literacy’ (Barton and Hamilton 1998) that enrich individual cultural experience without the aim of replacing real-life engagement. The amount of hip-hop websites on the German-speaking Web cannot be stated in a precise and definite way, because new websites appear and disappear every day. Judging from large link directories on web portals, several hundred German-speaking websites claimed to represent hip-hop on the Web in 2003.4 In this section, I first sketch a tripartite classification into (a) magazines/portals, (b) homepages, and (c) sites of online interaction, indicating relevant subdivisions where appropriate. In a second step, I outline a number of linguistic and non-verbal features shared by all websites in the field (cf. 4.2). 4.1
Outline of the field
Following Döring (2003: 520–548), I assume that the structure of a computer-mediated discourse field can be grasped in terms of a core-periphery scheme, a website's core position in the field being indicated by the amount
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
289
of page impressions and by the awareness of that website among users. At the time of my research, the core of the German hip-hop field on the Web consisted of no more than 12–15 online magazines, web portals and discussion boards that are widely known throughout Germany, including rap.de, hiphop.de, mzee.com, rapz.de, and webbeatz.de. Hip-hop magazines and portals5 operate in many respects like niche media (Thornton 1995). They address a comparatively wide audience, some receiving more than a million page impressions per month;6 they provide a wide variety of genres and constantly update their content, thus making maintenance a resource-demanding enterprise. Large hip-hop portals such as rap.de and hiphop.de depend on the music industry for the constant flow of new information, and place advertisements on their website. Their managers are in their late twenties; they are socialized in the hip-hop scene, and are in the process of turning their hobby into a profession. Young freelance writers contribute most of the site’s content, which is post-edited by the managers. While magazines and portals are the equivalents of traditional media institutions, homepages represent individual actors. Commercial homepages are run by established artists or music labels; personal homepages, on which I focus here, are run by fans, activists and amateur artists. Chandler (1998) suggests that the main purpose of making a personal homepage is to construct identity through text and other semiotic resources. In the hip-hop field, main communicative aims of homepages are self-presentation as an active member, promotion of individual productions, and making connections with like-minded activists (cf. Androutsopoulos 2003). Personal homepages presume a clear notion of authorship. Authors may not reveal their real name, but nevertheless work on establishing individuality and continuity. Being visited by a far smaller audience than magazines, personal homepages usually feature no advertisement, and they contain a limited range of genres. The typical hip-hop homepage consists of an artist bio and photos, samples of the author’s work, links to other homepages and/or to core websites, and a guest book. Discussion boards and chat channels offer platforms for the formation of online communities, and thereby constitute the field’s public sphere. Most large German-speaking hip-hop boards are hosted by portals (e.g. rap.de, hiphop.de), but independent boards exist as well (e.g. mzee.com). According to webmaster information, hip-hop board members are between 14 and 29 years of age, mostly males, with varying educational background. The six discussion boards I observed in more detail (rap.de, hiphop.de,
290
Jannis Androutsopoulos
mzee.com, rapz.de, webbeatz.de, and epoxweb.de) had a sum of more than 65,000 registered users in the summer of 2003. This number certainly includes a significant amount of occasional posters; others, however, stick to a board and develop social bonds, thus becoming community members over a period of time. My observations and members’ judgements suggest that these large boards differ in terms of regulars, their music taste, favorite topics as well as in aspects of language use. In contrast to magazines and homepages, anonymity and reduced responsibility of authorship turn boards into liminal spaces (cf. Sebba 2003b). In liminal contexts, speakers cross certain borders of social behavior and performance; they experiment with social identities and language styles in ways that are clearly outside their normal, everyday repertoire (Rampton 1995). In the German hip-hop context, acts of language crossing involve, expectedly, the ‘gangsta’ stereotype and other aspects of the imagery of African American hip-hop (cf. Androutsopoulos 2004). Multi-generic literacy practices are characteristic for online activists, i.e. participants with a strong commitment and engagement. In addition to board or e-chat membership and the authoring of a personal homepage, activists typically assume various responsibilities in the field’s public domain, e.g. as administrators of a discussion board or freelance writers for an online magazine. These activities involve different language styles, resulting in style-shifting as members turn from e.g. news writing to board contributions. A clear distinction between the language style of magazines and boards is borne out by the findings discussed in the next section (cf. 5.1). 4.2. ‘Global’ markers of hip-hop style To what extend is style a useful notion with respect to the hip-hop field as a whole? To rephrase this in empirical terms, what style features do all these hip-hop websites and discussion boards have in common? The answer first points outside the verbal domain. Visual cues that are highly typical for the field include logos and navigation bars in graffiti type, visual metonyms or synecdoche for hip-hop’s four elements (i.e. sneakers for breakdance, a turntable for dj-ing, a microphone for rapping, and a spray can for graffiti), as well as certain features of photographic representation (cf. 5.3). The answer then points to the naming patterns participants draw on in designing their personal nicknames and the names of their websites. For example, names such as BeatSkill Crew and Bad Grade Click are unmistakably
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
291
marked as hip-hop by means of specific lexical items, name formation patterns and intertextual references (cf. Androutsopoulos 2001; Bierbach and Birken-Silverman, this volume). The main linguistic realm of hip-hop style in the German-speaking Web is vocabulary and formulaic speech of English origin, including both major wordclass items (with various degrees of morphological integration into German) and instances of (emblematic) code-switching into (stylized African-American) English. This vocabulary includes a lengthy list of culturally significant key words that cut across hip-hop’s forms of expression, among them battle, bite, diss, flow, freestyle, props, represent, and respect.7 Other parts of hip-hop vocabulary are more practically described in terms of lexical fields, e.g. terms for song structure and music production. This is more or less a technical jargon, which partially overlaps with lexis more widely used in pop music discourse. It is supplemented by slang items, notably evaluators (whack, dope, fresh, burner) and categorizations (bitch) as well as by an extensive set of formulas and conversational routines: openers and farewells such as peace and one love, exclamations such as aight, formulas for giving props or respect to other community members. Obviously, a large part of these items are not ‘school English’ but originate from exclusive hip-hop sources, and have a strong indexical function. The same holds true for a set of stereotypical spelling variants applied mostly to English items, in particular as noun plural marker (as in beatz, cutz, trackz, and stylez), instead of as in phat and phunky, and instead of on lexical items such as kool and kru (‘crew’). Though systematic research was restricted to German data, glimpses at hip-hop discourse in other European speech communities (notably in Italy, France, Greece, and Norway) suggest that these items and variants are indeed globally spread. What is characteristic for the German hip-hop field, then, seems at the same time a good example for emergent global English “from below” (cf. Pennycook 2003; Preisler 1999). To support these observations, a 35,000-word sample of record reviews, personal homepages and board discussions from webbeatz.de, the website discussed in more detail in the next section, was subjected to a concordance analysis. All items with five or more tokens (N=983) were examined in semantic-functional terms. In particular, almost all nouns with 10 or more tokens in this sample8 refer to hip-hop and rap (e.g. hip-hop, rap, flow, style, battle, mcs) or to pop music in general (e.g. beat, album, track, text, band). This list also includes items with stereotypical spelling variants (beatz, cutz, trackz, stylez), slang evaluators (fett, dope, fresh, wack), and
292
Jannis Androutsopoulos
conversational routines such as peace and props. This suggests that discourse on webbeatz.de is quite homogeneous in terms of culture- and topicrelated lexical choices. Most lexical items in this cross-genre sample are highly specific to hip-hop discourse. A writing style considered highly typical for hip-hop websites was termed “hip-hop slang” by some participants. Discussion of examples in the interviews suggested two main features of this member category, i.e. hiphop related English (as outlined above) and markers of oral style. Hip-hop slang partly reflects face-to-face discourse; partly it is specific to computermediated communication, as some of its features are contingent to genres of online talk. Two examples will be used to illustrate these points: (1) Board discussion Um ehrlich zu sein, das Album finde ich echt dope. Afrob ist für mich sowieso eina der echt guten Texter. Sehr gute Lyricz und gute Beatz. Zwa jetzt keine Überbombe aba bessa alz andere Sachen die es zur zeit gibt, natürlich nicht so dope wie Azad aba es lohnt sich auf jeden fall mal reinzuhören. ‘To be honest, I think the album is really dope. Afrob is in my view one of the best text writers anyway. Very good lyrics and good beats. It's not the bomb, but still better than other things going on at the moment. Of course not so dope as Azad but it's worth it listening to it.’ (2) Board discussion @dable: hab gesehen, dass du mit civilrings nach berlin kommst. vielleicht können wa connecten, wie im Sommer 2001 (gloob isch). ‘@dable: [I] saw that you're coming to Berlin with the civilrings, perhaps we could connect like in the summer of 2001 (I believe).’
Example (1) displays a wealth of features which, as a webmaster suggested, are “judged as ‘underground’ affiliation” by members. There is a systematic substitution of –er endings by –a (einer > eina ‘one’, besser > bessa ‘better’, aber > aba ‘but’), and of –s by –z (Lyricz, Beatz, alz ‘than’). The –a variant is familiar from AAVE (cf. gangsta) and at the same time a phonetic spelling with respect to German. The –z variant is a major global stereotype for hip-hop slang. Originally a phonetic spelling (cf. boyz), it is often used as a purely visual marker (cf. lyricz). The author of (1) idiosyncratically extends the distribution of –z to a German lexical item, alz (‘than’). This is an illustrative example of what Sebba (2003a) terms “spelling rebellion”,
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
293
i.e. a deliberate transformation of orthography in an unregulated literacy field which uses the distance from orthographic norm to contextualize attitudes or to index cultural affiliations. However, a dense patterning of phonetic and homophone spellings as in (1) is not the rule in my data. An instance of vernacular writing without any of these features is (2). This is heavily marked as spoken in syntax (subject pronoun deletion) and spelling (habe > hab 'I have', wir > wa 'we'). In addition, it includes a style shift to an approximation of Upper Saxon vernacular (gloob isch instead of glaub ich ‘I believe’), the morphologically integrated English verb connecten and the @ sign, which is widely used as an addressing particle in online talk. These examples suggest that both German and English vernacular forms are valued resources in the German-speaking hip-hop field on the Web. However, assuming that hip-hop slang carries along uncontested symbolic capital would overlook the field's complexity in terms of genres and participation formats. The findings presented below suggest that hip-hop slang and the representation of typical spoken features are more densely patterned in online talk, but less so on personal homepages and even less in genres of edited content such as record reviews. Interviews with portal managers reveal a clear awareness of the appropriate language style for edited content. When it comes to the selection of freelance writers, gutes schriftdeutsch, i.e. a solid competence in written German is preferred to the vernacular style prevailing in boards and chats. My interviews also suggest that a writing style such as in extract (1) may seem prestigious to teenage board members, but is rejected as ‘kiddie stuff’ by older members with more experience who strive for a professional image.
5.
A case study: language style, genre, and member identities on webbeatz.de
Against this backdrop, we now focus on language style on a particular website, i.e. webbeatz.de. This is a well known website in the German speaking hip-hop field, with approximately 3,000 registered members and 100,000 page impressions per month during research. According to the webmaster, a young adult who works in the ICT sector, webbeatz.de is a non-commercial “one and a half man project”, which relies on volunteers for edited content and maintenance of discussion boards. While advert banners appeared on the site later on, membership and music downloads were still free of charge (as of mid-2004). Like other large hip-hop sites, webbeatz.de
294
Jannis Androutsopoulos
features both edited content (e.g. reviews, interviews, reports) and a large discussion board. Its particularity is the kostenlose HipHop Promotion Plattform, i.e. a free of charge service which provides amateur artists with a free homepage to present themselves and their music. At the time of research, webbeatz.de hosted more than 300 artist homepages, each composed of a short self-presentation text, a photo, a link to downloadable songs, and a link to a Kritikforum, i.e. a dedicated critique board. In a typical reception structure, the visitor will select an artist from the overview, read the self-presentation, listen to the songs, then proceed to the critique board, comment on the songs, and sometimes interact with other fans or the artists themselves who occasionally visit their board to read and respond to comments. Many critique boards feature dozens of entries, with discussions extending over weeks or even months. This is an ideal setting for a genre comparison as well as for tracing the construction of individual style. 5.1. Genre and spelling variation A random representative sample of the three main genres on webbeatz.de, i.e. artist homepages, critique boards and record reviews,9 was subjected to a quantitative analysis of spelling variation focusing on ‘colloquial spellings’, i.e. orthographic representations of phonetic-phonological features of colloquial spoken German (cf. Androutsopoulos 2000). Before discussing the results, I briefly introduce the three genres and discuss one example for each. Board discussions can be expected to display features of conversational style due to their situational features, i.e. immediacy of online interaction, spontaneous production, reduced responsibility of authorship, and affective interactional modality. By contrast, record reviews are modelled on a traditional written genre and are subject to post-editing by the website manager. Artist homepages on webbeatz.de require careful planning as far as their communicative purpose, i.e. self-presentation, is concerned. However, they do not follow a unique generic model. Some authors draw on the personal diary or the short bio note in designing their texts, others opt for a conversational style, and still others imitate professional press releases (see examples 6 and 7 below). Examples (3)–(5) illustrate some aspects of the three genres’ content and speech style.
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
295
(3) Board discussion Ich weiß nicht… reality HipHop und dann „Players" im Namen? Der Beat hört sich für mich zu poppig an und die raps zu sehr nach fantastischen vier… also irgendwie kann ich mich damit nicht identifizieren, die hook ist auch ein wenig langweilig, also is nich grade mein fall ‘I don’t know... reality hip-hop and then naming yourself ‘players’? The beat sounds too poppy to me, and the raps are too much like fantastischen vier... [German rap band, J.A.] well I somehow can’t identify with it, the hookline is a bit boring too, well it’s not really my thing.’ (4) Artist homepage Eigentlich sind wir keine richtige crew, eher mehr so'n Soundsystem. Das alles begann mal Anfang des Jahres 2000 und flachte seitdem wieder ab! Zur zeit mache ich selbst nur noch Beatz und Smartie ging mehr in djing über, schade. Die ersten Sachen der Zeit werde ich in nächster Zeit euch mal zuschicken. ‘We’re not a real crew actually, more like a sound system. It all began at the beginning of 2000 but levelled ever since! I’m doing only beatz at the moment, and Smartie has turned to DJ-ing, it’s a pity. I’ll send you some early stuff from this period in a while.’ (5) Record review Nach „Break Ya Neck“, wohl einem der meistgespieltesten Club-Tracks dieses Jahr, gibts jetzt die nächste Auskopplung aus Busta Rhymes „Genesis“-Album. „Pass the Courvoisier, Part II“, das P. Diddy and Pharrell featured, knüpft genau da wieder an, denn der stark nach vorne gehende Neptunes-Beat ist definitiv Clubtauglich. ‘After “Break Ya Neck”, one of this year’s most played club tracks, the next release from Busta Rhymes’s “Genesis” album now follows. “Pass the Courvoisier, Part II” featuring P. Diddy and Pharrell ties in with that, because the massively forward-pressing Neptunes beat is definitely club compatible.’
The author of (3) questions the artist’s stated mission (reality HipHop) visà-vis their name, Players, which evokes ‘gangsta rap’, and then comments on the song’s beat, rap style and hookline. His orientation to spoken syntax is indicated by simple clauses, paratactic structure, discourse markers such as ich weiß nicht and also. The last clause, also is nich grade mein fall (‘well it’s not really my thing’) is heavily marked as spoken through subject pronoun deletion, the discourse marker also (‘well’), final consonant
296
Jannis Androutsopoulos
deletion (ist > is, nicht > nich), and interconsonantal vowel deletion (gerade > grade). The author of (4) presents himself and his band in a narrative mode, and directly addresses his readers in the last clause. Simple clauses and clitization (so ein > so'n) indicate an overall informal style. The review excerpt (5) is written from an impersonal perspective. It includes comparatively longer, subordinate clauses, and complex noun phrases (einem der meistgespieltesten Club-Tracks ‘one of the most played club tracks’, der stark nach vorne gehende Neptunes-Beat ‘the massively forward-pressing Neptunes beat’). The variables selected for analysis are typical features of spoken German (cf. Schwitalla 1997) and are repeatedly mentioned in German CMC literature as frequent features of informal online writing. They occur in quantifiable amounts in my sample, ranging from 51 to 1,440 tokens.10 (a) deletion of word-final /t/ in consonant clusters, e.g. nicht > nich (‘not’) (b) reduction of the indefinite article in all genders and cases, e.g. eine > ne (‘a’ sing.fem.nom. or acc.) (c) negative adverb nichts (Standard German [nICts] ‘nothing’) written nix (corresponding to the colloquial pronunciation [nIks]); (d) clitization of post-verbal es (object or dummy pronoun) after four different verbs (finden ‘find’, geben ‘give’, gehen ‘go’, haben ‘have’) in the 1st and 3rd person singular, e.g. habe es > habs, gibt es > gibts; (e) deletion of verb-final /e/ in the 1st and 3rd person singular of 16 different verbs (including high frequency verbs such as brauchen ‘need’, haben ‘have’, kommen ‘come’, sagen ‘say’), e.g. habe > hab. All five features have a ‘written’ variant, i.e. the standard orthographic representation, and a ‘spoken’ one, corresponding to the colloquial reduced or cliticized form. Based on all tokens of each variable, the frequency of spoken variants was counted for all three genres in the usual variationist way, i.e. all factual over potential occurrences of the variant, excluding categorically invariant cases. The findings (Table 1) suggest a clear distinction between the boards and the two other genres, and a more subtle distinction between artist homepages and reviews.11 Board discussions have a much higher amount of spoken variants throughout, ranging from more than 60% for features (d) and (e), to slightly over half for (c), 38% for (b) and 22% for (a). Homepages score higher than reviews for four features, (d) being the exception. This is largely due to the cliticized form of the construction gibt es > gibt's
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
297
or gibts (‘there is’), which is frequently used in the reviews. A look at each variable reveals different distribution patterns. Features (a) and (b) have lower than average scores for all genres, with extremely low amounts of colloquial spellings in reviews and artist homepages. Feature (e) scores higher than average in all genres, and particularly high in the artist homepages, in which a personal style is more frequent. The distribution of (c) is similar to the average. Table 1. Frequency of five colloquial spellings in three genres
Reviews N % Final -t deletion Indefinite article reduction nix (instead of nichts) Clitization of post-verbal es Deletion of verb-final -e Total Average %
Artist homepages N %
Boards N %
Total N %
2
0,5
9
3
169
22
180
12,5
6
2
14
5
105
38
125
15
1
7
2
22
28
52
31
42
10
43
2
20
11
61
23
45
4 23 / 753
21
42 69 / 680
49
15
20
178 62 224 491 / 1390 583 / 2823 47
57 34
In sum, these findings suggest that writing style in the hip-hop field is partly determined by genre. This pattern was repeatedly found in the data: boards capitalize on written representations of spoken/colloquial features to a far greater extent than other genres. As a full description of these genres is beyond the scope of this paper, suffice it to say that reviews feature comparatively more of the technical jargon of hip-hop, while board discussions are the principal site for slang items, conversational routines, and stylized African-American English. Personal homepages have a less clear position in this respect.
298
Jannis Androutsopoulos
5.2. Genre and individual styling While these three genres are overall distinct in terms of their orientation towards the spoken or written mode, the language style of individual members sometimes follows and sometimes diverges from that pattern. Genres differ in the creative freedom they grant their users. While record reviews are quite homogeneously patterned in the webbeatz.de data, homepages display a striking range of individual variation. This is not surprising, taking into account that the main purpose of the artist homepage is precisely to communicate individual style. Some artists construe themselves in a more professional manner, while others aim at a style that is closer to street culture. Consequently, some homepage texts are stylistically closer to discussion boards, while others strive for a standard oriented writing style. This difference is evident in the two examples presented below. (Sentence numbering in square brackets is added for reference; italics in the glosses indicate English items in the original text; bold type in example 7 indicates reflexes of colloquial and regional speech.) (6) Artist homepage text [1] Einer der erfolgversprechendsten Gruppen der Flensburger HipHop-Szene ist die BeatSkill Crew. [2] Durch Zusammenarbeit mir Künstlern von Flensburg bis Salzburg, diverse von ihnen geplante Events und vor allem durch ihre Auftritte haben sich Mafuba und Dragon bereits einen Namen gemacht. [3] Während Mafuba durch ihren einzigartigen, teils mit Gesangspassagen gemischten Reimstil Eindruck macht, sorgt Dragon für die passenden, teils asiatisch und teils funkinspirierten Beats. [4] Zur Zeit in Arbeit sind das neue Album, das diesen Sommer fertig sein wird, sowie ein Beitrag für die Querschnitt-Compilation, die im März mit einer Erstauflage von 1.000 Stück erscheinen wird. [5] Live sind die beiden als nächstes am 16.03. in Flensburg mit Justus & Fumanschu (M.O.R.) zu sehen, danach sind erstmal wieder Studio-Sessions angesagt. ‘[1] One of the most promising bands from the Flensburg hiphop scene is the BeatSkill Crew. [2] Mafuba and Dragon have already made a name through the cooperation with artists from Flensburg to Salzburg [= end points of the Germanspeaking area, J.A.], through the planning of various events and especially through their gigs. [3] While Mafuba impresses through her unique rhyming style, which is partly interspersed with singing parts, Deragon takes care of the appropriate, partly Asian and partly funk inspired beats. [4] They are currently working on their new album, which will be ready this summer, and on a contribution to the Qerschnitt compilation, which will be out in March, starting with 1,000 copies. [5] Their next
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
299
live appearance is 3/16 in Flensburg, together with Justus & Fumanschu (M.O.R.), after that some more studio sessions will follow.’ (7) Artist homepage text [1] Straight Up Hip Hop - Straight aus München. [2] Akuma: „Wir wolln halt, dass ma München ned ausschließlich mit Sound wie Blumentopf oder David P verbindet. [3] Wo is der dreckige, abgefuckte Scheiß hier? [4] Ich kann mir nimmer diesen ‚Was geht ab digga’ Sound anhören, so isses ned. [5] Das Leben is ned nur aus Party und feiern und cool rappen, oider!“ [6] Aufgenommen wird der Sound im jahrelang selber erweiterten BRA.CHI.AL Studio a bisserl außerhalb Münchens. [7] Die Beats bestehen zu 100% aus samples, sonst nix! [8] Gemastert wird das ganze vom S in Darmstadt. [9] Grüße an Ka, So, Kr, Ha, LC, Gr. ‘[1] Straight up hip hop - straight >from Munich [2] Akuma: “What we want is that Munich not be exclusively associated with a sound like Blumentopf or David P. [3] Where's that dirty, fucked-up shit here? [4] I can't hear this ‘What's up digga’ sound no more, that's not how it is. [5] Life is not just partying and having fun and cool rapping, mate!” [6] The sound is recorded in the BRA.CHI.AL studio, which we have been extending for years, a bit outside Munich. [7] The beats consist of 100% samples, nothing else! [8] The whole thing is mastered by S. in Darmstadt. [9] Greets to Ka, So, Kr, Ha, LC, Gr.’
The first text (6) is strongly reminiscent of promotional discourse by the music industry. It is syntactically quite complex (see sentences 2, 3, 4), including two heavily modified noun phrases in sentence 3. Standard orthography is used throughout, indicated by noun capitalization, which is required in standard German, and the absence of colloquial spellings. These artists frame their self-presentation by their success potential (cf. erfolgsversprechend ‘promising’ in sentence 1) and foreground their current production activities. By contrast, the writers of the second text (7) foreground issues of style in their local hip-hop community, i.e. Munich; they challenge established artists (cf. sentence 2), and stress their contacts to the local scene (cf. sentence 9). This self-presentation consists of a headline, a quotation by a band member, and a description of the band’s sound. The quotation (sentences 2–5) is heavily marked as spoken (see items in bold type). Some of these spellings reflect general features of colloquial German including the ones discussed above (e.g. ist > is, wollen > wolln, nichts > nix); others are more
300
Jannis Androutsopoulos
specific to Southern German varieties (man > ma ‘one’, nicht > ned ‘not’, nimmer ‘no more’, a bisserl ‘a bit’, alter > oider ‘mate (lit. ‘old man’)). The author draws on these features in stressing the band’s outlook on hiphop in Munich, i.e. what they like and what they reject. The quotation also includes bits and pieces of hip-hop slang, e.g. the use of Scheiß probably modelled after AAVE shit, and the phrase ‘Was geht ab digga’ Sound (digga is a German hip-hop slang term of address). The last part of this text, a greeting to the writer’s crew, is reminiscent of online discussion boards and guest books. Despite these differences, both texts are immediately recognizable as belonging to the field of hip-hop discourse. From a member’s point of view, as reconstructed in online ethnography, none of these texts is more ‘authentic’ or ‘fake’ than the other. These are rather two different ways of designing a young artist identity through language, which co-operate with other dimensions of mediated identity design, including photos, band logo, and the music itself. They demonstrate the importance of transgressing the seemingly homogeneous genre to include individual profiles in online style analysis. 5.3. Debating style in online talk: The case of Hecklah & Coch Moving from homepage texts to online talk, this section will focus on a discussion that took place on the critique board of a band called Hecklah & Coch. These two young Berlin artists represent ‘Berlin rap’, a recently popular rap style that draws on the tradition of U.S. American ‘gangsta’ rap. Their identity design on their homepage clearly appropriates aspects of ‘gangsta’ rap. This holds true for their name, which refers to the German gun manufacturer Heckler & Koch. Its respelling to Hecklah & Coch draws on the -er/-ah spelling alternation (as in gangstah, sistah), which is quite common in hip-hop discourse, and has a straightforward model in the name of the well-known U.S.formation Smif & Wessun, which in turn appropriates the name of the U.S. gun manufacturers Smith & Wesson. Besides alluding to this U.S. predecessor, the respelling presumably protects the band from copyright trouble. Moreover, the homepage photo depicts the two band members holding guns and looking down to the viewer. This is a visual resource for positioning the represented person as powerful (Kress and van Leuuwen 1996: 146), and quite a popular pose in ‘gangsta’ rap, the imagery of which often capitalizes on aggressive domination (cf. Haugen 2003).
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
301
At the time of sampling, the Hecklah & Coch board included 90 contributions, posted in 19 days, with a sum of 4,056 words and an average length of 45 words.12 I will focus here on the first 29 entries, which were posted by 16 persons in 43 hours. In terms of sequential structure, this is a series of reactive posts that comment on the band, interspersed by short interactive sequences, which start at entry 10 and occur until 26. Eight participants contribute one posting each, six contribute two, and three participants contribute three postings each. I will quote three lengthy parts from this thread, i.e. entries 2–8, 19–21, and 22–29.13 (8) Board discussion (original numbering) 1. webmaster – 13.18 Hier ist Platz für deine Meinung zu Hecklah & Coch. 2. Dan – 14.00 beat geht so...raps auch...alles standard.nichts besonderes..euer foto find ich krass lächerlich... aber wer weiss ..vielleicht lauft ihr ja in berlin wirklich so hart rum...dann is ok... dieses „geld macht sex..geld macht fame..macht hass..“..hört sich krass scheisse an.. peace. 3. Dan – 14.01 aber vielleicht hate ich auch einfach nur... auch möglich...dann erschießt mich halt. 4. P-A – 14.20 wer ist Dan? track ist fett. auch dicker beat 5. Rolex –14.40 Ich find das Cover auch nich gut.....aber eigentlich isses mir Wayne! Beat is cool, Raps sind auch in Ordnung! 6. QM – 15.50 Hehe, dicker Track! [2nd and 3rd clause omitted] 7. Jim – 16.23 berliner style =) yes, ich hab paar tracks von euch am start und fand euch eigentlich schon recht fresh. also macht weiter jungs. Peace 8. Deep P – 16.35 Der beat geht nach vorn - ist aber trotzdem nichts besonderes.Zum text sag ich nur „hunde die bellen beissen nicht“ - mehr als peinlich jungs... Das photo ist echt der Hammer-fehlen nur noch zwei bitchez, ihr seid soooo lächerlich...
302
Jannis Androutsopoulos
‘1. webmaster – 13.18 Express your opinion on Hecklah & Coch here. 2. Dan – 14.00 beat is okay... raps too ... standard stuff. nothing special. your photo I find totally ridiculous... but who knows.. maybe you do run around as hard as that in berlin... in this case it’s ok... this “money makes sex..money makes fame..makes hate..” [= quotes song lyrics, J.A. ].. sounds totally crap. peace 3. Dan – 14.01 But maybe I'm just hating... could also be true... well then just shoot me. 4. P-A – 14.20 Who’s Dan? the track is fat. fat beat too. 5. Rolex –14.40 I don’t like the cover either, but it’s really the same to me! the beat is cool, the raps are also ok! 6. QM – 15.50 Hehe, fat track! [2nd and 3rd clause omitted] 7. Jim – 16.23 Berlin style =) yes, I’ve got some tracks from you guys and always thought you’re quite fresh. Well keep it up guys. peace 8. Deep P – 16.35 The beat moves forward, but is nothing special. About the text I can only say “barking dogs don’t bite” - more than just embarrassing, guys... The photo is just about the limit - only thing missing is two bitchez, you’re soooo ridiculous...’
The thread starts with a series of comments on the band’s music and selfpresentation, written in a usual board style. The contributions are quite short and syntactically simple; they feature non-standard orthography (lack of noun capitalization) and hip-hop slang (fresh, bitchez). The abundance of music-related terms (track, beat) and evaluators (cool, fett, fresh, ok) reflects the board’s communicative purpose, i.e. discussing the artist’s music. What is exceptional here is the attention paid to the band’s picture (cf. entries 2, 3, 8 as well as 19, 21, 22 later on). In post 2, Dan points out that the picture probably does not reflect lived experience; in 3, he ironically challenges the band to prove their authenticity, i.e. use their guns. In post 8, Deep P suggests a missing element, i.e. two bitchez, to complete the visual cliché. Both call the photo lächerlich (‘ridiculous’), emphasizing its lack of
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
303
authenticity. Significantly, the posts that are positive towards the music, partly identifying its local style (cf. 7), hardly refer to the photo, with the exception of Rolex (5) who downplays its importance. As the thread unfolds, other posters defend the band’s style and react to the critique. Some of these responses display two techniques of derogation that are reminiscent of ‘dissing’, i.e. the genre of (aggressive) verbal challenge in hip-hop culture. These techniques are the derogatory reference to the opponent’s origin and music taste. For example, in post 11 (not included here), Jimmy challenges Dan (author of posts 2 and 3) to shut up or keep on listening to beginner (original wording: halt einfach dein maul oder hör weiter beginner!) The reference to beginner, a commercially successful German rap band, indexes a different music taste within Germanspeaking rap, which Jimmy presumably rejects as being too soft when compared to Berlin rap. (9) Board discussion (continued) 19. Ryke – 14.25 (day after) [1st para discussing song omitted] euer foto.. naja, der eine ist aufm internat, der andere war fürn jahr im ausland, ob ihr gerade die richtigen seid um „das getto zu representen“ weiss ich jetzt auch net. versteh das nicht als dis gegen eure skills, da geht schon was, vor allem wenn ihr schon jams klargemacht habt und so... peace ryke 20. TOC – 14.54 tach... @deep p wo kommst du her? Pinneberg? keine angst die tun dir nichts. @ryke sylvestah war '98 ein jahr in england sonst sind alle in schöneberg geboren und keiner war lange im ausland (ausser letztes jahr auf hawaiiiii) ... und wer das mit dem cover immer noch nicht geplant hat, tut mir leid. 21. Ryke – 15.09 okay, ich weiss was du meinst, ihr wiss was ich mein - ich finde guns ja auch ganz flashig und so.. aber besonders innovativ oder selbstironisch ist das halt nicht, gerade für berlin. peace
304
Jannis Androutsopoulos
‘19. Ryke – 14.25 (day after) [1st para discussing song omitted] Your photo.. well, one is in a boarding school, the other one was abroad for a year, whether you guys are the right ones to “represent the ghetto”, I’m not sure. Don’t read this as a diss against your skills, which you do have, especially since you’ve already played on jams and all.. peace ryke 20. TOC – 14.54 hi… @deep p where do you come from? Pinneberg? Don’t worry, they’ll do you no harm. @ryke sylvestah was one year in england in '98 but apart from that they’re all born in schöneberg and no one was abroad for a longer time (except last year in hawaiiiii) ... and if you still don’t dig the cover, I’m sorry for you. 21. Ryke – 15.09 Okay, I know what you mean, you know what I mean. - I also find guns quite flashy and all... but it’s not especially innovative or self-ironic, especially as far as berlin is concerned. peace ’
Another instance of verbal aggression towards the critics is post 20. Here, TOC’s reply to Deep P (post 8) can be read as implying that Deep P, living in the provincial town of Pinneberg, is not familiar with the sight of guns the way Berlin youngsters are. In the second part of his post, TOC responds to Ryke (post 19), who is positive towards the band’s music, but doubts their legitimacy to represent criminal ghetto life. However, Ryke mitigates his critique by acknowledging the band’s skills and by closing with a signal of community solidarity, i.e. peace. In the last clause of 20, TOC seems to imply that the band’s photo should not be taken at face value, an argument he takes up in post 24. Ryke partly aligns with this in post 21. His statement (original wording: ich finde guns ja auch ganz flashig und so) seems to position guns as part of an impressive staging, a visual decoration without any correspondence to lived experience.
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
305
(10) Board discussion (continued) 22. Frank – 15.56 In gewisser Weise ist es clever jede Art von Kritik mit dem Vorwurf des „Ihr Hated Nur“ [Titel des Hecklah & Coch-Songs, J.A.] abzutun. Sehr einfaches, beneidenswert unkompliziertes Weltbild. Nur beschränkt man sich dadurch letztendlich selbst. Aber das fühlen Hecklah & Coch wohl nicht. Ein Quentchen übertriebener und es wäre beinahe hunmorvoll. So bleibt es, in meinen Augen, ein vergeblicher Profilierungsversuch der unterhalb der Mittelmäßigkeit rangiert. Der Beat fügt sich gut in diese Szenerie. 23. P-A – 16.08 blub blub blub. 24. TOC – 17.28 blub blub und abgesoffen! das man in alles immer so viel interpretieren kann. der track definiert sich ganz einfach. tighter beat, strophen geschrieben, aufgenommen. is doch nur RAP man, RAP !!!!!! kritik is cool. aber Deeuutschläänd (Mr.Banjo said so) hört auf mit pseudo intellektuellen phrasen irgendwelche tracks zu analysieren. feier doch einfach den track. (@frisbee) @ryke HAALLLOOOO H.E.C.K.L.A.H. & C.O.C.H - 99% Treffsicherheit - deutsche parade waffe - FlexRap -------------GUNZ auf dem Cover!!!! 25. TOC – 17.39 [21-word post offering web links omitted] 26. Frank – 17.43 Was gibts da zu feiern TOC ? Ich finde diesen Track langweilig. Wenn Du Dich über unreflektierten Konsum freuen kannst, dann wünsche ich Dir viel Vergnügen. Ich kanns nicht. Hilfe. Ich bin Student. 27. DownTown – 22.36 [11-word post praising the band’s style omitted] 28. Willy – 22.56 [38-word post praising band’s music and criticizing its photo omitted]
306
Jannis Androutsopoulos
29. Mark – 08.31 Frank schrieb am (DATUM): [ fullquote of (22) ] geh nach hause und höhr fanta4 ‘22. Frank – 15.56 In a certain way it is clever to block any form of critique with the reproach of “You’re just Hating” [title of a Hecklah & Coch song J.A.]. A quite simple, enviably uncomplicated worldview. But at the end of the day this amounts to a selfrestriction, though Hecklah & Coch do not seem to feel this. A bit more exaggerated, and it would be almost humorous. But this way it remains, in my view, an unavailing attempt to gain profile that ranges below average. The beat fits in well in this scenery. 23. P-A – 16.08 blah blah blah 24. TOC – 17.28 blah blah and down it goes! people can interpret so much in everything. the track defines itself quite simply. tight beat, write the stanzas, and record it. It’s just RAP man, RAP !!!!!! critique is cool. but Deeuutschläänd (Mr.Banjo said so) just stop analyzing these tracks with pseudo intellectual phrases. just celebrate the track (@frisbee) @ryke HEELLLOOOO H.E.C.K.L.A.H. & C.O.C.H – 99% marksmanship – German parade gun – FlexRap -------------GUNZ on the cover!!!! 25. TOC – 17.39 [21-word post offering web links omitted] 26. Frank – 17.43 What’s there to celebrate TOC? I find this track boring. If you can enjoy unreflected consumption, then please enjoy yourself. I can’t. Help. I’m a student. 27. DownTown – 22.36 [11-word post praising the band’s style omitted] 28. Willy – 22.56 [38-word post praising band’s music and criticizing its photo omitted]
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
307
29. Mark – 08.31 Frank wrote at DATE: [ fullquote of (22) ] go home and listen to fanta4’
Post 22 introduces a markedly different style. Frank goes beyond the critique expressed so far, and provides an analysis of the artists’ ideology. He attributes to H&C a simplistic worldview and judges their style as not exaggerated enough to be humorous. With seven clauses and 66 words, this post is considerably longer than the thread’s average. It is also syntactically more complex and follows standard German orthography, including noun capitalization. Its most striking features are the absence of hip-hop slang and the abundance of formal vocabulary and collocations such as rangiert unterhalb der Mittelmäßigkeit (‘ranges below average’), vergeblicher Profilierungsversuch (‘unavailing attempt to gain profile’), beneidenswert unkompliziertes (‘enviably uncomplicated’), beinahe humorvoll (‘almost humorous’). This post is written in the third person (except for a subjectivity marker, in my eyes), while most other contributions choose the first and/or second person. Other than previous critics, Frank does not round off his contribution with peace, which contextualizes his lack of orientation to the community. Two immediate responses to Frank come from Berlin residents who are already active in this debate (place of residence is part of the member profile that is displayed together with each post). In the first of them (post 23), P-A disparages Frank’s statement with a condensed evaluation, i.e. blub blub blub (equivalent to ‘blah blah blah’) About one and a half hours later, TOC’s reply follows (post 24). His opener is a repetition and variation of the preceding evaluation,14 thereby demonstrating alignment with P-A. He rejects Frank’s criticism, which he labels pseudo intellectual phrases, and draws attention to what he perceives to be the essence of rap. According to TOC, the (formal) quality of rap sound and lyrics should leave no need for further interpretations. He underscores his stance through a reference to a rap artist he identifies with.15 In the second part of post 24, TOC elaborates his alternative reading of the photo. He seems to suggest that guns can be understood as a visual metaphor, which transfers positive qualities of the German gun brand Heckler & Koch, such as 99% treffsicherheit (‘99% marksmanship’), to the band. He also seems to imply that the gun metaphor is legitimate in the frame of FlexRap (battle rap), a rap genre that focuses on aggressive verbal competition.16
308
Jannis Androutsopoulos
Just a few minutes later, Frank’s reaction (26) keeps in line with his initial stance and style. He rejects TOC’s suggestion to ‘celebrate the track’ and accuses him of ‘unreflected consumption’. He retains the verb final –e twice, the deletion of which is quite common on these boards, as demonstrated above. Frank rounds off his post with a self-labelling as Student (‘university student’). In current German slang, Student is a negative categorization among high school students; it stands for an intellectual outlook without real experience. Frank’s self-labelling evokes this stereotype, perhaps in order to ironically confirm inferences by fellow posters and readers. Entries 27 and 28 (not reproduced here) come in the default board style and re-iterate the controversy discussed above. The author of (27) praises the band’s local style, whereas the author of (28) is positive towards the song but rejects the band’s photo as peinlich (‘embarassing’) and calls the band möchtegern gangsta (‘wannabe gangstas’). Yet another reply to Frank follows in entry 29. Mark, who also comes from Berlin and praised the band earlier in this thread (post 13), quotes Frank’s critique in full, and challenges him to go home and listen to fanta 4, i.e. a highly successful German rap band of the 1990s, which is judged by many as too commercial and soft. As in post 11, the indexical power of this statement draws on the position of the referent within the discursive system of hip-hop. The brevity of this post and the misspelling of hör as höhr (imperative sing. of ‘listen to’) provide a formal contrast to Frank’s critique as well. Overall, the participants’ positions with respect to the band’s photo reveal a dichotomy between ‘critics’, most clearly represented by Dan and Frank, and ‘defenders’, most clearly represented by TOC. The defenders are residents of Berlin and present themselves as fans of Berlin rap, while the critics come from other parts of the country. The critics read the band’s photo as a mere imitation of African-American imagery, which is not rooted in the artists’ local context, i.e. Berlin. To them, the band’s visual style lacks authenticity because it deviates from a widespread maxim of rap discourse, i.e. that rap reflects lived experience. By contrast, the defenders, and in particular TOC, embrace a metaphorical reading of the picture, which is rooted in an equally widespread conceptual metaphor of rap discourse, i.e. RAP IS A WEAPON (cf. Androutsopoulos and Scholz 2002). While many posters from both sides equally draw on resources that are typical for hip-hop board discussions in making their point, in Frank’s contribution this clash of cultural values is articulated with a clash of language styles. Frank draws on a markedly more formal style in producing a more ‘elaborate’ critique, which fans of Berlin rap in turn reject as ‘intellectual babbling’.
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
309
However, Frank’s writing style is not exclusive to a critical perspective on ‘gangsta rap’, just as the defenders’ style is not exclusive to Berlin or to ‘gangsta’ rap fans. Moreover, a look at other postings by the protagonists of this debate suggest that they all style-shift to some extent. What we witness in this thread is how the capital value of linguistic resources in hip-hop discourse is reproduced, and contested, in a situated online interaction. The articulation of stance and style that is most salient in entries 22–26 is rooted in the fundamental ambivalence of spoken and written style in the field (cf. 4.2). Spoken and non-standard writing is a clear, often quite conscious divergence from school norms and mainstream media discourse, which all participants are familiar with. It is a resource for constructing nonmainstream and ‘down to earth’ attitudes; what this involves in every single case depends on local context and the topics at hand (cf. Eckert 2002). By contrast, more professional activities in the field, such as authoring and editing copy text for a large website, capitalize on a language style that is inevitably closer to institutional and mainstream media norms. However, sites of online interaction provide a space in which a writing style traditionally vested with symbolic authority can be devalued and made fun of. The emerging picture is certainly not unknown to sociolinguists, as it basically illustrates how the structural relation of standard and non-standard, as well as the covert prestige of vernacular speech, is reproduced in a new setting.
6.
Conclusion
The aim of this chapter was to demonstrate how the notion of sociolinguistic style can deal with the complexities of language use in CMC. A framework for online style analysis was sketched out, and its tripartite distinction between individual participants, genres, and computer-mediated discourse field was used to describe and interpret linguistic variability on a particular hip-hop website. The findings suggest that sociolinguistic style must be addressed at the intersection of these three levels, which frame and contextualize each other in online interaction. In sum, on the level of the field as a whole, participants are ‘doing hiphop’ by drawing on a small but highly typical list of items across various categories (vocabulary, discourse markers, spelling variation). They capitalize on vernacular English, and additionally draw on visual cues of affiliation. Hip-hop slang, a members’ resource, operates on this global level as
310
Jannis Androutsopoulos
well. Clearly, the prototypical instantiation of hip-hop slang is tied up with genres of online interaction. But hip-hop’s field of online discourse includes numerous other genres beside online talk, some quite close to offline traditions, and others genuinely specific to CMC. Hip-hop on the Web is constituted through non-institutional literacy practices, but these practices are not oblivious to genre differences. Depending on genre, users will follow established conventions, creatively transform them, or draw on different generic models to solve the communicative task at hand. The comparison of artist homepages suggests that the choice of a generic model is part and parcel of online identity design, as it contextualizes individual ambitions and alignments. Discarding a ‘default’ genre style and adopting a different generic model has stylistic significance and is clearly acknowledged as such by participants. Therefore, genre is the level on which online style in a collective and an individual sense is manifested. Finally, the analysis of the online talk suggests that the discussion board provides members with a discursive space to debate the artists’ visual and verbal style, and to construct their own style as community members. Although this approach was developed on data from an arena of youth (sub)culture, its usefulness is not restricted to such an arena. As researchers have repeatedly pointed out, youth and youth culture are particularly suited contexts for gaining sociolinguistic insights that reach beyond youth itself. Developing and negotiating social style plays a crucial role in adolescent identity constructions (cf. Bucholtz 2004; Eckert 2000; and papers in Androutsopoulos and Georgakopoulou (eds.) 2003); but the relevance of style in social life is obviously not restricted to youth. Given that the analytical distinctions developed in this paper are not exclusive to youth-cultural settings on the Web, this framework is capable of being applied to other fields of computer-mediated discourse as well. In conclusion, this chapter has argued for a sociolinguistic perspective on computer-mediated communication. With the social spread of the internet, new forms of community are emerging between real-life social networks and imagined communities. Informal written language is gaining new domains, and variation in written language, in particular spelling, is increasing (cf. Sebba, this volume). Sociolinguistics must address these issues and developments, modifying its tools and concepts to meet new social realities. It seems that the theoretical and analytic notions of sociolinguistics can account for the complexity of language use and variation on the Web, provided we adjust them to the new conditions of communication and community in what Castells (2000) has termed the ‘network society’.
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
311
Notes 1. On genre in the study of sociolinguistic style cf. Irvine (2001); Bauman (2001); on genre and online ethnography cf. Danet (2001). 2. The project “Jugendkulturelle mediale Stile” was carried out from 2000–2004 as part of the research group “Sprachvariation als kommunikative Praxis”, funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG); director Werner Kallmeyer, principal investigator Jannis Androutsopoulos, research assistants Daniel Kraft, Nina Nikolic, Ben Schneider. 3. I draw on Koch and Oesterreicher’s (1985) model of conceptually spoken/written style here, which separates the (phonic or graphic) materialization of discourse from its (spoken or written) conception. This model relates linguistic features to communicative situations by means of two ‘continua’. The communicative situation is modelled on a continuum between nearness and distance, based on criteria such as level of formality, level of spontaneity, monologue/dialogue, synchronous/asynchronous mode, etc. The conception of discourse is modelled on a continuum between spoken and written style, based on features of discourse structure, syntax, lexicon, and phonology/orthography (cf. Androutsopoulos 2000 for an earlier application to media discourse). 4. One of the fullest directories in the field, the mzee.com link project, featured 480 German-speaking websites in spring 2002. This figure rose to 831 sites some 15 months later. The link directory on webbeatz.de listed 263 and 458 German hip-hop sites, respectively. 5. The terms ‘online magazine’ and ‘web portal’ are used interchangeably in the following, as the boundary between them is fuzzy from the participants’ perspective. 6. According to webmaster information, hiphop.de reached 3.5 millions of page impressions per month in the beginning of 2004. 7. Cf. Morgan (2001), contributions in Mitchell (ed.) (2001) and Rap Dictionary (www.rapdict.org). 8. These are 99 types, excluding proper names, but including a few ambiguous forms such as word class membership and language, e.g. sample (verb or noun) and mag (Ger. verb mag or Eng. noun magazine). 9. The sample consists of a total of 54,550 words, divided into 73 reviews (17,400 words), 116 artist homepages (18,650) and 24 board discussions (18,500). It is representative with respect to the total amount of texts in these genres on webbeatz.de at the time of research. 10. Feature (c) has the most tokens (1,440), mainly due to the copula verb ist, followed by (a) (854), (b) (391), (e) (73) and (d) (51). Features occuring less frequently in the sample, and therefore excluded from analysis, are the clitization of es after a personal pronoun (e.g. du es > du’s) and of definite article after preposition (e.g. mit dem > mit’m). Analysis of features (d) and (e) includes
312
11. 12. 13.
14.
15. 16.
Jannis Androutsopoulos only verbs which show variation in the sample. Linguistic constraints were not examined. Although they presumably affect the distribution of the variants to some extent, genre differences are clear enough for the purposes of this paper. The difference between the three genres is statistically significant, χ²: 121.2 (df: 8), p 0.001. This sum excludes member profiles and system-generated data, but includes quotes from other posts. This is a simplified version of the screen display. Additional content such as member information and signatures as well as all features of online layout have been omitted. The time of posting has been retained, as swiftness of response is an important indicator for online interaction. Names of contributors are anonymized throughout. English items in the original are in italics in the glosses. In the German original, TOC’s opener (blub blub und abgesoffen!) is a word play on German blub, i.e. the stem of blubbern (‘to blabber’), which also has the colloquial meaning ‘confused talk’. TOC refers to a song called “Deutschland” by Olli Banjo, and devises the spelling Deeuutschläänd in order to imitate the song’s prosody. Rap Dictionary (www.rapdict.org) defines the hip-hop usage of flex as follows: “To show one’s mettle, flexing one’s muscles, showing one’s arsenal and readiness to put it to use, letting one’s pugilistic prowess be known. To strike someone or to get up in someone’s face for intimidation.” In my data, Flexrap is used as a synonym with Battlerap.
References Androutsopoulos, Jannis 2000 Non-standard spellings in media texts: The case of German fanzines. Journal of Sociolinguistics 4(4), 514–533. 2001 What names reveal about the music style: A study of naming patterns in popular music. In: Németh, Enikö (ed.), Pragmatics in 2000, vol. 2. Antwerp: IPrA, 16–29. 2003 Musikszenen im Netz: Felder, Nutzer, Codes. In: Merkens, Hans and Jürgen Zinnecker (eds.), Jahrbuch Jugendforschung 3. Opladen: Leske + Budrich, 57–82. 2004 Non-native English and sub-cultural identities in media discourse. In: Sandøy, Helge et al. (eds.), Den fleirspråklege utfordringa [The multilingual challenge]. Oslo: Novus, 83–98. Androutsopoulos, Jannis (ed.) 2003 HipHop: Globale Kultur – lokale Praktiken. Bielefeld: Transcript.
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web 2006
313
Sociolinguistics and computer-mediated communication. Special Issue, Journal of Sociolinguistics 10(4). Androutsopoulos, Jannis and Arno Scholz 2002 On the recontextualization of hip-hop in European speech communities: A contrastive analysis of rap lyrics. PhiN – Philologie im Netz 19, 1–42. URL: http://web.fu-berlin.de/phin/phin19/p19t1.htm (accessed 2006-10-20). Androutsopoulos, Jannis and Alexandra Georgakopoulou (eds.) 2003 Discourse Constructions of Youth Identities. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins. Androutsopoulos, Jannis and Evelyn Ziegler 2004 Exploring language variation on the Internet: Regional speech in a chat community. In: Gunnarsson, Britt-Louise et al. (eds.), Language Variation in Europe. Papers from ICLaVE 2. Uppsala: Uppsala University Press, 99–111. Barton, David and Mary Hamilton 1998 Local Literacies. Reading and Writing in One Community. London/New York: Routledge. Bauman, Richard 2001 The ethnography of genre in a Mexican market: Form, function, variation. In: Eckert, Penelope and John Rickford (eds.), Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Oxford: Blackwell, 57–77. 2004 A World of Others’ Voices. Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Intertextuality. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Baym, Nancy K. 2000 Tune In, Log On: Soaps, Fandom, and On-Line Community. Thousand Oaks: Sage. Bell, Allan 1999 Styling the other to define the self: A study in New Zealand identity making. Journal of Sociolinguistics 3(4), 523–541. 2001 Back in style: Reworking audience design. In: Eckert, Penelope and John J. Rickford (eds.), Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Oxford: Blackwell, 139–169. Berns, Jan and Peter Schlobinski 2003 Constructions of identity in German hiphop culture. In: Androutsopoulos, Jannis and Alexandra Georgakopoulou (eds.), Discourse Constructions of Youth Identities. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins, 197–219. Bourdieu, Pierre 1991 Language and Symbolic Power. Oxford: Polity Press.
314
Jannis Androutsopoulos
Bucholtz, Mary 1999 ‘Why be normal?’ Language and identity practices in a community of nerd girls. Language in Society 28, 203–223. 2004 Styles and stereotypes: The linguistic negotiation of identity among Laotian American youth. Pragmatics 14(2/3), 127–147. Castells, Manuel 2000 The Rise of the Network Society, 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell. Chandler, Daniel 1998 Personal home pages and the construction of identities on the Web. URL: http://www.aber.ac.uk/media/Documents/short/webident.html (accessed 2006-10-20). Coupland, Nikolas 2001 Dialect stylization in radio talk. Language in Society 30, 345–375. Crystal, David 2001 Language and the Internet. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Danet, Brenda 2001 Cyberpl@y. Communicating Online. Oxford/New York: Berg. Danet, Brenda and Susan Herring (eds.) 2003 The multilingual Internet. Journal of Computer Mediated Communication 9(1). URL: http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol9/issue1/ (accessed 2006-10-20). Döring, Nicola 2003 Sozialpsychologie des Internet, 2nd ed. Göttingen: Hogrefe. Eckert, Penelope 1997 Why ethnography? In: Kotsinas, Ulla-Britt et al. (eds.), Ungdomsspråk i Norden. Stockholm: MINS 43, 52–62. 2000 Linguistic Variation as Social Practice. The Linguistic Construction of Identity in Belten High. Oxford: Blackwell. 2002 Constructing meaning in sociolinguistic variation. Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Anthropological Association, New Orleans, November 2002. URL: www.stanford.edu/~eckert/AAA02.pdf (accessed 2006-10-20). Eckert, Penelope and John Rickford (eds.) 2001 Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Oxford: Blackwell. Fairclough, Norman 1995 Media Discourse. London: Arnold. Georgakopoulou, Alexandra 1997 Self-presentation and interactional alignments in e-mail discourse: The style- and code switches of Greek messages. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 7(2), 141–164.
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web 2003
315
Computer-mediated communication. In: Verschueren, Jef et al. (eds.), Handbook of Pragmatics. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins, 1– 20. Goffman, Ervin 1981 Forms of Talk. Oxford: Blackwell. Haugen, Jason D. 2003 ‘Unladylike divas’: Language, gender, and female gangsta rappers. Popular Music and Society 26(4), 429–444. Hawisher, Gail E. and C. L. Selfe (eds.) 2000 Global Literacies and the World Wide Web. London/New York: Routledge. Herring, Susan 1999 Interactional coherence in CMC. Journal of Computer Mediated Communication 4(4). URL: http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol4/issue4/herring.htm (accessed 2006-1020). 2003 Gender and power in online communication. In: Holmes, Janet and Miriam Meyerhoff (eds.), The Handbook of Language and Gender. Oxford: Blackwell, 202–228. Herring, Susan (ed.) 1996 Computer-Mediated Communication. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins. Hine, Christine 2000 Virtual Ethnography. London: Sage. Holly, Werner 2002 Klare und normale Sprache als sozialer Stil. Zu Elke Heidenreichs ‘Brigitte’-Kolumnen. In: Keim, Inken and Wilfried Schütte (eds.), Soziale Welten und kommunikative Stile. Tübingen: Narr, 363–378. Irvine, Judith 2001 ‘Style’ as distinctiveness: The culture and ideology of linguistic differentiation. In: Eckert, Penelope and John Rickford (eds.), Style and Sociolinguistic Variation. Oxford: Blackwell, 21–43. Kallmeyer, Werner and Inken Keim 2003 Linguistic variation and the construction of social identity in a German-Turkish setting. In: Androutsopoulos, Jannis and Alexandra Georgakopoulou (eds.), Discourse Constructions of Youth Identities. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins, 29–46. Karlsson, Anna-Malin 2002a Skriftbruk I föröndring. En semiotisk studie av den personliga hemsidan. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell.
316
Jannis Androutsopoulos
2002b
To write a page and colour a text. Concepts and practices of homepage use. In: Coppock, P. (ed.), The Semiotics of Writing. Turnhout: Brepols, 295–310. Keim, Inken and Wilfried Schütte (eds.) 2002 Soziale Welten und kommunikative Stile. Tübingen: Narr. Koch, Peter and Wulf Oesterreicher 1985 Sprache der Nähe – Sprache der Distanz. Mündlichkeit und Schriftlichkeit im Spannungsfeld von Sprachtheorie und Sprachgeschichte. Romanistisches Jahrbuch 36, 15–43. Kress, Gunther 1998 Visual and verbal modes of representation in electronically mediated communication: The potential of new forms of text. In: Snyder, Ilana (ed.), From Page to Screen: Taking Literacy into the Electronic Era. London/New York: Routledge, 53–79. 2003 Literacy in the New Media Age. London: Routledge. Kress, Gunther and Theo van Leeuwen 1996 Reading Images. The Grammar of Visual Design. London: Routledge. Mendoza-Denton, Norma 2001 Language and identity. In: Chambers, Jack K., Peter Trudgill, and Natalie Schilling-Estes (eds.), Handbook of Language Variation and Change. Malden: Blackwell, 475–499. Mitchell, Tony (ed.) 2001 Global Noise. Rap and Hip-Hop outside the USA. Middletown: Wesleyan University Press. Morgan, Marcyliena 2001 ‘Nuthin’ but a G thang’: Grammar and language ideology in hip hop identity. In: Lanehart, Sonja (ed.), Sociocultural and Historical Contexts of African American English. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins, 187–210. Paolillo, John 2001 Language variation on Internet Relay Chat: A social network approach. Journal of Sociolinguistics 5(2), 180–213. Pennycook, Alistair 2003 Global Englishes, Rip Slyme, and performativity. Journal of Sociolinguistics 7(4), 513–533. Preisler, Bent 1999 Functions and forms of English in an European EFL country. In: Bex, Tony and Richard J. Watts (eds.), Standard English. The Widening Debate. London, NY: Routledge, 239–267.
Style online: Doing hip-hop on the German-speaking Web
317
Rampton, Ben 1995 Crossing. Language and Ethnicity among Adolescents. London: Longman. Richardson, Elaine and Sean Lewis 2000 ‘Flippin’ the Script’ / ‘Blowin’ up the Spot’: puttin' hip-hop online in (African) America and South Africa. In: Hawisher, Gail E. and Cynthia L. Selfe (eds.), Global Literacies and the World Wide Web. London/New York: Routledge, 251–276. Schilling-Estes, Natalie 2002 Investigating stylistic variation. In: Chambers, Jack K. et al. (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change. Oxford: Blackwell, 375–401. Schwitalla, Johannes 1997 Gesprochenes Deutsch: Eine Einführung. Berlin: Schmidt. Scollon, Ron 1998 Mediated Discourse as Social Interaction: An Ethnographic Study of News Discourse. London: Longman. Sebba, Mark 2003 Spelling rebellion. In: Androutsopoulos, Jannis and Alexandra Georgakopoulou (eds.), Discourse Constructions of Youth Identities. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins, 151–172. 2003b ‘Will the real impersonator please stand up?’ Language and identity in the Ali G websites. Arbeiten aus Anglistik and Amerikanistik 28(2), 279–304. Siebenhaar, Beat 2006 Code choice and code-switching in Swiss-German Internet Relay Chat rooms. Journal of Sociolinguistics 10(4), 481–506. Snyder, Ilana (ed.) 2002 Silicon Literacies. Communication, Innovation and Education in the Electronic Age. London/New York: Routledge. Storrer, Angelika 2001 Getippte Gespräche oder dialogische Texte? Zur kommunikationstheoretischen Einordnung der Chat-Kommunikation. In: Lehr, Andrea et al. (eds.), Sprache im Alltag. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 439– 465. Streeck, Jürgen 2002 Hip-Hop-Identität. In: Keim, Inken and Wilfried Schütte (eds.), Soziale Welten und kommunikative Stile. Tübingen: Narr, 537–557. Thornton, Sarah 1995 Club Cultures. Music, Media and Subcultural Capital. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Part 3. Identity-work through styling and stylization
Introduction to Part 3 Peter Auer In many papers of the preceding two parts of this book it has been argued that variable grammatical, phonological and discursive features combine into styles, which in turn are socially meaningful, i.e. their use can index a certain social category. Through being linked to membership categories, social styles become relevant for the construction of social identities. However, the social interpretation of styles is not a trivial issue. How can we know which styles are indexes of which social categories? The more discrepancies there are between the relevant social categories of a community and those of the researcher and his or her academic readership, the greater the methodological problem for sociolinguistic research on social styles and identity work in interaction. It requires a detailed ethnography of the relevant field in which the actors display identities through certain linguistic practices. There is, however, another way to demonstrate that a constellation of stylistic features receives a certain social interpretation in a community; it is possible to look at the way speakers themselves stylize each other or third parties in order to portray the stylized person as a member of a certain group, i.e. to categorize him or her. Since the social category which is applied to the stylized person is usually easy to reconstruct from the interactional context in which the stylization takes place, stylizations provide evidence that for the speaker, certain stylistic features are linked to certain social personae. This ‘proof procedure’ is applied in the papers of this third part of our volume. It should be noted, however, that it has obvious limitations. First of all, stylizations by definition involve misrepresentations: the portrayed person is rendered in a streamlined and exaggerated way. Also, the stylistic features used in stylizations are usually only a subset of the features that make up a social style: those that are salient and easily recognized by outsiders, and easy to reproduce. Finally, stylizations rely on stereotypes; these stereotypes may diverge more or less radically from actual identity work. But despite these restrictions, which mandate some kind of caution in the use of stylized material in order to reconstruct identity work, “playing with the voice of the other” (to quote Deppermann’s title)
322
Peter Auer
provides an excellent resource for the investigation of the social meaning of linguistic variation. In Ch. 11, Arnulf Deppermann applies this approach to stylized renderings of young immigrants in conversations among monolingual German adolescents. These are done in a kind of exaggerated ethnolect (sometimes called Kanaksprak in Germany, after a book title of the Turkish-born German writer Feridun Zaimoglu, cf. Zaimoglu 1995) which is ascribed to 2nd and 3rd generation immigrant youth, particularly from Turkey and other Southern European as well as Arabic countries. Obviously, we are dealing with a case of ‘crossing’ here, which is common in Western multi-cultural societies. Deppermann investigates the linguistic features, the sequential organization and the interactional functions of stylized Kanaksprak with a special emphasis on how identities of self and other are projected. He finds three practices in which stylized Kanaksprak is frequent: personal quotations, category-animations and playful assessments. Kanaksprak is meaningful because it is linked to the stereotype of the aggressive and dumb immigrant. However, it is mainly used as a fun-code which sets the ‘key’ of an interaction and is a resource for poetic performances marked by playful competition. It also is a display of youth cultural capital acquired from the media in which stylized Kanaksprak also plays an important role. Mark Sebba’s chapter (Ch. 12) directly links up with this interplay between media representations of ethnolectal styles and their everyday use in youth culture. He examines how identities are constructed through language in an online community devoted to discussing the British comedy character ‘Ali G’, created by the British comedian Sacha Baron Cohen in 1999. The ‘Ali G’ act consists of impersonating a gang leader (of ambiguous ethnicity but based on a black stereotype) complete with stereotypical dress, mannerisms and language. Soon after his advent on television, ‘Ali G’ became the subject of a number of websites frequented mainly by adolescents and devoted to the playful mock-adulation of ‘Ali G’ and the discussion of the show. While ostensibly focussed on ‘Ali G’, these websites are also places of identity construction and contestation for the contributors. In particular, race and ethnic identity emerge as topics in certain discussions. An important part of the fantasy world of these sites is their special language, modelled on that of ‘Ali G’ himself. This language is a mixture of Southern British English and Patois (creole), a linguistic stereotype recognizable to most urban British adolescents. In these computer-mediated texts, it is constructed, mainly by white adolescents with little first-hand
Introduction to Part 3
323
knowledge of creole, through means of grammatical and orthographic deviation from Standard English. As Sebba shows, three distinct phases can be identified in the history of the ‘Ali G’ websites. The first one is involved in creating a specifically ‘Black British’ form of creole, and predates the show itself. The second is the act of identity (performed by ‘Ali G’), whereby this variety is taken up by people from outside the Caribbean community. The third act of identity is the one performed by the contributors to the ‘Ali G’ websites, who take the ‘Ali G’ language and use it together with English as an ingroup marker in their own on-line ‘community’. Since all identities are constructed through computer-mediated texts in this virtual meeting place, participants may leave their everyday identities behind them. Language becomes the essential part of identity-construction and ascription. While Deppermann and Sebba are concerned with ethnic stylizations, Alexandra Georgakopoulou looks at gender stylizations in Ch. 13, i.e. talk about men that was found to resonate in the conversations of four Greek adolescent female ‘best friends’. Georgakopoulou investigates the interactional resources that participants draw upon to refer to, identify, categorize, and, more generally, ‘represent’ men, the local meanings that such modes of reference and identification have, and their consequentiality for gender identity projections (both masculinities and femininities). The portrayal of men’s personae makes use of sets of co-occurring and patterned resources (i.e. nicknames, character assessments, stylizations, category bound knowledge) that have developed over time through the participants’ interactional history. As such, they bear meanings more indexically than referentially, evoking a host of associations that have to do with participants’ lived experiences and a social typology of localities (‘practiced place’). They are recycled and recontextualized. The styles that are more routinely invoked in this context mark men as either babyish (‘soft’, ‘feminine’) or ‘tough’/’hard’. Both are conjured on the basis of indexical linguistic choices and stylings of sterotyped sociolects, including shifts into the local dialect (for the ‘tough’ men). In addition, parody, critical distancing, and performative play are central to the enactment of both social styles. The last two chapters of this part of the book address questions of styling in the performance of narratives, and more particularly in reported dialogues. In Susanne Günthner’s contribution (Ch. 14), it is shown how in transmitting the speech of others, story-tellers animate and stylize others’ characters and thereby cast them into categories (social types) with associ-
324
Peter Auer
ated characteristics, such as foreigners, innocent children, snobs, machos, choleric persons, petty bourgeois, etc. By typifying their animated characters, and by affiliating with or disaffiliating from them, tellers implicitly also construe their own identities. Identity management is shown to essentially rely on the portrayal of ‘otherness’, i.e. identity relies on alterity. Along the same lines, Helga Kotthoff in Ch. 15 investigates mocking stylizations of Germans as old-fashioned and conservative by tellers who thereby display their own identity as that of a liberal and modern person. The data are evening meals among friends in Germany and Switzerland, with academics between 30 and 40 as participants who identify in a broad sense with egalitarian gender politics. Kotthoff looks at how these academics make their liberal stance ‘accountable’ in a humorous way, by overdoing the distinction between themselves and those who adhere to traditional gender roles, through mocking and parody. The stylization of the ‘conservatives’ also includes South German dialects, while the ‘liberal’ tellers present their own words in (colloquial) Standard German.
Reference Zaimoglu, Feridun 1995 Kanak Sprak. 24 Mißtöne vom Rande der Gesellschaft. Berlin: Rotbuch-Verl.
Chapter 11 Playing with the voice of the other: Stylized Kanaksprak in conversations among German adolescents* Arnulf Deppermann 1.
Introduction
Compared to other Western industrial nations, it is only quite recently that Germany has become multi-cultural. It neither has a substantial colonial history, such as France and England, nor was it founded as an immigrant society, such as the USA or Canada. Until the 1960s, Germany essentially was a monolingual society with only regional, that is dialectal, but no ethnically-based linguistic variation. This picture has successively changed since the late 1950s, when the first so-called ‘guest-workers’ came to Germany. Today, in the large cities of Germany, such as Berlin, Frankfurt or Cologne, but also in small towns, immigrants and their descendants of the 2nd and 3rd generation make up more than 20 percent of the population. Apart from cultivating their own native languages, these immigrants have created new varieties of German, starting with the first generation’s so-called Gastarbeiterdeutsch (‘guest-workers’ German’; see Dittmar and Rieck 1977; Hinnenkamp 1982). The linguistic features of this variety were caused by insufficient knowledge of the German language and by interferences from the native tongue. In the first half of the 1990s, however, a new ethnolectal variety of German evolved (see Dirim and Auer 2004: Chapter 1). It is spoken by 2nd and 3rd generation immigrants, mainly young males who have grown up in Germany, but who are oriented towards a ‘ghetto’ identity. They strongly oppose integration into German society and culture, which they consider discriminating and hostile, lacking traditional male values and clearly defined gender-roles. But they refuse to continue their parents’ way of life as well. These speakers have developed a code that has come to be known as Türkendeutsch (Androutsopoulos 2001), Türkenslang (Auer 2003) or Kanaksprak (Zaimo÷lu 1995). Although it includes some Turkish features, it is a variety of German, since German is the main lexi-
326
Arnulf Deppermann
fier language. I will henceforth use the term Kanaksprak, because the group of people who are considered to be the ‘owners’ of this code are not only Turks, but also Romani, Maroccans, Egyptians, etc. Kanaksprak is derived from the social designator Kanacke. In Germany, this is widely used as an abusive term to refer to people who look like foreigners of southern origin. It has, however, been appropriated by so-called Kanacken themselves as a self-categorization which at least partly inverts its negative evaluation (comparable to the ‘black is beautiful’-movement in the U.S.; see Zaimo÷lu 1995). Kanacke strictly is neither an ethnic nor a national category-term. It is predicated upon perceptual features which are taken to index people’s national and ethnic membership, and thus is applied to people with very different ethnic origins and national identites. Moreover, its use is restricted to refer to male adolescents and male young adults. Despite its potentially abusive connotation,1 I will use Kanaksprak as an emic term for analytical concerns. This practice should not be mistaken to mean that I subscribe to its evaluative meaning. Starting in 1995, Kanaksprak increasingly has become an object of stylization in the media, mainly in various comedy-formats (see Androutsopoulos 2001). Consequently, it has become popular among German youngsters to insert fragments of stylized Kanaksprak into their conversations.2 To date, there are some studies of Kanaksprak (Auer 2003; Dirim and Auer 2004; Eksner and Orellana 2005; Füglein 2000; Kallmeyer and Keim 2003b; Keim 2003a and 2007; Tertilt 1996 and 1997) and its representation in the media (Auer 2003; Androutsopoulos 2001; Kotthoff 2004). Auer and Androutsopoulos have gathered interview data on German youngsters’ attitudes towards Kanaksprak and self-reports concerning their use. But to my knowledge, no studies exist that inquire into how Kanaksprak is used by German adolescents.3 This will be the focus of my paper. My study is concerned with a secondary ethnolect, a stylization of the ‘other tongue’ (cf. Coupland 2001; Rampton 1999). It is adequate to speak of ‘stylization’, because the German speakers do not use this code as a substantive enrichment of their basic repertoire (in contrast, e.g., to the use of Turkish as described in Auer and Dirim 2003 and Dirim and Auer 2004). They treat stylized Kanaksprak as a fun-code that is used for very restricted conversational and identity-related concerns. Since in many cases this stylization of Kanaksprak does not rest on direct experience with its speakers, but is modelled upon media representations (which are already stylizations), the code under study can in many cases aptly be called a ‘tertiary ethnolect’ (cf. Auer 2003). While it is often
Playing with the voice of the other
327
impossible to tell exactly how media stylization and direct experience relate to one another, there are some hints in my data to answer this question at least partially (see below). The use of stylized Kanaksprak is a case of language crossing as defined by Rampton (1995, 1998): A group of people uses a code that ‘belongs’ to a different ethnic (or cultural) group. Moreover, stylized Kanaksprak is an appropriation of a minority code by a majority. This is a kind of codeswitching which is in some ways atypical (but see Androutsopoulos 2003, the papers in Rampton 1999 and Keim 2002). The primary aim of my paper is to analyze the use of stylized Kanaksprak in conversations among German adolescents. My approach will mainly be conversation analytic as it focuses on the sequential analysis of the conversational organization and functioning of Kanaksprak. However, it is supported and supplemented by ethnographic data and by recovering intertextual references to media models, which are vital for gaining a fuller understanding of the identity-dimensions of Kanaksprak in German youngsters’ conversations. Section 2 will sketch how Kanaksprak is represented in the media. Section 3 will summarize how the German youngsters under study talk about speakers of Kanaksprak. The main body of analyses will then be devoted to conversational sequences in which the German youngsters use stylized Kanaksprak. I take the conversation analytic view which holds that people’s social identities and the features associated with them are neither invariably fixed nor relevant for just any interaction. Rather, they are locally invoked and flexibly shaped by ways of speaking (see Antaki and Widdicombe 1998). Social identities are associated with co-occurring ways of speaking, nonverbal behaviour, preferences for dressing, status symbols, etc. In sum, these co-occurrences amount to a social style, that is, a holistic configuration which is perceived as belonging together and which represents a common socio-symbolic meaning (see Kallmeyer and Keim 2003a), in our case, the image of the stereotypical speaker of Kanaksprak (see section 3). Like most other practices of crossing, stylized Kanaksprak is a kind of stylization (cf. Coupland 2001) of the other tongue which locally invokes and assesses social identities. Stylized Kanaksprak bears as well on the identity of the speaker as on the identities of the addressee and of the stylized other. So, using stylized Kanaksprak accomplishes various acts of selfand other-positioning at once (see section 4). My data come from a research project that aimed at inquiring into the range of interactional practices by which male adolescents organize their
328
Arnulf Deppermann
peer-group interactions.4 We observed a peer-group of about 20 boys aged 14 to 17 who live in a small town near Frankfurt/Main (Germany). Naturally occurring verbal interactions were tape-recorded in various settings, such as in the local youth center, on bus tours, in restaurants, and on the local skating-ground. Extensive fieldwork included regular participant observation for more than two years. Additionally, we conducted in-depth interviews with the members of the peer-group and with youth workers, the mayor, parents, and further significant others. The central members of the peer-group we studied were of German origin, but it also included an Italian, a Moroccan and a Turkish boy as peripheral members. The boys’ social background was mostly upper working and middle class. In the small town where the study was conducted, only a small percentage of the inhabitants is of immigrant origin (ca. 10% of the inhabitants). In nearby Frankfurt, where the boys went to school rsp. to work, the percentage of immigrants is more than 50%. It was at school and in work settings, but also at local events, such as disco nights and in the youth center, that members of the peer group regularly got into direct contact with youngsters of Turkish, Arabian and Slavic origin. Situations of contact are mostly avoided by both sides. Direct verbal conflicts and physical fights were rare, but happened now and then. While the members of the peer-group sometimes referred to these specific ethnic and national identities, they mostly used the abusive cover-categorizations Kanacken or Hawacks (see above). Individual immigrant members of the peer-group were also sometimes addressed by these terms. This, however, only happened in a playful, but competitive frame, especially in ritual insulting sequences called dissen (from: to disrespect, see Deppermann and Schmidt 2001b).
2.
Stylized Kanaksprak in the media: A fun-code
Stylized Kanaksprak and related varieties have been en vogue in German media since the late 1990s. Comedians such as Kaya Yanar (who is of Turkish-Arabian origin) and Stefan und Erkan have made stylized Kanaksprak popular among German adolescents. I will concentrate on the comedy-duo Mundstuhl, because the dialogues between their characters Dragan and Alder serve as the main media model for the adolescents under study. Mundstuhl are Germans who originate from the same region as the adolescents, i.e. the south of Hessia. The youngsters know the Mundstuhl produc-
Playing with the voice of the other
329
tions, which are available on CD and can also be heard on radio and seen on TV, very well and can even cite some passages by heart. Typical linguistic features of the Mundstuhl-dialogues are (see also Androutsopoulos 2001; Auer 2003; Kotthoff 2004): – phonetics: sometimes an epenthetic vowel is inserted into word-initial clusters of consonants (Schäpäruch instead of Spruch ‘slogan’); lack of umlaut (grunst statt grünst ‘gets green’); reduction of /st/ to /s/ (weiß=u? instead of weißt du? ‘do you know?’); coronalization of /ch/ to /sch/ (isch for ich ‘I’ – this feature is probably borrowed from the Hessian dialect) – prosody: syllable-timed instead of stress-timed prosody – syntax: omission of prepositions and articles; inversion of word order: VS instead of SV as in hab isch gekauft neue BMW (instead of ich habe einen neuen BMW gekauft ‘I bought a new BMW’) – lexis: extremely frequent use of some adjectives and adverbs such as krass (‘gross’), korrekt (‘correct’), konkret (‘concrete’). Their semantics deviates from standard German; they are all used as (positive and negative) evaluative markers; in contrast to primary ethnolectal speakers, there are no non-German lexical items e.g. of Turkish origin – semantics: these adjectives (krass etc.) and other lexical items are almost desemanticized, retaining only a ritual function as code- and identity-markers. Moreover, stylized Kanaksprak is marked by hyperbolic and vulgar expressions used for intensification. Many of them are taken from German youth slang (such as the adverbials voll ‘full’, echt ‘real’, the prefixes scheiß- ‘shit’, arsch- ‘ass’, ober- ‘over’; cf. Androutsopoulos 1998) – phraseology: incorrect, contaminated idiomatic expressions like nicht mehr alle Tassen im Kopf which is a blend of nicht mehr alle Tassen im Schrank (idiomatic like ‘one stick short of a bundle’; literally: ‘no longer all cups in the cupboard’) and nicht richtig im Kopf (literally: ‘not right in the head’) – turn-design: Alder expands most of his turns by the tag weiß=du (‘you know?’); Dragan uses in almost all of his turns the address-term alder (‘oldster’) as a turn-exit device – sequence-organization: ritualized openings/greetings was geht – was geht (‘what’s up?’); highly ritualized, symmetrically paired actions (mainly positive evaluations: konkret – konkret (‘concrete’) sometimes repeated over several turns) and a stereotypical closing du bist krass al-
330
Arnulf Deppermann
der – ich weiß (‘you are gross oldster – I know’); often there is a competition between the two characters about who can tell the more newsworthy and the more extreme story; this results in a competition of bragging and showing off – topics: mobile phones, tuning cars, personal achievement (sports, business) or talk about newly learnt words, songs. Mundstuhl ridicule the purportedly prototypical Kanaksprak-speaker who lives in Germany’s immigrant ‘ghettos’. Indeed, a lot of these linguistic features can also be identified in authentic speech of immigrant adolescents with a ‘ghetto’-background (see Auer 2003; Auer and Dirim 2004).5 However, their frequency and intensity is exaggerated in the comedies, and the linguistic repertoire of the stylized characters is much more restricted. The majority of these features (apart from coronalization, phraseological blends, use of tags and topical choice) are specific to (stylized) Kanaksprak and do not appear in domestic varieties of German. Kanaksprak-speaking comedycharacters are ridiculous because of the contrast between their high aspirations and claims on the one hand and their poor actual performances; one can laugh at them because of gross verbal and reasoning mistakes, absurd and stupid ideas and the repetitiveness of their conversations that are framed as being most important but that carry only minimal meaning. All of these comedy-formats are popular predominantly among youngsters, while adults take less notice of them. Among immigrants, their evaluation is not unanimous. Some of them take them to be offensive, some of them identify themselves with the comedy-characters as role-models, while the majority seems to regard the comedies as inoffensive gags. Public debates on the media also focus on whether such comedies are merely fun, blatant racism or rather a sign of an advanced public reaction to experiences of immigration which are no more subject to taboo or domination by a moral agenda.6
3.
Images of the other: German adolescents’ constructions of the identity of the Kanaksprak speakers
Talk about Kanacken was very common among the peer-group of German youngsters we studied. They repeatedly told jokes and stories about experiences with Kanacken and they assessed locations (such as clubs, restaurants or a foreign town), clothing or music styles with respect to them. Places
Playing with the voice of the other
331
where Kanacken go were avoided or devaluated. ‘You look/talk like a Kanack(e)’ was a severe criticism that could be used to threaten the status of a peer-group member or any other German youngster. Kanacken were generally despised and sometimes feared. But this rejection did not apply to every male adolescent of Arabian or Turkish origin. Some of them were respected and accepted as peers, and the boys also told stories about positive experiences with them. Positive individual ascriptions, however, were not generalized to the generic categories Kanacke/Hawack. The identity-attributes that the German adolescents ascribed to Kanacken are quite similar to those which are peculiar to the speakers of stylized Kanaksprak in media comedies (see Androutsopoulos 2001): – Kanacken use mobile phones, although they do not need them, just in order to show how important and popular they are. – They wear distinctive clothes from specific brands, such as Buffalo shoes and Helly Hanson jackets. – They are aggressive and violent; we recorded several accounts of brawls which were said to have been provoked by Kanacken. They were portrayed as looking for trouble without a cause. The German adolescents were afraid of them and conceded that they had no physical means against the Kanacken. – They perform dismally at school. Intellectual, educational deficits and disadvantages that go along with them, such as bad marks in school, having to repeat classes or difficulties in finding estimated jobs, were welcomed by the Germans boys as compensation and revenge for their own physical inferiority. – Kanacken were often associated with drug-dealing and petty crimes. – They were portrayed as bragging and claiming abilities and moral characteristics they do not live up to, thus as being ridiculous and untrustworthy. The last ascription touches on the role of Kanaksprak for defining the identity of the Kanacken. Their language is judged to be indicative of their character, since its pragmatics, but also its semantic and phonological features attest to identity-attributes that are quite distinctive and subject to devaluation and ridicule.
332
4.
Arnulf Deppermann
German adolescents’ use of stylized Kanaksprak
My observations rest on 23 sequences of stylized Kanaksprak which I could find in my corpus of about 30 hours of audio-recordings of adolescents’ conversations. The following typology covers all instances in my corpus. One use of stylized Kanaksprak in conversation consisted in quoting Kanaksprak speakers. We can discern two different kinds of animating them in conversation: Personal quotations (4.1) and a practice which I will call ‘category-animation’ (4.2). The overwhelming quantity of conversational uses of Kanaksprak in my data, however, consisted in playful assessments (4.3). I will discuss the three practices taking into account the following aspects: – In which conversational contexts is stylized Kanaksprak used? – What are the linguistic properties of stylized Kanaksprak (including turn design)? – How is code-alternation between the conversational base-code and stylized Kanaksprak managed? – How are sequences of stylized Kanaksprak organized internally? – What are the semantic and interactional functions of stylized Kanaksprak? – What are the local identities of self and Kanaksprak-speakers that are constructed in these sequences? 4.1. Quotations By a quotation, I mean an instance of stylized Kanaksprak that is framed as the rendering of the speech of a specific person. Here is an example. The German adolescents talk about Turkish boys who live in their region. Denis refers to Hawacks who are supposed to be in Knut’s class, but Knut does not manage to understand whom Denis refers to (lines 03, 05). Denis now uses a quotation of a Hawack, in order to provide identification (lines 08–09). (1) der spast (Juk 17) 01 Denis: du hast ja auch (-) 02 ‘with you in your group ay, (-)’
Playing with the voice of the other 03 Knut: 04 Denis: 05 Knut: 06 07 Bernd: 08 Denis:
09
10 11 Frank: 12 Knut:
333
ja wer bei mIr? ‘well who with mE?’ =äh also in de kl:- (.) in de klAsse, (-) ‘=ah I mean in the cl:- (.) in the clAss, (.)’ isch hab kein: hAwack‘I have no: 'hAwack-'’ (1,0) [höhö-] [‘haha-’] [kenns]te der eine spAst der immer so ‘[dya k]now the crAnk who always speaks so strange’ ‘HOY MAte hoy MAte oy grĻOss oy,’ hh. he. (-) (-) ‘is real ĹUldra the guy y=knOw. (-)’ wEn meinst=en dU? (1,0) ‘whO d=you mEAn? (1,0)’
Up to line 08 the youngsters use their preferred we-code. This is a more or less dialectal variety of colloquial German interspersed with youth slang. It is the code that is mostly used in informal leisure-time conversations among the adolescents when adults are not present. In line 09, Denis quotes a Hawack that allegedly attends Knut’s class: HÖY Aldär höy AOldär öy krĻAss öy. The quotation is most prominently set off from previous talk: Denis not only frames the quotation metapragmatically (see line 08), it is also the dramatic change of his voice that marks the switch. Denis speaks in a choked and rasping voice, which gets increasingly lower until it reaches almost the lower extreme of Denis’ intonational range. The articulation sounds imprecise: the vowels are realized by a backward move of the tongue (velar [Ĵ] instead of [a]; [є] instead of [ȳ]); there is a lenisation of the fortis-plosive [t] to [d], and the /r/ is pronounced [Ӱ]. The lexis consists of items that are ‘code-markers’ for Kanaksprak: the words krass and alder, together with a few others (such as korrekt, konkret, see the next examples), form a repertoire of lexical items speakers of Kanaksprak are assumed to use in nearly every turn. The quotation has no syntactic structure; it consists of the attention-getter or intensifier ey, the tag alder and the
334
Arnulf Deppermann
evaluative adjective krass. The extreme assessment contrasts with almost complete lack of propositional content – there is no recognizable referent (for a summary of features of stylized Kanaksprak in my data see Table 1). Table 1. Linguistic features defining stylized Kanaksprak in German youngsters’ conversations Phonetics/Pronunciation
Phonology/Voice quality
Grammar Syntax and turn-design
Lexis
Semantics
Phraseology
apical [r]; reduction of /ts/ Æ /s/; coronalisation of [%] Æ [5]; lenisation of fortis-stops ([t] Æ [d]; [k] Æ [I]); vowels pronounced with a backward move of the tongue: closed Æ open vowels; (imprecise articulation) frequent elongation of vowels; (choked voice), (rasping voice), (rumbling, scanning rhythm), ((slow and sluggish)) overgeneralized use of den as pronoun/article; wrong agreement excessive use of tags; inversion of the word order of main clauses: VS instead of SV, violation of the German Verbklammer; often lack of syntactic structure (one-word sentences) excessive use of stereotypical code-markers krass, korrekt, konkret, alder; Turkish lexis, such as lan, tam, tschai, tschi, tschucki semantic widening of evaluative adjectives; use of upgrading prefixes such as ultra as evaluative adjectives abusive slogans/ritual sayings (threats, insults)
Features that are only typical of quotations and category-animations are represented in brackets; double brackets indicate idiosyncratic variants.
Frank aligns with Denis’ code-switch (line 11). He also uses stylized Kanaksprak (see next section for the analysis), but changes the perspective: He assumes the voice of an anonymous Kanacke who admires the character that Denis has quoted. Frank thus agrees with the upshot (cf. Heritage and Watson 1979) of Denis’ quotation which is designed to characterize the quoted speaker’s identity. In this sequence, we can see how stylized Kanaksprak is used in order to ascribe identities to self and other by a layering of voices (cf. Günthner
Playing with the voice of the other
335
1999, 2002; see also Bakhtin 1981; Rampton 1995, 1998). In order to describe the different identities that are involved, I will use the ‘positioning’theory (Bamberg 1997; Harré and van Langenhove 1999; Korobov 2001; Lucius-Hoene and Deppermann 2002) which in my view is most suited to capture the different levels and referents of local identity-constructions in discourse (see Table 2). Table 2. Layers of positioning 1st layer
other-positioning
representation of the self-positioning of the Kanacken
2nd layer
attitude toward other-positioning
attitude toward the 1st layer
3rd layer
representational self-positioning
versus represented other (= Kanacken)
4th layer
interactional self-positioning of * I: as individual * we: as peer-group as Germans as media-experts
versus co-interactant
In line 11, Frank shows his agreement with Denis’ other-positioning of the Kanacke in Knut’s form by ironically formulating the social identity that the quoted speaker is said to claim (cf. Kotthoff 2002): It is the identity of a strong and very macho male who demands attention and issues apodictic statements. But this other-positioning is only a first layer. A second layer is the attitude that the speaker assumes towards the other. The social identity that the fictitious Kanacke claims for himself is contested and ridiculed. This already becomes apparent by the metapragmatic framing of the quotation, where the quoted speaker is called a spast (line 08).7 Spast is an abusive youth slang-term derived from Spastiker (denoting ‘disabled persons suffering from spastic paralysis’). Spast is used in order to attribute intellectual deficits and social incompetence. The spast’s way of speaking is characterized as komisch labern (‘speaking strange’), which is a derogatory
336
Arnulf Deppermann
verbum dicendi. This attribution is further elaborated on by the quotation itself: The quoted speaker is despised as a braggart, who in reality is extremely stupid and incompetent. Especially Denis’ imprecise pronunciation contextualizes his contempt against the spast’s lack of civilization and selfcontrol (see above). In this sequence, Kanaksprak is judged as a low prestige variety that attests to a lack of cultural capital (Bourdieu 1983). Paralleling Bourdieu’s argument concerning the lacking prestige of dialects, it seems to be mainly the phonological features of stylized Kanaksprak that are linked to an uncivilized habitus (Bourdieu 1982). Language is not just one contingent feature among others that belongs to a Kanacke – the linguistic and pragmatic properties of Kanaksprak are regarded as features that have intrinsic sociosymbolic values and that are central in order to define the speakers’ identities (cf. Kallmeyer and Keim 1994; Kallmeyer 1995). They express ‘cultural rich points’ (Coupland 1996; here: central dimensions of identity) and can be used to identify speakers. A third layer of self-positioning can now be seen as being implicitly contextualized by the other-positioning and the attitude towards it: The speakers claim higher status for themselves. Their language contrastively is framed as attesting to more intelligence, a higher degree of civilization and verbal skill. In Rampton’s terminology (Rampton 1995: 300 and 1998), the boys switch to stylized Kanaksprak ironically, performing a ‘vari-directional double-voicing’, in which the (allegedly) original intention of the Kanaksprak-character is subverted, ironicized and mockingly held against him.8 4.2. Category-animations Frank’s turn is=hald ĹUldra den kerle wEIß=u, (line 11) is spoken in a footing (Goffman 1981) that is not his own. It is not a personal quotation, but an instance of a practice I will call ‘category-animation’ (sensu Goffman 1981: 143; see also Levinson 1988). By this I understand cases in which an utterance is framed as indexing some category of persons. The speaker does not claim to report something that has really been said (cf. Hartung 2002: 99). Sometimes the speaker does not even pretend that he refers to a specific person at all. Instead, category animations represent ways of speaking that are regarded as most typical and at times even constitutive of the category Kanacke itself.9 This indexicality concerns the (linguistic) form of the utterance as well as its content and its pragmatics (speech acts, claims to identity).10 I use the term ‘category-indexical’ in-
Playing with the voice of the other
337
stead of the well-known ‘category-bound’ (see Sacks 1972; Jayyusi 1984) in order to stress that the ways of speaking in question are not only framed as being typical of Kanaksprak; rather, they are used as devices to identify the animated character unambiguously as a Kanacke.11 Frank’s turn: is=hald ĹUldra den kerle wEIß=u is a clear case of a category-animation as he assumes the voice of an anonymous speaker of Kanaksprak. Frank partly uses the same linguistic properties as Denis in line 10 (e.g. lenisation, inarticulate speech, an extreme, but propositionally empty assessment (uldra), tag – here: weiss=u), and some additional features which are assumed to be typical of Kanaksprak: he reduces the consonant cluster /st/ to /s/ (weiß=u instead of weißt du), he talks slowly and sluggishly, he inverts the order of subject and verb (VS instead of SV: is uldra den kerle instead of der kerl ist ultra), the standard German pronoun der is replaced by den, and he uses the prefix uldra as an adjective. In this case the category had already been established in the previous turn. In the next example the category-animation serves to construct a theyidentity. Bernd and Wuddi are passing a house where Romani people live. They do not know these people, but they know that it is the home of Kanacken. Pointing to the house, Bernd sings: (2) isch fig disch (Juk 9) 01 Bernd: lümm> (-) kanagge [näd da:? ] ((singing tone)) (-) ‘'kanak' [not the:re?]’ 02 Wuddi: [ korRE:KT- (.) ‘I: give you corRE:CT- (.)’ 04 Wuddi: trEt dir in die frEsse; [(.) LAN; ] ‘kIck you in the fAce; [(.) MAN; ]’ 05 Bernd: [aha ha HA-] ‘[uhu hu HU-]’
In the beginning, Bernd makes a non-lexical, singing noise12 which establishes an imitation-frame: [y]-sounds are considered as code-markers for Turkish among German youngsters. Bernd’s question about the whereabouts of the Kanacken (line 01) is some kind of stylized Gastarbeiterdeutsch rather than stylized Kanaksprak: Not only the article, but also the verb is missing. Wuddi answers with a category-animation (lines 02–04). In his first turn-constructional unit (line 02: I:sch fI:g dI:sch lA:n;), he pro-
338
Arnulf Deppermann
duces a scanning rhythm. It represents the rumbling sound that Germans often attribute to Kanaksprak. Wuddi uses also lenisation and speaks very slowly with elongated vowels ([ʪ] instead of [k], [fiڴʪ] instead of [fik]). As a tag, he twice uses the Turkish word lan, meaning ‘young man’.13 Probably, this is the most widely known Turkish word among German youngsters, and it is regularly used as a social categorization for Kanaksprak-speakers. The phraseologism I:sch gib dir korre:kt (line 03) does not only contain the code-marker korrekt with its characteristic apical pronunciation. It is a ritualized saying, meaning that the speaker announces that he will defeat, and maybe do harm to, the addressee.14 The three turn-constructional units have very similar structures: they are about the same length and share a common rhythm, each of them contains a sentence, and they all perform a ritualized threat. This repetitive structuring evidences the speaker’s stylistic orientation to a poetic performance (cf. Bauman and Briggs 1990; Hymes 1996). The poetic features themselves reflexively index locally relevant identities. In contrast to the first example, this category-animation is semantically dense – it is not only a vignette of a communicative habitus, but it also denotes values and action preferences of the fictitious, animated speaker. All three phrases are ritualized threats that position their author as someone who is physically strong and dangerous. Since there is no justifying context, this practice is contextualized as violent, obscene and looking for trouble without a cause. Especially the threat to ‘fuck’ the opponent is well known from Turkish verbal duelling: the attacker announces that he will make his opponent a victim of sadistic homosexual practices, thus depriving him of his sexual reputation as a male.15 While the positioning of the other in this case is quite easy to see, self-positioning is not so clear: Does the animator position himself as being afraid of becoming a potential victim? Does he discredit the threatening behaviour as a ridiculous bluff and see himself as superior? Does he symbolically take revenge for defeats and a perceived disadvantage by exposing Kanacken to mockery? As in the previous example, there are again implicit claims to a more civilized status and higher verbal and intellectual competence. Wuddi’s self-positioning towards the addressee Bernd, however, is easy to be seen: He displays the rhetorical skills of a spontaneous artful verbal performance. Bernd’s laughter acclaims this and makes it an interactional success. Quotations and category-animations are primary ways to characterize Kanacken. This does not only highlight the fact that Kanaksprak itself and the actions performed with it are defining characteristics of Kanacken. Quoting and mimicry can also be seen as a rhetorical resource that is de-
Playing with the voice of the other
339
signed to convey identity-ascriptions in an implicit, but nevertheless effective way which is much better protected from criticism than any explicit propositional statement about Kanacken would be (cf. Günthner 2002). While the latter could be challenged, the inferences drawn from mimicry can be rejected as not intended. The same rhetorical advantage applies to the claims to the speaker’s own identity, such as being more civilized, more verbally competent and intellectually superior to the Kanacken. 4.3. Playful assessments The overwhelming number of stylized uses of Kanaksprak in my corpus are instances of a practice I call ‘playful assessments’. Playful assessments are evaluations that are contextualized as being unserious, jocular remarks. Stylized Kanaksprak is thus mostly used as a fun-code that defines the key of the ongoing interaction: it is framed as entertainment. This will become apparent in the next sequence. The boys are standing at a ski-lift and are talking about girls they have just seen. Frank and Denis disagree on which girl is most attractive (lines 01–08). In line 11, Denis switches into stylized Kanaksprak, and Bernd and Frank align with the code-switch: (3) blond ultrakrass (Juk 17) 01 Frank: hey die blond knut, (.) ‘ay the blond one knut, (.)’ des war en traum oder? (.) 02 ‘wasn't she a dream? (.)’ 03 Denis: (.) ‘the other, (.)’ die- (-) ziemisch 04 ‘the- (.) quite’ 05 ‘BRĹOWN ONE, (.) she was FUCKin A ey.’ 06 Frank: och komm- (.) ‘now come on- (.)’ die blond war- (-) 07 ‘the blond was- (.)’ ä BRETT war des e [geRÄ::T (.) 08 ‘a HIT was this a [thI:ng (.)’ 09 [((incomprehensible)) (---)
340
Arnulf Deppermann
10 Bernd: ja, (.) ‘yes, (.)’ schieb ma=n a:rsch wieder hoch, (---) 11 ‘move your a:ss up again, (.)’ 12 Denis: (.) 13 ‘(.) supergross. (.)’ 14 Bernd: is ultraKRASS. (.) ‘is ultragross. (.)’ 15 Frank: (.)] ‘[year, (.) ay mate.> (.)]’ ey::, ] total TA::M. (.) 19 Denis: [TA::M ‘[ri::ght ay::, ] totally RI::GHT. (.)’ (ha:h.)] 20 [ta::m ‘[ri::ght (hu:h.)]’ TSCHU ]cki aldär. (.)
21 Frank: [ta:m ‘[ri:ght FINE] mate. (.) fi:ne;’ 22 Wuddi: mayer- (-) mein bein mal- (-) an deinem- (.) ‘mayer- (-) my leg just- (-) past your- (.) fuß da vorbei dass isch (nacken) könnte. 23 foot that I could ( ).’ 24 ((door of the car is closed)) 25 Bernd: vierzsch mack kost=es. (-) ‘forty marks is this. (.)’ altär.> 26 Frank: tschucki, (.) 06 Denis: Alder, (.) ‘Oldster, (.) hm?’ 07 (4.0) 08 Frank: ((exhales smoke and points to the microphone)) 09 (-) ‘conCRE::TE- (-)’ 23 Frank: ich hätt jetzt mal lust auf ne BLUE 20;> ‘I just would like to have a BLUE now;’ 22 Denis: ĬĮ ıİ įİȚ șĮ IJȠȞ įİȚȢ< șĮ ıȠȣ ʌİȚ ʌȫ:Ȣ Įʌȩ įȦ! ‘He’ll see you you’ll see him he’ll say what brings you here?’ ȁȠȚʌȩ:Ȟ șĮ IJȠȣ ʌȦ >ʌȐȦ ȞĮ ʌȐȡȦ țȐIJȚ Įʌȩ IJȠ ȋȩȞIJȠ< șĮ ʌȚȐıȠȣȝİ IJȘȞ țȠȣȕȑȞIJĮ ȖİȞȚțȫȢ țĮȜȐ șĮ IJȠȣ ʌȦ (.) İıȪ ȩIJĮȞ IJİȜİȚȫȞİȚȢ Įʌȩ įȦ įİȞ ʌĮȢ ʌȠȣșİȞȐ? ((ȤĮ ȤĮ)) ı' ȑȤİȚ ijȐİȚ IJȠ ȖĮȜĮțIJȠȝʌȠȪȡİțȠ= ‘So I’ll tell him I’m just popping to Hondo’s to get something we’ll strike up a conversation about this and that then I’ll say you going somewhere when you finish here heh huh or are you too preoccupied with the milk pies?’ ȤĮ ȤĮ ǻİȞ șİȢ ȞĮ ʌȐȝİ ȖȚĮ țȡİȝȠȪ:ȜİȢ ‘heh huh don’t you want to go for a crème brû:lée?’
404
Alexandra Georgakopoulou
Before line 37, Tonia and Vivi negotiate the time and place of the projected meeting with “Carnation”. It is only when the character is located by Vivi in the familiar surroundings of his family business (Kanata’s, the name of the patisserie) and when the meeting is projected there that Tonia seems to agree with the time and space co-ordinates of the projected events (39). The agreed on place of the meeting immediately invokes category bound activities which are drawn upon humorously by Tonia as part of her plotting the meeting. The dairy product (milk-pies) is mentioned jokingly as the character’s preoccupation (line 44), as perhaps one that Tonia will try to take the character away from for a social outing. Vivi (line 45) responds with another joking reference to a dairy product (crème brûlée). In addition to colluding with Tonia in reaffirming shared images about the character, Vivi formulates a suggestion for a date on its basis which is a twist of the theme of “let’s go out for a drink”. By dislocating the character’s activities from the patisserie and relocating them in the incongruous context of a date, new associations are momentarily created and evoked.4 Deciding on the plot of stories ultimately hinges on the participants’ joint locating of men in time and place; working around their sports playing or watching activities; considering the implications of meeting them in one hang-out as opposed to another; debating over and rehearsing the lines appropriate for one meeting place as opposed to another. Styling men Stylizations of men more often than not draw on iterative, quotable fragments of language (see Coupland 2001: 345). This is where the recontextualization of shared sources in the group comes into its own. Every talked about man has developed in the group’s conversations a recognizable voice that is time and again discursively re-enacted and, through such reenactments, increasingly stylized. Men’s stylized voices are invariably traceable to quotations from shared stories. For stylization to work, the language in which the voice is performed has to deviate from that of the current speaking context. Stereotyped and exaggerated renderings of other voices are a necessary ingredient here. The participants have a wide range of social varieties as well as French and English at their disposal. Of those, they mostly opt for a) the local dialect, frequently mixed with elements of other regional dialects, b) mangika (a sociolect based on slang and historically associated with marginalized groups of men), c) baby-talk, and d) katharevousa (lit. pure; a formal variety of Greek that originates in its long
Positioning in style: Men in women’s jointly produced stories
405
history of diglossia, which was officially abolished in 1976). What they borrow from those sociolects is the pronunciation (particularly regarding a– c above) and a specific (limited in repertoire) lexis. However, the actual instances of stylizations are made up of quotations that have some kind of meaning for the group and are rooted in the group’s shared interactional history. In this sense, culturally familiar codes blend in with and are mediated by micro-culturally shared codes. A case in point is the colloquial phrase se pao (‘I fancy you’). The group frequently stylizes its (Northern Greek) dialectal form se pau (note the raising of the unstressed mid-vowel /o/ to /u/). This stylization originates in a quotation from a shared story line involving a truck-driver who made an unsuccessful pass at one of the participants. The use of the dialectal form combines associations of lack of sophistication and unfortunate chat-up lines. It thus stands for a male social type, frequently called by the participants as vlachos (‘peasant’).5 In this case, the use of a regional (and, in effect, social) dialect “becomes imagined as connected with focal individuals and scenes, or with characteristic activities and ways of being” (Irvine 2001: 31).6 In turn, those connections or associations “become available as a frame of reference within which speakers create performances and within which audiences interpret them” (idem). Mila tu re, mila tu (‘Talk to him man, talk to him’) is another quotable fragment, partly overlapping in connotations with se pau, as it is frequently used to stylize the voice of inarticulate men. It nonetheless also indexes men’s awkwardness around women and shyness, in the general sense outlined above. In the story from where the phrase was originally extracted, it served as the punchline and was addressed (by one of his friends) to Mikes, a male character that epitomizes lack of communication skills and sociability in the group. As in the case of other positioning cues, what is notable about stylized phrases like Mila tu re, mila tu is the reflexivity, meta-awareness and knowing allusiveness that is involved in their recycling (cf. Coupland 2001). Their use brings up what is known and familiar thus inviting the participants to look for an understanding of what is said beyond the encounter on hand. Tellingly, all of the stylizations in the data are done laughingly and playfully and immediately responded to as such: there is shared enjoyment in the acknowledgement and reaffirmation of the familier, even if the local use of a stylization may be contested afterwards, as we will see below:
406
Alexandra Georgakopoulou
(3) ȍȡĮȓĮ (…) ȕȡȓıțȦ İįȫ țȐʌȠȣ IJȠ ȂȐțȘ ( ..) ȑIJıȚ? ‘Tell me now … we are talking serious. Okay … I bump into Makis right?’ 22 F: MȚȜȐİȚ Ƞ ȆĮȪȜȠȢ ȝİ IJȘ ǺȚȕȒ İțİȓ, țȚ Ƞ ȂȐțȘȢ İȓȞĮȚ İțİȓ, țĮȚ IJȚ IJȠȣ ȜİȢ, TI TȅȊ ȁEȈ? ‘Pavlos is talking to Vivi, and Makis is there, and what would you tell him, Ǿǹȉ?’ ĺ23 T: Ta țȐȜĮȞIJȡĮ?= ‘The carols?=’ 24 V: =Ta țȐȜĮȞIJȡĮ ‘The carols’ ((laughs)) 25 F: ǵȤȚ IJĮ țȐȜĮȞIJȡĮ ȡİ ʌĮȚįȓ ȝȠȣ, ȐȝĮ ıȠȣ IJȪȤİȚ ʌȡȫIJĮ Įʌ’ ȩȜĮ (..) ȞIJȐȟİȚ? ‘Not the carols man, assuming it’s going to happen (..) right? WHAT do I tell him?’ ĺ26 V: ĬĮ IJȠȣ ȝȚȜȒıİȚȢ ıIJȘ ȖȜȫııĮ IJȠȣ IJȠȣ ʌĮȚįȚȠȪ, ıİ ʌȐ:Ƞȣ ȤĮ ȤĮ ȤĮ ‘You’ll speak to the guy in his language, I fa::ncy you hhh=’ ĺ27 T: ȅȣ ȓįȚȠȣȢ ȤĮ ȤĮ ȤĮ ‘=hhhh It’s me=’ ĺ28 F: Ǿ ȓįjȚĮ, Ș ĭȦIJİȚȞȒ. EȖȫ ıİ ȟȑȡȠȣ:, ȤĮ ȤĮ ȤĮ İıȪ įİ ȝİ ȟȑȡİȚȢ? ‘=It’s me, Fotini. I kno:w you, hhhhh (..) d’you know me:?’ 29 F: ȀĮȚ ʌİȢ ȩIJȚ țȠȚIJȐȦ Įʌȩ įȦ, ʌȦȢ șa IJȠȣ IJȡĮȕȒȟȦ IJȘȞ ʌȡȠıȠȤȒ? ‘And say he’s looking this way, how am I going to draw his attention?’ ĺ30 V: ȤĮ ȤĮ ȤĮ MȓȜĮ IJȠȣ ȡİ: ȝȓȜĮ IJȠȣ ‘hhhhhh Ta:lk to him man, huh talk to him man’ ((personation of Pavlos, allusion to a shared story)) ((They all laugh)) (2.0) 31 T: KaȜȐ: (..) ʌȐȞIJȦȢ Ƞ ȆĮȪȜȠȢ İȓȤİ ʌȠȜȪ ȖȑȜȚȠ ‘Gee (..) Pavlos was so funny’ 32 F: EȁA ȇE( ..) EȁA ȇE, ȑIJıȚ ȑțĮȞİ ȡİ ʌĮȚįȓĮ, IJȠȞ ȟȑȡİȞĮ IJȠ ȤȡȚıIJȚĮȞȩ= ‘COME ON (..) come on you gu:ys, he d-didn’t mean it tha::t way, they were all friends of the guy=’ 21 F:
Makis (nickname Mikes) is the man that Fotini is at that point romantically interested in and the participants are planning a meeting with him. Makis is stylized with regionally marked forms. The first involves a dialectal form (kalandra instead of the standard form kalanda, line 23), as a response to Fotini’s question about her projected verbal interaction with Mikes. The second involves the quotable fragment se pau (line 26), which we discussed
Positioning in style: Men in women’s jointly produced stories
407
above. This comes as an example of “his language”, in other words, it indexes Makis’s language as a regionally marked variety and himself as a peasant. As instances of stylization constitute a temporary breach of the ongoing activity (cf. Rampton 1999), that is, a ludic and playful moment, they tend to generate further stylizations. The uptake of Vivi’s se pau is laughter and further stylizations on the same theme by both of her interlocutors: e.g. the /o/ of o idjos (lit. ‘the same’; ‘it’s me’) and of kser-o (‘know’) in lines 27 and 28 is raised to /u/ (kseru). Furthermore, mila tu re, mila tu indexes similar images, associations, and personality attributes in line 30. Laughter from all three interlocutors is the typical response to the stylizations. However, Fotini, having conformed to the norm of positive uptake, goes on (line 32) to differentiate her position (come on come on you guys) and defends the talked-about character, implying that he was not being unsociable and shy as he is being accused of by her interlocutors (he didn’t mean it that way). The above suggests that not only have repeated performances led to a specific set of stylized phrases but they have also generated a closed set of sequentially immediate responses (e.g. laughter, exact repetition, repetition with variation, further stylization), which can be subsequently followed up by a wide variety of affiliative or disaffiliative moves. Assessing men Positioning men invariably involves an element of assessment (cf. evaluation): past actions and words (in specific time and place) are scrutinized, and, on their basis, future actions gauged, predicted, allowed, and disallowed. It is through a joint process of piecing together events and interpretative viewpoints that the participants decide on who the good men are and equally who should be avoided. Assessments frequently mobilize membership categorization devices. The term γatos (‘cat’) is reserved for older, cunning, and sexually exploitative men; the term aϑoa peristera (lit. ‘innocent pigeon’) is used for men who lie, yet protest too much about their innocence.7 As suggested above, stylizations too index social roles and attributes. Over a period of negotiation and debate in the interactional history of the group, a list of good and bad male personality traits has been agreed on by the participants: lying is close to the top, but being shy, as defined by the participants, is worse.8 These assessments come close to Davies and Harré’s known roles: they tend to be larger roles and attributes that the participants know to hold
408
Alexandra Georgakopoulou
above and beyond local storytelling situations. As such, they can be described as the talked about men’s “transportable” identities that can be at any stage brought about in local contexts and made relevant (Zimmerman 1998). The degree of routinization that accompanies other positioning cues is to be found here too. Talk about Eclairette for instance very frequently generates the phrase Eclairaki kai pali Eclairaki (‘Eclairette again and again’). Recontextualizations of assessments often serve as argumentative devices in the course of jointly constructing stories. In other words, participants invoke them in order to defend their own views and challenge their interlocutors’ viewpoint or version of events. In that role, assessments tend to appear later in the course of storytelling, as the ultimate negotiating chips, when the argumentative use of other positioning cues (e.g. nicknames, stylizations) has failed. A case in point is the long projection story, from which excerpt (3) is taken from. The gradual undermining of Mikes by Tonia and Vivi9 by means of stylizations (the phrase “talk to him man, talk to him man” is brought up 5 times in the course of the storytelling), references to shared stories, and membership categorization devices find a lot of resistance from Fotini. Tellingly, assessments come in towards the end of the story: Mikes is called anti-social by Vivi in line 179 (the story is 206 lines long); the assessment is immediately colluded by Tonia: (4) 179 V: 180
181 F:
Dzȁǹ ȇǼ, įİȞ İȓȞĮȚ țȠȚȞȦȞȚțȩȢ (..) IJİȜİȓĮ ʌĮȪȜĮ= ‘OH COME on, he is not sociable (..) full stop= =ȂĮ IJȚ ȜİȢ (...) ĮȣIJȩȢ? ȉȑȡĮȢ țȠȚȞȦȞȚțȠʌȠȓȘıȘȢ. =You don’t say (…) him? But they don’t come more social than that.’ ǺȚȕȒ, įİȞ ȟȑȡİȚȢ, ȝʌȠȡİȓ ȞĮ IJȠȞ įȦ țĮȚ ȞĮ ȝȘ ȝ’ĮȡȑıİȚ, ȝʌȠȡİȓ ȞĮ ȝİ įİȚ țĮȚ ȞĮ ijȪȖİȚ ȝĮțȡȚȐ, įİȞ ȟȑȡİȚȢ. ‘Vivi, you don’t know, I may not fancy him once I meet him, he might see me an’ run a mile, you ju:st don’t know.’
This assessment is not challenged by Fotini; what her next turn simply shows is that she is still keen on the meeting with Mikes. Typically, assessments are accepted as agreed on and un-contestable, holding above and beyond the local context of the current telling. Assessments frequently involve comparisons between men, which reinforce the group’s agreed positive and negative evaluations of certain per-
Positioning in style: Men in women’s jointly produced stories
409
sonality traits. In the story of Fotini’s meeting with Mikes, Eclairette is towards the end of the story invoked as a more suitable candidate for a date with Fotini than Mikes:
(5) 1 T: 2 V: 3 F: 4 V: 5 T: 6 V:
=ȇİ (.) IJȠ ǼțȜĮȚȡȐțȚ İȓȞĮȚ ıĮijȫ::Ȣ țĮȜȪIJİȡȠ!= ‘=Man (.) Eclairette’s s-so:: much better!=’ ȂĮțȡȐȞ (..) ȝĮțȡȐȞ ‘=By far (..) by far’ ȇİ ǺǿȕȒ ȝȘ ȝȠȣ ȜİȢ ȝĮțȡȐȞ, ʌȑıȝȠȣ ȦȢ ʌȡȠȢ TI:? ‘Don’t by far me VIvi, you tell me on account of WHA:T?’ >ȍȢ ʌȡȠȢ İȝijȐȞȚıȘOn account of looksȍȢ ʌȡȠȢ ȤĮȡĮțIJȒȡĮOn account of character=ȍȢ ʌȡȠȢ IJȘȞ ʌȡȠıȦʌȚțȩIJȘIJĮ=On account of personalityșĮ ȖȓȞȠȣȝİ ıțĮIJȐ!< ‘Don’t even think about it, >we’ll fall out big time! ‘one of them said yeah (.) lyotard is really an epigone.>’ HHH O::H .hhh [↑↓IRRE. ] 45 Klara: ‘hhh oh .hhh [wild]’ 46 Inge: [der nächste] meinte ich sollte ihm mal erKlÄRN, ‘[the next one] wanted me to explain to him,’ was man unter ↑DEkonstruktion ver [steht.] 47 ‘what deconstruction [is.]’ 48 Klara: [hihihi] [‘heehee’] das das wär doch en ↑↓mO::de (.)↑GAG. ‘it was just a silly fashion. ’ ja sind die eigntlich be↑SCHEUert. ne::h.= 50 Klara: ‘yeah are they totally stupid or what.’= ja . 51 Inge: ‘=hm ’ 52 (--) naja. eh: hh ja:hh und ein einer (.) der war echt Ü:bel 53 Inge: drauf. ‘well eh: hh well and one one of them he was really bad.’ (heh?) 54 Klara: (‘hm?’) ( ) hat mich auf franZÖsisch angesprochen. 55 Inge: ( ) ‘he started talking to me in french.’ 56 Klara: [mhm. wollt dich testn.] [‘mhm he wanted to test you.’] prüfungsmäßig)] 57 Inge: [( [( ‘like an exam’)] jaja. = 58 ‘yeahyeah ’=
The construction of otherness in reported dialogues 59
Klara:
60
Inge:
61
62
Klara:
423
=kanns mir leibhaftig VO:R[stelln.] =‘I can really imagine [that.]’ [naja] (-) [‘oh well’] (-) jedenfalls machte er mich auch ↑BLÖD an; ‘in any case he also got on my back;’ von=wegen ‘went like ’ ‘why does it have to be paris’ ↑↓NE::HH. (hast du ) ‘no. (did you )’
Starting in line 39 ff., Inge depicts the details of the interview. By metaphorically referring to the interviewers’ series of questions as a ‘bombardment’, she evokes the image of a war setting, where she is being attacked by her opponents sitting ‘across from her’. She explicitly evaluates the interviewer’s questions as ‘totally stupid’ (line 42), and in order to illustrate this evaluation, she reconstructs their utterances. The first interviewer’s commentary ↑tja (.) lyoTARD is doch eine epi↑GO::ne.>. (‘yeah lyotard is really an epigone’) (line 44) is reproduced in such a way that it is given an arrogant overtone: the mannered articulation of the particle ↑tja contextualizes the arrogance of the quoted character, and the noun epi↑GO::ne. (‘epigone’) is prosodically distorted in such a way that we can detect a “layering of voices” (Bakhtin 1981; Günthner 1997b; Schwitalla 1997): We ‘hear’ the voice of the interviewer but – due to the prosodic stylization – we also ‘hear’ Inge’s evaluation of this utterance as exaggerated, inappropriate, and arrogant. Thus, several voices are superimposed on one utterance: The reported speech of the character blends with the narrator’s negative evaluation (Günthner 1999a, 2000, 2002). The staging of the interview situation invites Klara to communicate her indignation about the interviewer’s behavior. Her affectively marked sign of disapproval HHH O::H .hhh ↑↓IRRE. (‘hhh oh .hhh wild’) displays her co-alignment. The lengthening of the vowel O::H, the marked rise-fall intonation contour and the adjective IRRE (‘wild’) function as indignation markers, i.e. ritualized expressions which the recipients employ at strategically important locations in order to signal affective co-orientation and coindignation (Günthner 2000). In re-animating the second interviewer, we also recognize this “layering of voices” even though it occurs in indirect speech: das das wär doch en ↑↓mO::de (.)↑GAG. (‘it was just a silly fashion. ’) (line 49). The lengthening of the vowel O::,
424
Susanne Günthner
the high onset with the falling glide on ↑↓mO::de and the high onset on ↑GAG communicate a certain condescension. Furthermore, the reported speech is stylized in such a way that it comes close to what Bakhtin (1981: 339) calls “parodistic stylization”, i.e. a “malicious and deliberately parodistic distortion of another’s word, slander”. For Bakhtin, parodistic stylization is the subversive part of a polyphonic utterance: the reporter uses the voice of the other and exploits it for his own purposes (Günthner 1997a, b). Silverstein (1985), who draws on Jakobson’s insights on the metalinguistic function of language (messages about messages), treats reported speech as ‘metapragmatic activity’ par excellence: by quoting past utterances, speakers represent and comment on the use of language. In doing so, they express their stance. Inge’s stance of the interviewer’s attitude towards deconstructivism as ‘a silly fashion’ is supported by Klara's reply in line 50: ja sind die eigntlich be↑SCHEUert. ne::h.= (‘yeah are they totally stupid or what.=’). In the episode at hand, the narrator indicates – by means of indirectly commenting on the reproduced utterance – her stance towards the quoted characters and distances herself from their arrogant habitus. The dramatic staging of the characters’ styles of interviewing invites the recipient, Klara, to communicate her alignment with the reporter’s perspective on the inadequate behavior of the antagonists (lines 45; 48; 50). In line 53, Inge introduces the third interviewer with the explicit, evaluative comment ‘he was really bad’ and refers to the fact that he spoke French. Klara (line 56) as well as Inge (line 57) interpret his code-switching into French as his attempt to construct ‘an exam situation’. By adding the membership category ↑OBERschullehrer (.) typ. (‘the school inspector type’), Inge explicitly introduces social categorization.6 In German, the term Oberschullehrer is ambiguous: On the one hand it refers to a high school teacher, on the other hand, Ober- can be interpreted as an intensification of the category school teacher, i.e. an exaggerated school teacher type. As Bucholtz and Hall (2004: 37) argue, by categorizing someone, the speaker positions him/herself. By labelling the interviewer Oberschullehrer, Inge disaligns herself from this social type and constructs her own identity ex negativo: in stating what “they are”, Inge indirectly expresses what “she is not”. Thus, the construction of alterity serves as a resource for the construction of self. In the following, Inge uses category-bound activities (Sacks 1972: 335) to substantiate the social category of the Oberschullehrer. These are based on the use of French in combination with marked prosody and change in voice quality, i.e. increase in volume, high register and an aggressive, impertinent-sounding voice:
The construction of otherness in reported dialogues
425
(‘why does it have to be paris’). The reporter’s code-switching into French (line 61) is used as a resource to stage the interviewer with the corresponding membership activity (to test the interviewee) and, at the same time, to claim authenticity. This French speaking↑OBERschullehrer (.) typ tops the other two interviewers (in the list of three) by his outrageous communicative behavior. Again, the recipient shows her alignment with the reporter’s stance, and her reaction (↑↓NE::HH. (‘no’)) is oriented towards the staging of the third interviewer’s utterance. In categorizing the interviewers as men who believe themselves to be important (‘terribly self important faces’), who attack the protagonist in a war-like scenario (‘bombarded me’), who are arrogant but who are not academically up-to-date (‘stupid questions’), who do not appreciate or understand deconstructivism and who turn the interview into a school-like testing situation (by even asking questions in French), the narrator implicitly positions herself as someone who is in favor of deconstructivism, who is academically up-to-date and who is more of a true scholar than the ‘school inspector types’ who were interviewing her. The contrast built up between the arrogant interviewers (who are only school teacher types) and the up-to-date candidate (i.e. the narrator) can be understood as justification for the fact that the interview did not go well, that it, in fact, was ‘horrible’. (2) The ‘arrogant, condescending doctors’ In STIPENDIUM (‘SCHOLARSHIP’) the reporter used code-switching into a different language in co-occurrence with prosody, voice quality, and the reconstruction of communicative activities (asking ‘stupid questions’) to symbolize social types and disaffiliate herself from them. In the following transcript LUNGENKREBS (‘LUNG CANCER’), the narrator makes use of code-switching into standard German in combination with particular prosodic and lexico-semantic features to construct otherness.7 Hedda, her niece Ulla and Ulla’s daughter Sara are having coffee, when Hedda tells them about her doctor’s behaviour when her husband had lung cancer. The participants speak in the local Southern German dialect (Swabian): LUNGENKREBS (LUNG CANCER) 32 Hedda: i han dortmols no: (.)zum doktor ↑↓hartmann gsa: (0.8) ‘well I then (.) said to doctor hartmann’ (0.8) 33 i TRAU net. ‘I am sceptical.’
426
Susanne Günthner
34 35 36
37
Sara:
38
Hedda:
39
Sara:
40
Ulla:
41
Hedda:
42
Ulla:
ob des net LUNGEkrebs isch. ‘if it isn't lung cancer.’ no hot der glei gsa ‘then he immediately said’ ACH. DAS könnt auch was ANDERS SEIN.> ‘ah it could also be something else.’> hat der ↑↓gsagt. ‘that's what he said.’ und der hats aber GWIßT. ‘but he knew it.’ haja. FREILE.= ‘yes. of course.’= =haja. [(des )] ‘=of course.[(it )]’ [(der hat)] ja s' ergebnis vom labor ghät. ‘[(he already)] had the results from the laboratory.’
Hedda reconstructs a dialogue-scene between herself and her doctor. In line 34, she quotes the I-protagonist’s worries about her husband’s illness by using the local Swabian dialect and thus the same variety the participants in the reporting world (Ulla, Sara and Hedda) are speaking. However, with the doctor’s reply she switches to standard German (line 36) ACH. DAS könnt auch was ANDERS SEIN.> (‘ ah it could also be something else.>’). This codeswitching stands out from the surrounding context and contrasts not only with the local dialect variety used by the protagonist but also with the local dialect variety of the participants in the reporting world. Thus, heterogeneity within the linguistic system (dialect variety vs. standard German) becomes a resource with which to symbolically express otherness, and to create a separation between ‘us’ and ‘him’. In co-occurrence with the marked prosody (very high register and the sharp voice), this switch to standard German also contextualizes the doctor’s condescension towards his patient. Due to the exaggerated prosodic stylization (by means of a very high register and a mannered articulation), the reporter contextualizes her own evaluation of the doctor’s behavior. Thus, again we can observe a “multi-
The construction of otherness in reported dialogues
427
voiced text” (Bakhtin 1981): the narrator’s voice penetrates the character’s reply and evaluates it. The reconstructed scene between Hedda and the doctor reflects different ways of communicating: The I-protagonist’s concerned inquiry about her husband’s illness is contrasted with the arrogant sounding dismissal of the doctor, who even lies to her. Instead of employing explicit category names (such as ‘arrogant doctors’), the reporter relies mainly on indexical means (such as code-switching into standard German, prosodic features, means of voice quality, the interjection ACH) to signal social meanings and at the same time to contextualize her evaluation of the doctor’s communicative behavior. Thus, what Voloshinov (1978) calls “speech interference”, also happens in everyday reported speech: one utterance can simultaneously belong to two persons (the quoted figure as well as the reporter), it can be anchored in two “worlds” (the story-world and the reporting-world) and it can carry two points of view (the quoted figure’s perspective and the evaluative perspective of the reporter). As in STIPENDIUM (‘SCHOLARSHIP’), we can also observe in LUNGENKREBS (‘LUNG CANCER’) how linguistic stylization in reported speech is closely connected to the social categorization of the animated figures and how code-switching into different languages or varieties in co-occurrence with prosodic means, voice quality, and particular communicative activities are used to construct otherness and to produce patterns of inclusion and exclusion. Instead of a single parameter, reporters make use of co-occurring cues – i.e. of social communicative styles – to typify the animated figures. As the following example will show, social communicative styles and social categorizing are related to shared cultural knowledge and stereotypes concerning social groups. (3) ‘Kanaken (‘wogs’) trying to exploit the German welfare system’ In the preceding examples, reporters used code-switching into French and standard German to stylize individual characters and typify them as members of particular social groups (‘aggressive interviewers’, ‘condescending doctors’). In the following piece of talk (TEPPICHJODEL ‘CARPET SALESMAN’), code-switching into pidgin German is exploited to reconstruct and animate ethnic groups. The transcript segment stems from an interaction between a carpet salesman (T) and Willi and Bea Müller. While at the Müllers’ house trying to sell carpets, the salesman talks about politics. In the following lines he
428
Susanne Günthner
complains about foreigners who come to Germany to exploit the German welfare system: TEPPICHJODEL (CARPET SALESMAN)8 32 T: ne?= ‘right?’= 33 W: =[des stimmt.] =[‘that's right.’] 34 T: [die kanAK]en komm=n hier an, ‘[the wogs] arrive here (in Germany),’ und sagn [ich mein eh eh,] 35 T: ‘and say [I mean eh eh,]’ 36 W: [da hasch schon recht.] [‘you are right about that.’] die sagn, 37 T: ‘they say,’ die ruMÄnen alle (.) nech? 39 ‘all the romanians (.)right?’ 40 41 ja. 42 B: ‘yeah.’ 43 T: 44 JAHAJAHAHA 45 W: ‘YEAHHAYEAHHAHA’ ne: (.) un=un=un=un=und die andern ja AUCH. 46 T: ‘right (.) an=an=an=an=and the others too.’ nech? 47 ‘right?’ die TÜ:Rken und so weider, nech? 48 ‘the turks and so on right?’ jedes jahr KI:ND, 49 ‘every year child,’ 50 ‘a=a=a=a=and good money (children),’> 51
The construction of otherness in reported dialogues 53 54 55
W: T:
429
= = =HAHAHAHA[HAHAJAJA] [ja is] doch WAHR; ‘[well] it really is true;’
T complains about die kanAKen9 (‘the wogs’) who come to Germany to exploit the German welfare system by having many children and receiving child benefits. In using the derogative and even racist term kanAKen (‘wogs’) and in animating their voices in a stylized pidgin German variety with a high pitched falsetto voice, T’s negative stance is apparent: (‘’). In line 39, T explicitly introduces the ethnic category of the Romanians and reanimates their voices speaking as one; i.e. as “choral dialogue” (Tannen 1989: 113f.): Individuals are melted into one social group speaking with one voice. Romanian migrants address other Romanians and encourage them in pidgin German (with a high pitched, choked, falsetto voice) to go to Germany and benefit from the German social system (lines 40–44): (‘ child (.) every year child.>’). Again, we can detect polyphonic strategies: The quoted utterances simultaneously express the (seeming) attitudes of the characters, and the refracted intention of the reporter. The high pitched falsetto voice parodistically distorts the quoted speech. Again, the reporter uses the voice of the other and exploits it for his own purposes (Günthner 1997a, b). W acknowledges T’s performance by laughing out loud (lines 45 and 54). In line 48, T switches to the ethnic category of ‘the Turks and so on’. Without explicitly introducing the reported speech in line 49, the reporter “puts on the voice” (Rampton 1995: 54) of Turkish immigrants addressing Turkish recipients and telling them – again in pidgin German – how to make use of the German social system by collecting child benefits in order to get rich: jedes jahr KI:ND, (‘every year another child,> you rich man.>’). Again, we observe a layering of voices (Bakhtin 1981; Günthner 1999a, 2002; Deppermann in
430
Susanne Günthner
this volume): We hear the animated voices of the foreigners (the Turks and Romanians) who advise other foreigners to go to Germany, exploit the German social system and get rich, but we also hear the reporter’s negative evaluation of the animated figures and their way of thinking. T’s performance invites his recipients to co-align with his evaluation towards immigrants; and W’s laughter in lines 45 and 54 shows his appreciation of T’s performance and his co-alignment. In this example, the reporter T makes use of various membership categories (die kanAKen (‘the wogs’), die ruMÄnen (‘the romanians’), and die TÜ:Rken (‘the turks’)). In order to illustrate category-bound ways of thinking and speaking, he applies specific linguistic resources (morphological, syntactic, and lexico-semantic elements) stereotypically associated with the verbal style of guest-workers (‘Gastarbeiterdeutsch’): – deletion of definite and indefinite articles: KIND (.) jedes jahr KIND (instead of standard German: ein Kind, jedes Jahr ein Kind) – inadequate congruence in complex NP: u=u=u=u=und schöne GELD (kinder-), du du (.) rEIche MANN; (instead of: und schönes Geld, du du reicher Mann) – deletion of prepositions: geh du DEUTSCHland (instead of: gehst du nach Deutschland) – incorrect use of prepositions: zehn jahre du zuRÜCK nach=nach=e (.) türKEI (instead of: nach zehn jahren gehst du zurück in die Türkei) – deletion of verbs: jedes jahr KI:ND, du vier hundert MARK, du du (.) rEIche MANN, zehn jahre du zuRÜCK nach=nach=e (.) türKEI (instead of: jedes Jahr bekommst du ein Kind, du bekommst vier hundert Mark, du du bist ein reicher Mann, nach zehn jahren gehst du zurück in die Türkei) – reduced morphology: wiss=du, BRAUCH, geh du (instead of: willst du, braucht, gehst du). Thus, the subsumption of the animated characters under the social category kanAKen is supported by their category-bound ways of speaking; i.e. the choice of a particular verbal style embodies ethnic characters. Linguistic and communicative choices are portrayed as part of collective actions and wider social processes. The lack of prestige of the staged variety is obvious. The linguistic features of the animated style (pidgin German) have sociosymbolic value and are made relevant for the construction of social groups (Kallmeyer and Keim 1994; Kallmeyer 2002; Deppermann in this volume).
The construction of otherness in reported dialogues
431
(4) The ‘Nazi’ So far, reporters drew on code-switching into various linguistic varieties (French, standard German, pidgin German) in co-occurrence with prosody and voice quality to index social types and to portray otherness. In the next example, DIE NAZIS (‘THE NAZIS’), the reporter uses particular communicative activities and routine formulas – combined with prosodic means and features of voice quality – to animate a quoted figure and portray him as a ‘Nazi’. The segment is taken from an interaction between Hedda and her greatniece Sara. Sara is asking Hedda about the Jews in their village during Hitler’s reign, and whether the villagers had any idea what had happened to them. When Hedda reveals that everyone had known about torture and concentration camps, Sara asks why no one did anything about it: DIE NAZIS (THE NAZIS) ha MÄDle, 112 Hedda: ‘well my girl,’ 113 114 = = 122 = hi hh' hoh hh'> 123 Sara: hot der gmacht 124 Hedda: ‘that's what he did’ 125 Sara: net? (-) 126 Hedda: ‘isn't it?’ (-) 127
432
Susanne Günthner
128 129 130 Sara: 131 Hedda:
hahahahaha [hahahahahahaha [IS DAS TO:::LL hehe [‘IS THAT GREA::T hehe’
‘The woman’ instead of ‘my wife’ mimics the millionaire’s wording and gives the phrase an element of citation. It is a typical formulation in Alemannic German. Maria is astonished, two people laugh. In line 7 Rudolph begins to stage the millionaire’s words directly. The quotation is introduced with a strong evaluation of ‘it was the last straw’ (es war der Hammer).5 Then Rudolph modifies his voice and switches to the Alemannic dialect (bisch wahnsinnig?). He renders the entire speech of the millionaire somewhat more softly and with tense articulation (kannsch net mache), an iconization of the millionaire’s stinginess. Through this alone the millionaire makes a disagreeable impression. He approaches Rudolph with strong
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others
453
warnings (bisch wahnsinnig? kannsch net mache. wennsch dere langweilig isch, got die eikaufe, dann hot die schuh für tausend mark) rendered in Alemannic dialect. Remarkable are the two verbs in the second person and the contraction of the conjunction wenn ‘if’ with the pronoun es ‘it’, which stand syntactically in the first position and which result in a repetitive final SH, a salient pronounciation feature of integral /st/ and /sp/ and of some final /s/ in the Alemannic dialects, such as bisch (7) … kannsch (8) … wennsch (9). Interjections like Menschenskinder (‘good god’) (8) and ha (9) and the very colloquial, familiar form of address Du (‘you’ in line 9 and 10; this is hard to translate into English) are used. He makes the millionaire’s warnings sound very urgent and not very sophisticated. The interjection ha is typical for the Alemannic dialect. Alemannic dialect serves here as a marker of backward attitudes. For Bakhtin (1986: 89), such stylizations are important evidence for his often cited dictum that “our speech... is filled with others’ words, varying degrees of otherness or varying degrees of ‘our-own-ness’, varying degrees of awareness and detachment”. Here the varying assignment of dialect and standard language participates in authorizing social distinctions. Rudolf himself speaks standard German. Contrasting stylizations of social types play an important role here in lending comical twists to situations, as they often do in our everyday conversations. Details are put in the mouth of the millionaire, e.g. that Rudolph’s wife will buy thousand-mark shoes at Rudolph’s expense. Together with the ironic designation millionaire, these procedures help to create a conversational caricature of him as hopelessly prejudiced against women from Eastern Europe, maybe against women in general – and as stingy. With Tannen (1989), Couper-Kuhlen (1998) and Günthner (1996, 1999, this volume), I see reported dialogue as a play with double voicing in the sense of Bakhtin. The persons and situations spoken about are stylized and typified like the characters in a joke. The dialogues are reported with a claim to authenticity but nonetheless extend into the realm of fictitious dramatization (Günthner 1999; Kotthoff 1998, 2002). These implicit typifications of the dramatis personae are easily identifiable for the group members because they are based on shared knowledge about typical speech styles, which is confirmed in this manner. The prejudiced man is not sophisticated and neither is his speech style. The shared morals of what the group considers to be a good or bad attitude are also confirmed. The group is very amused by Rudolph’s stylization. There is long laughter in line 11. In this little nar-
454
Helga Kotthoff
rated scene, the self remains in the background, but we can infer its attitude. A discourse of indignation does not result and is not aimed at. The attitude that the story-teller and his audience communicate with respect to prejudiced people such as the millionaire is one of amused distance. Indeed, the presentation is even judged by Maria in line 12 as “great”. This reception makes it evident that the quality of the performance is appreciated immediately and that it is essential for the shared amusement. In the next story the central figure is also introduced by a label. 3.2. The rapist In Example (2) Maria parodies the way of speaking of the Swiss owner of a bakery. This person is introduced as ‘the rapist,’ without any explanation. In the course of the narrative it becomes obvious that this labeling simply serves as a very negative characterization of a shop owner who is seen very critically by the group. Line 1 already violates normal expectations, since we normally do not associate rapists with cooking recipes. The labeling creates interest in the man introduced so negatively. Only David, Johannes, Ulf and Maria know the man. The example stems from another evening with the two couples David and Katharina and Johannes and Maria. Ulf, a German journalist of 36 years, invited them to his house on the Swiss side of the border, including his friends Anni and Bernada, two Sinologists from Berlin. Johannes and Maria also know Anni and Bernada quite well. Until recently, Johannes shared the house with Ulf. (2) (Conversation 6) Anni (A); Bernada (B); David (D); Johannes (J); Katharina (K); Maria (M); several (m); Ulf (U). 1
M:
die rezepte gibts beim vergewAltiger. ‘the recipes are from the rapist.’ Ihr könntet die mitbringen. (- -) die (? ?) ‘you could bring them with you. (- -) they- (? ?)’ der anni schick ich immer vom vergewaltiger diese ‘i always send Anni these cheesecake recipes from the rapist.’ kÄsküchlirezepte.
A:
wer is denn der vergewAltiger?
2 3
4 5
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others
6 7 7 8 8 9 9 10 10 11 12 12 13 13 14 14 15 15 16
M:
A: A: M: M: D: D: U: U: D: D: M: M:
J:
17 M: 18 D: 19 M: 20
21 A: 22 U:
23 m: 24 D: 25 M: 26 D: 27 M:
455
‘who exactly is the rapist?’ hehehehe (‘hh) ja ich will jetzt nich mehr da ‘hehehehehe (‘hh) well, now I don’t want’ hIngange. ‘to go there any more.’ ach sO. ‘oh, I see.’ (? ?) ↑dIE dinger. (? ?) ↑’these things.’ warum vergewAltiger? ‘why rapist?’ ((noise in the background)) aus wOhlfeilen gründen, würd ich sagen. ‘for good reasons, I would say.’ °ach° °‘ah’° des is son schmIeriger typ, ‘he’s such a greasy type,’ der da unten den laden hat. ‘the one who has the shop down there.’ na. ‘oh.’ ein faschIst. irgendwie AUsländerfeindlich, ‘a fascist. sort of xenophobic,’ der bO:cksberger, ach, dE:n kenn ich auch. ‘Old Bo:cksberger, ah, I know him too.’ wenn du da hingehst, un willst sEmmeli, ‘if you go there and want buns,’ und ‘AND < ((pretentious)) ↑here the buns are called ↑Gipfeli.>’ die hab ich gern gekocht. ‘I liked cooking them.’ WA::S? bi Ü::s heiße die ↑gIpfeli:: (-) ‘WHAT? Here the buns are called ↑Gipfeli:: he[hehe’ [hehehe und der hat so ne gAnz kleine, zarte frau. ‘and he has such a really small, fragile wife.’ ja und die schEIßt der zammen. vor allen. ‘yeah and he bitches at her. in front of everyone.’ vor lEUten. ja ja. ‘in front of people. yeah yeah.’ po::: ‘po:::’
The rapist figure (Vergewaltiger) is introduced quite abruptly without any explanation. Maria informs the group about where they could get copies of
456
Helga Kotthoff
the recipes. The suggestion in line 2 is made to Ulf and his former cohabitant Johannes, who often buy from the “rapist”. In lines 3–4 Ulf is being informed as to why he should bring the recipes from the rapist, which are available free at his bakery. The abrupt introduction of the unusually designated figure evokes a question from Anni (5). But at first Maria laughs and explains that she does not want to go there anymore. Then David also asks about the rapist (10). Ulf seems to know the man and agrees with Maria, using an extremely elevated formulation (aus wohlfeilen6 Gründen, würd ich sagen ‘for good reasons, I would say’), but does not answer the question either (12). In lines 14–15 Maria characterizes the shop owner as a greasy type. The harsh term and negative characterization evoke a critical “well” (na) from Johannes. Then Maria intensifies the negative characterization still more (17). David reveals that he suddenly recognizes the person (Old Bocksberger). Maria then performs a scene in his shop with a direct quotation. She uses the generalized personal address form du. She portrays herself trying to speak Swiss German to him by tagging the Swiss diminutive li to a Bavarian word for buns (producing Semmeli). Herr Bocksberger is cited without introduction (merely a phatically spoken und), uttering a pretentious correction (20); he wants the term for bread rolls to be correct in Swiss-German dialect: Gipfeli. The words of the fascist (Faschist) are spoken louder to iconize Bocksberger’s excitement. Maria, who is a native speaker of Alemannic dialect, is playing here with linguistic knowledge, presenting herself as naïvely mixing Bavarian and Swiss German lexemes like a recent immigrant to the South. For speakers familiar with these dialects, there is a witty effect in attaching a Swiss diminutive ending to a Bavarian word and then pretending to offer it as Swiss German. The diminutive li is one of the most characteristic morphemes in Swiss German. Anni, a guest from Berlin, does not react to this staging, but in line 21 she offers an additional comment on the recipes. In line 22 Ulf repeats and dramatizes the Bocksberger quotation in better Swiss German (bi ü::s heiße die ↑Gipfeli::). He elongates two vowels and produces with Gipfeli the typical Swiss German intonation contour with high onset. He laughs at this himself, which also elicits responsive laughter from others present. David offers more information about Herr Bocksberger (24): he snaps at his wife in front of people. Maria uses this to further negatively characterize him (25, ‘he bitches at her’), which David affirms with a further specification. In line 27 Maria utters an interjection of indignation. Herr Bocksberger is characterized as totally disagreeable. His correction activities are parodied as simply aggressive. In the co-constructed narrative
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others
457
the group agrees on a shared moral attitude towards him and people like him. Husbands who ‘bitch at’ their wives (zammenscheißen) are vehemently condemned. The parody in example (2) is embedded in a characterization which works with exaggerated categorizations of a man (‘fascist’, ‘xenophobic’) and his activities (zammenscheißen). The person is staged in quotations which do not correspond to the categories used for him but help to create a distinction. For the group members it is clear that he never raped anyone, and that uttering harsh words is not enough to qualify someone as a fascist. It seems to be obvious to everybody that realistic characterizations are not at stake here. From the start, the choice of wildly exaggerated characterizations eliminates any claim to realism in the restaging and prepares the audience for a fictitious dramatization. Again, the performance as such is pushed into the foreground, along with subtle irony in regard to the exaggerated moralizing of progressives. The presentation relies on the background knowledge that in certain left-oriented milieus labels like fascist and rapist are used in an inflationary manner. Speakers distance themselves even from their own voices via comic exaggeration. In conclusion, speakers’ high knowledge of typical formulations is exemplified once again. In the example, the narrator Maria uses some dialect features (hingange in line 7 instead of hingehen). She presents herself as accepting and speaking the Alemannic dialect and striving even to manage Swiss German. But Bocksberger’s tolerance is too small to appreciate her endeavor. In southwestern Germany the Alemannic dialect is accepted, but most educated persons with a global professional orientation speak a colloquial language quite close to the standard. This is true for many members of the network of acquaintences, including Ulf and Maria, who are from the Southwest. In Switzerland, however, we have a diglossic situation (Siebenhaar 2004). The switch to standard is made mainly in conversation with foreigners. Bocksberger is portrayed as someone who is unable to perform stylistic variants. The episode around his wife is meant to further discredit him. In Example (3) the dialect is also associated with a kind of social backwardness.
458
Helga Kotthoff
3.3. The kitchen appliance demonstrator The scene talked about in Example (3) takes place in Germany; in conversation 7 nearly the same round of people is having dinner together as in conversation 6. Instead of Johannes and Maria, the two 38 year old linguists Juergen and Erika take part. Ulf tells how he once attended a kitchen appliance demonstration and how he took part in the demonstrator’s presentation. He staged himself as a modern man interested in kitchen appliances and the saleswomen as being astonished about this. (3) (Conversation 7) A: Anni; B: Bernarda; D: David; E: Erika; J: Juergen; K: Katharina; U: Ulf. 1
U:
2 3
K:
4
U:
5 6
A: U:
7 8 9 D: 10 U: 11 12 13 14 E:
15 U: 16 17 ?:
ich war AUch mal bei so ner küchenvorführung für ‘I, too, was once at such a cooking demonstration for’ heimische küchenmaschinen, ‘home kitchen appliances,’ ja ‘yes’ bei hUber auf der klosterstätte. ‘at Huber on Klosterstätte.’ (? ?) und dann hat die frau so frAgen gestellt, ‘and then the woman asked questions,’ und dann hab Ich gesagt, also ich find ja beim RÜHRteig, ‘and then i said, well i find with batter,’ hat er ja ne gewisse schwÄche. ‘it really has a certain weakness.’ hahahaha und die frau °ja woher wisset sIE des?° ‘and the woman °well how do you know that?°’ ich, ja denken sie ich mach kEIne kuchen? ‘I, well do you think I do not make cakes?’ und und dann hab ich mit der rumgefachsimpelt über ‘and and then I talked shop with her about’ über rÜblitorte und was ich fürn rezept hätte, ‘about carrot cake and what sort of recipe I have’ und da dachtense nIcht, du bist professioneller kondit(h)or? ‘and didn't they think you are a professional p(h)astry cook?’ nein °und dann hat se gemeint,° °ja wIsset sie,° ‘no °and then she said, well, you know°’ weil am Anfang ham se gelacht, ne? ‘because at the start they laughed, you know?’ mhm ‘uh-huh’
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others 18 U: 19 E: 20 U: 21 E: 22 U: 23 24 25 E: 26 U: 27 28 E: 29 U:
459
und ham gedacht, was will denn der DEPP da? ‘and they thought, what does that dope want?’ hehe der mAnn, der k(h)ennt sich doch überhaupt nich aus, ‘the man he kno(he)ws nothing at all ab(h)out it,’ ja(haha) ‘yes(haha)’ wenns um kÜchenmaschinen geht. ‘when we are talking about kitchen appliances.’ und dann wurde mir also verspätet zugegeben ‘and then it was admitted belatedly’ °ja die mÄnner heute, die brauchen auch sowas.° ‘°well the men today, they also need such things.°’ hehehehe ham se sich da(ha) also(he) allgemein AUsget(h)auscht. ‘they generally excha(he)nged opinions.’ hehe dass die z(h)eit(h)en sich geÄndert haben. ‘hehe that t(h)im(h)es have changed.’ hehehehe fand ich sEhr schön irgendwie. ‘I found that really nice somehow.’
In lines 7 and 8 he animates a commentary he made in the manner of an expert and in a rather stilted language (beim Rührteig hat er ja ne gewisse Schwäche ‘with batter it really has a certain weakness’). David immediately laughs. The kitchen appliance demonstrator is presented as very astonished in direct speech, speaking in Alemannic dialect (woher wisset sie des?). After that, in line 11, the narrator renders himself in standard language. He presents himself as being astonished by the woman’s question. Then a metalinguistic orientation is given to the further course of the conversation’s topics in the shop. Erika asks a question in regard to the impression he made on the kitchen appliance saleswoman (14), which Ulf answers in the negative. In line 15, Ulf commences a further, not consistently maintained, presentation of the saleswoman, again using Alemannic dialect (wisset). Then he goes back in time to the beginning of the dialogue and acts out the reservations of the women present (18, 20, 22). Erika laughs. In conclusion, the women are quoted as persons who have learned their lesson about the new men: °ja die MÄnner heute, die brauchen auch sowas°. Ulf summarizes the consciousness expanding impression of the women in Standard German and in conclusion makes a positive evaluation of the whole exchange: times have changed and he finds it “really nice somehow”.
460
Helga Kotthoff
In example (3) Ulf approaches conservative women in an everyday scene to make clear to them that the old division of labor between the sexes is no longer self-evident. Modern men also want to be addressed with questions of kitchen appliances. The contours of a progressive identity emerge in the course of the conversation. The favorite emotion of this self is by no means outrage at the environment’s inappropriate gender categories but rather an amused superiority. Again a play with typified ways of speaking is involved. This parodistic sort of intertextual humor allows the teller to demonstrate and test for shared social knowledge and authenticates a self that is well-placed in the social cosmos. 3.4. The Social Democrat In example (4) the group talks about a Swiss couple that is of opposed political opinions. Ulf and Maria jointly recount an episode which they experienced with Herr and Frau Vroner at a reception they participated in for professional reasons. Herr Dr. Vroner is one of Maria’s superiors in a cultural center in a Swiss border town. He is conservative, whereas his wife had just campaigned for the Social Democrats. In line 18 Maria characterizes his wife as ‘a very very nice woman’. The journalist Ulf then informs the group about her husband, the director of a cultural center. For the majority of the others present, the Vroners are simply casual acquaintances. Absent bosses and higher-placed persons often serve as objects of mocking humor in intimate groups. In the story-telling we recognize a similar configuration of personalities as in examples (1), (2), and (3). The conservative Dr. Vroner is most strongly caricatured by a childish way of speaking, not by quoting him in an Alemannic speech variety. (4) (Conversation 6 Episode 4) Everyone (a); Anni (A); Bernada (B); David (D); Johannes (J); Katharina (K); Maria (M); several (m); Ulf (U). 19 U: 20
21
des is AUch so nett, also ihr mann ist ‘that is also so nice, well her husband is’ kultUramtsleiter und schreibt für die zÜrcher, ‘director of the cultural center and writes for the zürcher7,’ eigentlich auch n ganz lIEber, aber doch eher e bissle kOnservativ. ‘actually also a darling, but still a bit conservative.’
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others 22 23
24 M: 25 U: 26 J: 27 D: 28 U: 29 30 31 32 m: 33 J: 34 M: 35 36 36 37 a: 38 A: 39 M: 40 m: 41 M: 42 43 44
461
und dann eh ich hab dann eh ich hab dann mich nur mit ‘and then uh I have uh I have only chatted with’ der frau vroner über die eff a Achtzehn bomber unterhalten, ‘mrs. vroner about the eff a eighteen bombers,’ ja ‘yes’ und warum man die NICHT beschaffen soll, ‘and why one should NOT buy them,’ nei:n. ‘no:.’ mhm ‘mhm’ un da hatter °ja.° hat gesagt, °also° ‘an then he said °well.° °then°’ des hAb ich dir doch jetzt schon so: Oft gesagt. ‘I have told you that already so: often.’ (-) wir WOLLEN ↑NICHT mehr über die (-) ‘we do ↑NOT WANT to talk about’ bomberbeschaffung reden. ‘buying the bombers any more.’ hehehehehehehehehe [he [aja: [‘I see:’ ja ja. und zum Ulf hat sie auch gesagt beim essen, ja, ‘yeah yeah. and she also said during the meal to Ulf,’ ich bin ↑schO:n eine sozialdemokratin. ‘well, I ↑am after all a social democrat.’ °und er immer°, psch::::t, psch:::::t ‘°and he was like° ’ hahahahahahahahahahahahaha [ha:::::: [sü:::ß: [‘cu:::te’ hahahahaha [hehehehehe [und jetzt war er wohl auch nich so GANZ [‘and now he probably was not so ENTIRELY’ einverstanden, dass seine frau kandidiert, ‘pleased that his wife was going to campaign for office,’ hat peter dObendorfer gesagt, ‘Peter Dobendorfer said,’ .
The sentence des ist auch so nett (‘that is also so nice’) functions as an evaluative introduction. Mr. Vroner is characterized in terms of profession, character and political attitude, whereby n ganz lieber (‘a darling’) in line
462
Helga Kotthoff
21 and konservativ (‘conservative’) are presented as almost contrasting by the adverb doch (still). The conversational topic F A 18 bombers contains a certain tension, since it was being heatedly debated between conservatives and progressives at the time of the recording (1995). Herr Dr. Vroner’s attitude is conservative, i.e., he favors continuing to arm the Swiss army with fighter jets. His wife and the circle whose evening chats make up the subject of this article are against a further armament of the Swiss army. In line 25 Ulf attributes self-evidence to his position and performs for himself a cool daredevil attitude. Johannes utters an astonished nein (‘no’), presumably in comprehending the touchy, conflict-laden situation. Ulf presents himself as self-assertive. He doesn’t attempt to hide his opinion. In line 28 he starts to parody Herr Vroner with a direct quote. He reproduces Herr Vroner’s speech with a typical conversational introduction, ja, also (‘well, then’), which is spoken much more softly. A strongly stereotypical parental statement follows (I have told you that already so often), directed at his wife (who thinks like Ulf), with the paternalistic “we” (she was talking about purchasing the bombers, while he was not involved) and an elongation of the so:, which signals emphasis. Mr. Vroner starts softly and increases volume in line 30. Everyone laughs. The culture office director is parodied as old-fashioned and avuncular. He forbids his wife, who thinks differently, to continue talking about controversial topics. The avuncular manner of speaking attributed to him violates the usual conception of a formal and distinguished culture office director (and men in similar positions). The amusingly hyperbolistic stylizing of the protagonists through quotation procedures again holds the center here. Maria continues the story of the meal with the Vroners from line 34 onwards. She also quotes Frau Vroner in direct speech. The sentence ich bin ↑SCHO:N EINE Sozialdemokratin (‘I am after all a Social Democrat’) is clearly articulated, as is typical for Swiss who speak Standard High German. Maria imitates the Swiss-German sentence intonation with the strong rise on schon (‘after all’) and the following fall. The German modal particle schon translated here as ‘after all’ is also interesting because Frau Vroner’s statement is thereby shaped as a concession. The modestly progressive selfidentification of the culture office director’s wife is thereby presented as an act of courage. Maria parodies Dr. Vroner as being shocked about his wife’s political commitment and tells his wife to be silent. In an extremely paternalistic manner, Frau Vroner’s self-identification as a Social Democrat is declared taboo by Herr Vroner. This is particularly implausible, because she has just
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others
463
campaigned for this party in the community council elections (the group knows that). The interjections pscht in line 36 are accompanied by the appropriate childish gesture of laying a finger on the mouth. Herr Vroner, known as an authority figure with a high office, is turned into a caricature thereof in Ulf’s and Maria’s dialogue construction. His authority is undermined. Speakers can increase intimacy among friends through shared amusement at the expense of people who, due to their power, are potentially threatening. The mocking has a releasing function, but also communicates distinction from social circles like those of the Vroners. Maria hyperstylizes Herr Vroner’s shock at his wife’s confession. He is presented as if for him Social Democrats were something quite monstrous. Everyone laughs. Maria rates Herr Vroner’s speech as cute which also presupposes ‘not dangerous’. Starting in line 41 she explains Herrn Vroner’s contradictory attitude toward his wife’s political candidacy. The image of the self which is carried out in episodes like example (4) could be paraphrased as: we know these funny conservatives and amuse ourselves about them. They are mocked. Mocking humor always integrates a grain of indignation (Christmann 1996). This indignation is not proclaimed in a straight manner but evoked in dialogue parody and sometimes in exaggerated labelings of the dramatis personae.
4.
Other means of stylization
4.1. The pan-seller in the street In example (5) a chat is recounted which three of the women present at the dinner table had that day with a Swiss saleswoman trying to sell teflon pans in the street. In this chat, Erika portrayed herself as though she had a husband who did everything in the kitchen. This is not the actual state of affairs, but it created astonishment on the part of the saleswoman and amusement on the part of the listeners. In the story, many voices intermingle. Example (5) is hard to follow because the narrators Erika, Anni and Bernarda restage the chat in the street without explicitly indicating from moment to moment with whose voice they are speaking. We hear an intermingling of voices even in one turn. Exaggerated typification is again an important element in the directly reported speech (Brünner 1991; Kotthoff 1998). However, the saleswoman is not cited in Alemannic dialect. As a reason for this I see that Anni and
464
Helga Kotthoff
Bernada who participated in the scene and also in the narration are from Berlin and not able to join the play with variation. The group is amused by the little talkshow with the saleswoman, which is now being recreated at the table. (5) (Conversation 7) K: Katharina; E: Erika; A: Anni; U: Ulf; B: Bernarda; D: David; J: Juergen; s: some of them. 1
E:
2 3
B:
4 A:
5 6
s: B:
7 8
s: J:
9 s: 10 E: 11 K: 12 B: 13 E: 14 B: 15 E: 16 B: 17 E: 18 19 B: 20 21 A:
ich hab heut schon mit dir Angegeben. mein mann kOcht. ‘i already bragged about you today. my husband cooks.’ hehe[hehehe [ja(ha) (? [ ?) [‘ye(he)s’ (? [ ?) [ mit bU(h)tter. ha[hahahahahahaha [‘with bu(h)tter. ha[hahahahahahahaha’ [hahahahahahahahaha [naI:v [‘nai:ve’ [hahahahahahahahahaha [aber gesU:nd. hehe [‘but hea:lthy. Hehe’ [hehehehehe [hehehehehe [ja er kocht SE::HR gesund und bewusst. [‘yes he cooks ve::ry health consciously.’ WAS? ‘WHAT?’ wir wurden gefrAgt auf der straße, und da hat‘we were asked on the street, and then-’ über unsere Essgewohnheiten. von einer schwEIzerin. ‘about our eating habits. by a swiss lady.’ wer kOcht. mein mAnn. (-) [sagt sIE. ‘who cooks. my husband (-) [she says.’ [ich wollt n tOpf für meinen mann. [‘I wanted a pot for my husband.’ wie Oft? jEden tag. (-) [was für tÖpfe haben sie. ‘how often? every day. (-) [what sort of pots do you have.’ [wI:rklich? wie die mich [‘really? how she looked at me.’ Angeguckt [hat. [(? geschirr?) aluminium, tEflon, [‘(?utensils?) aluminium, teflon,’ °wEIß ich nich. mein MANN kocht.° [na und dann hab ich gesagt, ‘°I don't know. my husband cooks.° [well and then I said,’ [hehehehe
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others 22 B: 23 A: 24 E: 25 26 B: 27 E:
28 B: 29 E: 30 s: 31 E: 32 33 U: 34 35 K: 36 E: 37 U: 38 M: 39 K: 40 B: 41 K: 42 43 s:
465
ihr habt Alles. Ver [schIEdenes. ‘you have everything. Var [ious things.’ [hehehehehehe[hehehe [und dann hat se noch [‘and then she’ gfragt, (- -) wo [mIt er kocht. ‘asked, (- -) with [what he cooks.’ [womIt kocht er. [‘with what does he cook.’ hamma gsagt, manchmal mit Ö::l, aber natürlich mit BUTTER. ‘we said, sometimes with oi::l, but of course with BUTTER.’ und da gesagt WA:::S? ‘and then she said, WHA:::T?’ und DAS nennen sie gesundes essen? ja SEHR. ‘and you call that healthy food? yes VERY.’ hahahahahaha die wollte uns nämlich nur so was verkaufen, wo du gAr ‘she only wanted to sell us something for which you’ nichts brauchst. hehehehehe ‘don't need anything. hehehehe’ (? ?) von der schweizerischen megalit. ‘from the swiss firm megalit.’ ((hard to understand)) SCHMECKT doch alles überhaupt nich. ‘but nothing has any flavor at all.’ ja ja. ‘yeah yeah.’ ach sO. das war son stAnd. ‘ah. there was such a stand.’ ja ja. ‘yeah yeah’ dann habt ihr natürlich die ganze statistik ruiniert. ‘then you naturally ruined all the statistics.’ (? ?) mein mann kOcht. das macht Alles mein mann. ‘with my husband cooks. my husband does all that.’ ich habe überhaupt keine ahnung. ‘I know nothi(h)ng at all.’ hahahahahahahahaha
Jürgen enters with a fish dish and Erika, Jürgen’s wife, then takes up the culinarily accomplished husband as a topic. The transcript begins here. Erika says that she has already bragged about Jürgen that day and then quotes herself in direct speech: ‘my husband cooks’. She laughs and
466
Helga Kotthoff
thereby contextualizes what follows as a humorous story. Anni laughingly presents a further detail from Erika’s self-citation in line 4: with bu(h)tter. ha[hahahahahahahaha. Bernarda comments, naiv (‘naive’) in line 6. At first glance the adjective naiv makes little sense. It may be that Bernada finds it naïve to cook with butter. But it is more plausible to suppose that Bernada has adopted the voice of the saleswoman to whom Erika bragged about her husband. She assumes a role in the dialogue which occurred on the street. Anni and Bernarda identify themselves as having participated in the episode to be narrated. It is mostly the three women who participated in the episode who laugh in response to this. Jürgen laughingly offers a commentary in line 8 which also provokes a mirthful response. Although it is rather uncertain with whose voice Bernarda spoke the word “naïve”, the comment could be understood as a reference to cooking with butter. Jürgen defends this practice with an exaggerated intonation and laughter. Thus, a play with stereotypical comments is staged in reference to cooking practices, which the others also consider funny. Erika in line 10 again places herself directly in the dialogue on the street, which has not as yet been otherwise introduced. In line 11 Katharina shows problems in reception. Bernarda and Erika give background information in a highly collaborative manner. Again in line 14 Bernarda cites the question of the saleswoman (‘who cooks?’) and then Erika’s answer (‘my husband’); then Erika continues to explain what she wanted from the Swiss saleswoman: a pot for her husband. In line 16 Bernarda again recounts the dialogue between Erika and the pan-seller; she first takes on the voice of the Swiss saleswoman, then switches to Erika’s voice (‘every day’), and back again to the saleswoman’s question (‘what sort of pots do you have’). Erika’s questioning wirklich ‘really’ in line 17 is staged as if taken from the saleswoman’s lips. She portrays her as astonished. In line 19, Bernarda presumably first restages the saleswoman’s questions about their cooking utensils in order to reply suddenly with Erika’s voice (in line 20): ‘°I don't know. my husband cooks.°’ Then she cites her own contribution (‘well and then I said, you have everything. various things’). In lines 24 and 25, Erika adopts indirect speech to recount the saleswoman’s further questions. Bernada repeats it transformed into a direct question. Erika quotes the group’s answer in the street. The three women have tried to shock the saleswoman not only by having a husband who does all the cooking but also by not favoring fatfree ways of preparing food. In line 28 Bernarda repeats the saleswoman’s cry
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others
467
of astonishment (‘wha::::t?’). Again, Erika continues the cry of astonishment (‘and you call that healthy food?’). She then quotes her own reply (‘yes very’). A few listeners laugh. Then Erika explains what the woman was selling (‘special pots for which no fat is needed’). Ulf knows the manufacturer of the pots (33). Katharina rejects such products in line 34, thereby joining her friends’ attitude concerning cooking without fat. Ulf also shows his sudden understanding of the narrated scene in line 35. Katharina summarizes the event in regard to German and Swiss statistics about the division of household labor (37). Namely, it has invalidated the statistics which for decades have said that in Germany and Switzerland women do a greater share of homework. In line 39 she speaks with Erika’s voice in the dialogue with the kitchen utensil saleswoman. Bernarda continues the speech in the same role (‘I know nothi(H)ng at all’). The audience laughs. Two performances are intertwined here, the one on the street and the one at the table. On the street, the three provoked the saleswoman and taught her that her expectations about normality are out of date. At the table they present themselves as being able to use an everyday situation for a little stand up comedy. The saleswoman is portrayed as simply taken aback by Erika’s revelation. The progressive customer replies quite matter-of-factly, with a manner of speaking suggesting that it is a foregone conclusion. Erika presents her norm-violating marital relationship with the greatest matter-of-factness. This modality of certainty is used here to obtain a double effect: first, in contact with the saleswoman, as a means of stylizing herself as a ‘new woman’ with a ‘new man’. Second, it is offered to the group as a successful portrayal of ‘pulling the saleswoman’s leg’. The listeners laugh at the special stylizations in this “mimetic satire” (Auerbach 1971; Schwitalla 1994; Jaffe 1998). Those present know that Erika has greatly exaggerated in portraying her husband as a house-husband. She plays with gender norms. The group’s presentation at the dinner table serves, for one thing, as an amusing way to tell about provoking the saleswoman in the street, second, reproduces a distinction in regard to life styles (the saleswoman embodies the littlevalued normalcy), and, third, helps to characterize the narrators as persons capable of exploiting the comic potential inherent in everyday situations, thus as active and go-ahead fellows. At the end, Katharina evaluates the performance as such, recapitulating a few of the key punch lines. The play with “others’” voices was evidently made accessible to all. That may be the reason that the pan-seller was not
468
Helga Kotthoff
mimed in Swiss German, since the participants Bernarda and Anna, from Berlin, can’t speak a word of it. In example (3) we witnessed a very similar content, with a similar constellation of dramatis personae and performance. Ulf’s self-stylization in example (3) is very close to that of the three women in example (5) (Erika, Anni, and Bernada) who told their story first and later enjoyed Ulf’s story. However, the three women do not use dialect features to portray those whom they confronted with their different life style. Anni and Bernada are unable to play with Alemannic dialects. Thus, in example (3) and in example (5) the self is staged as confronting the world with new standards of behavior. Voices are rendered in a very similar way, and the ideological relations confirm those in the other stories being told in the group. The narrator Ulf affiliates himself with Erika, Anni and Bernada. 4.2. The young gentleman In the next example, dialect again plays no role in the citation. The cited mother is from Northern Germany. Nevertheless, a specific speech style can be attributed to her. Martin, a homosexual journalist, shows his friends around his new flat. The group arrives in the kitchen. (6) (Conversation 12 Episode 4) Friederike (F); Annette (A); Martin (M); Lars (L); Bernd (B). 1
F:
2
L:
3
A:
4
L:
hier hats ja nur ein fEnster. ‘there is only one window.’ is aber doch schÖn fürn jungen herrn. weischt. ‘but it is really nice for a young gentleman. you know.’ ja das rEI(hhh)cht fürn jungen mann. ‘yes, it suffi(hhh)ces for a young man.’ fürn jungen hErrn, sagt deine mutter immer. ‘for a young gentleman, your mother always says.’
Friederike (a lecturer of about the same age) notes that Bernd’s kitchen has only one window and will accordingly be dimly lit during the daytime. Thereupon Lars delivers a phrase from the repertoire of elderly women: is aber doch schön fürn jungen Herrn. Annette agrees with her boyfriend and raises the level of playful impoliteness. Bernd, the forty year old journalist,
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others
469
is defined as a ‘young gentleman’ who needs no brightly illuminated kitchen. This activity integrates dimensions of a mock challenge. In line 4 Lars makes the source of the flowery phrase explicit, Annette’s mother. The attribution of domains and objects to gentlemen and ladies is found equally absurd in this circle. Lars also corrects Annette’s utterance ‘young man’ to ‘young gentleman’. The correct wording is important for the stylization. It is unclear whether ‘really nice for a young gentleman’ should be understood as a compliment or just as being ironic.8 This scene, too, lives on knowledge about typical ways of speaking. These, inserted in the manner of unintroduced quotation-like speech, suggest the attitudes of those from which the self is differentiated. Categories like ‘young gentleman’ appear from the beginning as if in quotation marks. Both ‘you know’ as well as the laugh particles and the correction in line 4 point to these symbolically.
5.
Humorous distance
All six dialogs that I have grouped together here for the analysis of identity deal with normative encounters in the life of the sexes. They deal with marriage candidates, division of labor in the kitchen, couple’s differences of opinion, patterns of behavior of married men, and generally with gender attributes. The narrators present themselves in confrontation with persons from whom they differentiate themselves in the story world and in the narrative situation. They do this with humorous keying,9 in which, however, the degree of exaggeration of the staged persons varies. The conservative figures are made to appear conservative by placing highly formulaic phrases in their mouths, by letting them speak on stage in dialect, react inflexibly and unsophisticated and get excited. Likewise, in confrontation the self shapes itself on the levels of the story world and the current, real situation. In mutual, complementary orientation to one another, common facets of identification can thus be created. Humorous keying inhibits the appearance of arrogance. The self in the story world takes the initiative, but without stress. It does not hesitate to introduce its own view of things to the locals. In this way, the self’s standpoint as well as its brashness are displayed as morally correct. The congruency of the stories confirms this model of the self in the current situation. Working with exaggerated categorization and deconstruction, the story-tellers also create distance from their
470
Helga Kotthoff
own selves. In all of the stories, the performance is accountable and as such is enjoyed. As already mentioned before, explicit moralization has a bad reputation in the Western world (Bergmann and Luckmann 1999), but people try to give their identities a moral underpinning. Attributing superiority to one’s own attitude and behavior is also a delicate undertaking. As Coupland (2001) put it: Straightforward formulation might be too obvious and stark a claim to succeed in the late-modern climate. Humorous stylizing of ingroup and outgroup seems to be a successful symbolic practice that can achieve a distanced validation of speakers’ social identities. The humorous keying allows the members of the network to play with distanced validation: the conservatives are more strongly typified and distanced, but also for their own social image a certain distance remains. Also, the orientation to gender remains indirect in these episodes but accountable. In humorous discourses this issue is dealt with in a playful frame. Attitudes are transmitted mainly by evocation. Stylization is thus a subversive form of multi-voiced utterance, one that can discredit a voice and a person by reworking them into the local purpose of a playful realization of the superiority of the speaker’s own attitude. In direct or mediated contact the group processes the constant changes in political, economic and ecological developments in a similar manner; the members show each other what is normal, what knowledge and what attitude one disposes of – and by doing this they simultaneously constitute features of their social identity. Gender relations have in the meantime become milieu-specific in the Western world (Koppetsch and Burkart 2000). We face a range of masculinities and femininities (Connell 1995; Baron and Kotthoff 2002), integrating a variety of different lifestyles and behavioral standards, among them the traditional ones. As in the past, traditional masculinity is, for example, still symbolized in certain professions and types of sport (Connell 1995, 2002; Behnke and Meuser 2002). Clear power relations with male dominance continue to exist in the higher spheres of politics, economy, religion and the sciences. Traditional femininity still centers around home, beauty and body care. Alongside these, there are varied deviations from traditional roles and norms. Even if we must start from the fact that milieus with symmetrical gender relationships constitute a minority in the Germanspeaking countries, it is nevertheless (or precisely for this reason) interesting to observe how these groups create normality for their social identity, which diverges from traditional societal standards.
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others
471
Appendix: Transcription conventions (-) (- -) (0.5) (? what ?) (? ?) ..[.. ..[.... . =
one hyphen indicates a short pause two hyphens indicate a longer pause (less than half a second) pause of half a second; long pauses are counted in half seconds indicates uncertain transcription indicates an incomprehensible utterance
indicates overlap or interruption latching of an utterance of one person; no interruption hahaha laughter hehehe slight laughter goo(h)d integrated laughter (h) audible exhalation ('h) audible inhalation , slightly rising intonation ? rising intonation . falling intonation , ongoing intonation : indicates elongated sound ° blabla° lower amplitude and pitch COME ON emphatic stress (pitch and volume shift) come On accent syllable (only in the German original) ↑_ high onset of pitch ↓ pitch goes down low pitch register within the brackets comments ((sits down)) nonverbal actions or comments
Notes 1. Mummendey (1995) discusses various sociological and psychological concepts of the self and of identity. He concludes that the concept of self overlaps with that of identity, with the exception of a few special traditions. An individual performs various social and situational identities, but is also identical with heror himself. For him it seems justified to translate self to identity and to see the two concepts as semantically equivalent.
472
Helga Kotthoff
2. Davies (2006) analyzes the humor of movie characters in connection with typification. 3. In this article I am not concerned with humor theory; see Kotthoff (1998) on that matter. When I speak of conversational caricatures, I mean the exaggeration of character traits, created by linguistic rather than visual effects. 4. I also have data from other social milieus. Mocking humor which relates to gender norms I only found among the academics of this age group. 5. Such introductions are typical for humorous stories. 6. In German this is a stylistically elevated expression. 7. The Swiss newspaper Neue Zürcher Zeitung. 8. See Kotthoff (2002) on conversational irony and its relation to citation-like footing. 9. The term keying is used in the sense of Hymes (1974).
References Antaki, Charles and Sue Widdicombe 1998 Identity as an achievement and as a tool. In: Antaki, Charles and Sue Widdicombe (eds.), Identities in Talk. London, 1–14. Auerbach, Erich 1971 Mimesis. Dargestellte Wirklichkeit in der abendländischen Literatur. Bern/Munich: Francke. Bakhtin, Mikhail M. (Bachtin, Michail M.) 1969 [1985] Literatur und Karneval. Zur Romantheorie und Lachkultur. Munich: Hanser. 1981 The Dialogic Imagination, C. Emerson and M. Holquist (eds.). Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. Barbour, Stephen and Patrick Stevenson 1999 Variation in German. A Critical Approach to German Sociolinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Baron, Bettina and Helga Kotthoff (eds.) 2002 Gender in Interaction. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Behnke, Cornelia and Michael Meuser 2002 Gender and habitus: Fundamental securities and crisis tendencies among men. In: Baron, Bettina and Helga Kotthoff (eds.), Gender in Interaction. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 153–175. Bourdieu, Pierre 1979 Distinction. Paris: Minuit. Brünner, Gisela 1991 Redewiedergabe in Gesprächen. Deutsche Sprache 1, 1–16.
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others
473
Christmann, Gabriele 1996 Die Aktivität des „Sich-Mokierens“ als konversationelle Satire. Wie sich Umweltschützer/innen über den „Otto-Normalverbraucher“ mokieren. In: Kotthoff, Helga (ed.), Scherzkommunikation. Beiträge zur empirischen Gesprächsforschung. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag, 49–80. Connell, Robert W. 1995 Masculinities. Cambridge: Polity Press. Cook-Gumperz, Jenny and John Gumperz 1976 Context in children’s speech. In: Papers on Language and Context. Working Papers 46. Berkeley, CA: Language Behaviour Research Laboratory. Couper-Kuhlen, Elizabeth 1999 Coherent voicing. On prosody in conversational reported speech. In: Bublitz, Wolfram et al. (eds.), Coherence in Spoken and Written Discourse, 11–35. Coupland, Nikolas 2001 Dialect stylization in radio talk. Language in Society 30(3), 345– 375. Davies, Catherine E. 2006 Gendered sense of humor as expressed through aesthetic typifications. Journal of Pragmatics 38(1), 96–114. Deppermann, Arnulf this volume Playing with the voice of the other – Stylized Kanaksprak in conversations among German adolescents. In: Auer, Peter (ed.), Style and Social Identities: Alternative Approaches to Linguistic Heterogeneity. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. de Fina, Anna this volume Style and stylization in the construction of identities in a cardplaying club. In: Auer, Peter (ed.), Style and Social Identities: Alternative Approaches to Linguistic Heterogeneity. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Dupréel, Emile 1928 Le problème sociologique de rire. Revue Philosophique 196, 213– 260. Eckert, Penelope 2000 Linguistic Variation as Social Practice. London: Blackwell. Goffman, Erving 1981 Forms of Talk. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
474
Helga Kotthoff
Günthner, Susanne 1996 The contextualization of affect in reported dialogue. In: Niemeyer, S. and R. Dirven (eds.), The Language of Emotions. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 247–277. 1999 Polyphony and the ‘layering of voices’ in reported dialogue: An analysis of the use of prosodic devices in everyday reported speech. Journal of Pragmatics 31, 685–708. this volume The construction of otherness in reported dialogues as a resource for identity work. In: Auer, Peter (ed.), Style and Social Identities: Alternative Approaches to Linguistic Heterogeneity. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. Gumperz, John 1982 Discourse Strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1996 The linguistic and cultural relativity of inference. In: Gumperz, John J. and Stephen Levinson (eds.), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 374–407. Hymes, Dell 1974 Ways of speaking. In: Bauman, Richard and Joel Sherzer (eds.), Explorations in the Ethnography of Speaking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 433–451. Jaffe, Alexandra 1998 Comic performance and the articulation of hybrid identity. Pragmatics 10(1), 39–59. Koppetsch, Cornelia and Günter Burkart 1999 Die Illusion der Emanzipation. Constance: Universitätsverlag Konstanz. Kotthoff, Helga 1996 Impoliteness and conversational joking: On relational politics. Folia Linguistica 30(3–4), 299–327. 1998 Spass Verstehen. Zur Pragmatik von konversationellem Humor. Tübingen: Niemeyer. 2000 Konversationelle Parodie. Über komische Intertextualität in der Alltagskommunikation. Germanistische Linguistik 153, 159–186. 2002 Irony, quotation, and other forms of staged intertextuality. In: Graumann, Carl and Werner Kallmeyer (eds.), Perspective and Perspectivation in Discourse. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 201–233. 2006 Gender and humor. The state of the art. Journal of Pragmatics 38(1), 4–26. Malone, Martin J. 1997 Worlds of Talk. The Presentation of Self in Everyday Conversation. Cambridge: Polity Press.
The humorous stylization of ‘new’ women and men and conservative others
475
Mead, George Herbert 1934 Mind, Self and Society from the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Mulkay, Michael 1988 Humor. Cambridge: Polity Press. Mummendey, Hans-Dieter 1995 Psychologie der Selbstdarstellung. Göttingen: Hogrefe. Rampton, Ben 1999 Styling the other. Special issue of Journal of Sociolinguistics 3(4). Schutz, Alfred 1970 On Phenomenology and Social Relations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Schutz, Alfred and Thomas Luckmann 1973 The Structures of the Life-World. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. Schwitalla, Johannes 1994 Sprachliche Ausdrucksformen für soziale Identität beim Erzählen. Beobachtungen zu vier Gruppen in Vogelstang. In: Kallmeyer, Werner (ed.), Kommunikation in der Stadt. Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter, 511–577. Siebenhaar, Beat 2005 Dialekt und Hochsprache in der deutschsprachigen Schweiz. Zurich: Wyler. Tannen, Deborah 1989 Talking Voices: Repetition, Dialogue, and Imagery in Conversational Discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Taylor, Charles 1989 Sources of the Self: The Making of the Modern Identity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Voloshinov, Valentin N. 1926 [1978] Reported speech. In: Matejka, Ladislav and Kristina Pomorska (eds.), Readings in Russian Poetics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 149–175.
Chapter 16 A postscript: Style and identity in interactional sociolinguistics John J. Gumperz and Jenny Cook-Gumperz
Identity may well be gaining a significance beyond that of an intrinsic human life-cycle, and its stages and crises. For, in our time the historical emphasis on the differences between individual, national and even religious identities must find a re-orientation that emphasizes and cultivates the essential unity of all human identities. By this I mean the consciousness and ethical responsibility of being one species that must learn to orient its outlook and inventions towards the preservation and enrichment of all life instead of a deadly extension of senseless, technical perfection and power. For this however, it is necessary to learn to understand fully what a specieswide identity can do for each individual’s and each community’s vitality to make senseless mass murder impossible. Erik H. Erikson (1983)
1.
Identity as a social construct
The above statement from the father of psychoanalytic identity theory and social biography stresses the need for a common intellectual frame that can bring together different and divergent views in times of political change and uncertainty. If Erikson’s statement seems somewhat idealistic or vague, speaking as it does of the unity of humankind, it was written in response to what he saw as the exceptional challenges of the beginning of the nuclear age. For the citation dates back more than two decades to a time when scholars saw an unavoidable tension between the individual’s embodied distinctiveness and the socially shared cultural solidarity of group belonging, or between the individual psychologically constituted ego and the laws of the social world. As anthropologist Meyer Fortes (1983) commented at the same conference, the problem facing research on identity is that it appears to be cast in terms of fluctuations between two ends of a continuum.
478
John J. Gumperz and Jenny Cook-Gumperz
In other words, identity is “located in the space between two poles”. For the individual the emphasis is on “what goes on inside my skin”, at the other pole one says “What I am is what society makes of me” (1983: 393). Both positions are constitutive of a person’s identity, but traditionally researchers have looked for causal explanations exclusively at one or the other end of the continuum. The possibility Fortes introduces, i.e. that we are neither made entirely by others nor do we make ourselves without other’s support and interaction, was ignored. More recent social theories which adopt the perspective of ordinary societal members rather than the analyst’s position as primary view the person as a thinking, speaking being, an active agent within a social world constructed through interaction with others. These theories have changed the focus of identity theorizing. We can now see that identity involves not the opposition between the individual and the social; rather, the two are intertwined and it is the continuity of the person that we work to maintain through acts of speaking. We present a stable social self by providing internally consistent narratives about our selves and our actions in a changing world. If identity is communicated through acts of speaking then we can think of speaking styles as representations of such identities. Treating identity as a communicated phenomenon allows for the possibility of multiple and flexible, inherently contingent selves that have coherence only from specific points of view and in specific contexts.
2.
Identity as life style
Contemporary society is increasingly shaped by mediating bureaucratic institutions that impact many areas of daily life and mandate communicative standards of their own. We now recognize that while identity requires continuous validation of a bureaucratically sanctioned self on the one hand, on the other it also calls for the on-going reinvention of the individual persona. This position therefore presents specific challenges. Individuals are expected to construct coherence through explanations about their own fit or lack of fit to bureaucratically mandated categories, yet at the same time they must present a self that in any one context can be seen as continuous with a history that both precedes and extends beyond the narrative present. The situation becomes even more complex when we consider that late modern societies also provide new possibilities for individual change and for the progressive development of the self. Change inevitably involves
A postscript: Style and identity in interactional sociolinguistics
479
risks, but it also requires that individuals present themselves as socially acceptable and attractive beings – the new growth industry of selfawareness. As Giddens (1991) describes it, late modern society’s emphasis on new life-style movements “represents an era beyond the emancipation from want and from hierarchical domination into a politics of choice. Life politics is centered on lifestyle choices and issues of self-actualization from which political consequences flow” (1991: 209). From our own position we argue that these choices are ultimately mediated by sociolinguistic choices and, as this paper and others in this volume show, not by selection from among a limited set of behavioral options. Judith Irvine (2001) suggests that speech styles share some of the characteristics of dress styles: they can be put on to suit an occasion and a situation. However, speech styles also gain durability over time as they become typified as indexes of identity. Though open to some revision, they count as an integral part of an individual’s self presentation. To quote Giddens again: In the post-traditional order of modernity and against the back-drop of new forms of mediated experience self identity becomes a reflexively organized endeavor. The reflexive product of the self which consists in the sustaining of coherent, yet continuously revised biographical narrative takes place in the context of multiple choices as filtered through abstract systems. The more tradition loosens its hold, and the more daily life is reconstituted in terms of the dialectical inter-play of the local and global the more individuals are forced to negotiate life-style choices among a diversity of options (1991: 5).
Giddens’ use of the term “biographical narrative” here, notions of “discourse” suggest that modernity theorists are growing aware of the crucial importance of language as discourse. Yet, as we argue below, speech styles have only recently come to be recognized as relevant to social science theorizing. There is moreover little if any agreement on what we mean by these terms and how we can relate them to social categorizations.
3.
Linguistic style
Our current notions of linguistic style can be traced back to Thomas Sebeok’s (1960) edited volume Style in Language that brought together literary scholars, psycholinguists and linguists, all writing on various aspects of
480
John J. Gumperz and Jenny Cook-Gumperz
style each from their own disciplinary perspectives. While the linguists mostly took up problems emerging from the stylistics of written literary texts, the questions of spoken language and performance remained in the background. Roman Jakobson calls attention to these limitations. In his postscript to the volume entitled “Linguistics and Poetics”, he criticizes the narrowness of then current linguists’ work as follows: Keeping linguistics apart from poetics is warranted only when the field of linguistics appears to be illicitly restricted, when the sentence is viewed by some linguists as the highest analyzable construction or when the scope of linguistics is confined to grammar alone, or uniquely to non-semantic questions of semantic form, or to the inventory of denotative devices with no reference to free variations (1960: 352).
Asking “what do language and poetry have in common?”, Jakobson argues that both are communicative acts and the relationship between them must be assessed at the super-ordinate level of speech event not through morpheme by morpheme sentence level comparative linguistic analysis. Jakobson’s suggestive paper was one of the first to suggest that context be treated as a communicative phenomenon. Although his functional semiotic perspective on speaking has undergone extensive theoretical and conceptual refinement in the course of the last few decades, it was and continues to be fundamental to the ethnography of communication and related interpretive anthropological linguistic research on discourse where, as we will show in this chapter, notions of poetic, emotive and metapragmatic aspects of performance are again at the center of attention. 3.1. Linguistic style as community-wide resource A second main tradition in the study of linguistic style is that of William Labov who rejected his predecessors’ person-centered approach to linguistic analysis, arguing that regularities of speaking can only be revealed at the community level. Labov incorporates style into sociolinguistic theory as one of two key dimensions of variability that relate the linguistic to the social by combining the dialectologists’ measures of variability with the quantitative sociologists’ model of society. His initial goal was to devise valid, replicable methods for charting on-going processes of linguistic change in urban language. To achieve these goals he understood that it was necessary to account for both inter-speaker variability and for individuals’
A postscript: Style and identity in interactional sociolinguistics
481
variation across different speaking contexts. The concept of style, defined as a relation between linguistic and extra-linguistically defined social (or rather macro-societal) measures such as education, social class and prestige was introduced to account for the variations across contexts. The speech community’s linguistic resources no longer focus on a language or a dialect and its variants, but a repertoire, a set of speech varieties with respect to which speakers’ stylistic variability is assessed. The purpose of Labov’s analysis is to determine the range of variability that characterizes the speech community. Rules of variable use are then incorporated into grammatical description by distinguishing between categorical rules, that is, those which hold for the community as a whole, and variable rules that change along social or geographical parameters. In this way, the linguistic and the social are treated as related. Although they count as separate entities, they can be studied in relation to each other, first through qualitative observation and then through quantitative validation. Stylistic variation thus becomes the crucial nexus between individual speech and a community’s shared practices. The assumption is that the styles that constitute the repertoire vary from the vernacular to the prestigious standard-like ways of speaking, monitored for conformance to stabilizing influences imposed from above. Such prestige scales are seen to mirror the society’s class based social structure. It is the vernacular that most closely reflects variability, and is thus most likely to reveal locally based changes. Labov goes on to argue that stylistic variation can be elicited in field studies by presenting speakers with different situations and topics. Labovian sociolinguistics has dominated linguists’ research on style, even as the range of social phenomena dealt with has expanded and the notion of a clearly definable speech community and its distinct norms becomes more controversial (Coupland, this volume). More recently we are beginning to see a shift in this basic paradigm so as to account for speakers’ creative use of stylistic features to mark an individual’s identity in relation to changing social situations, and to newly emerging speech norms associated with them (Eckert and Rickford 2001). The scope of variability analysis has also been broadened from concentrating on ongoing processes of linguistic change and social class variation in geographic regions, to issues of ethnicity identity and gender variation (Baugh 2000; Eckert 2000; Rickford 1999). Yet, despite these developments, linguists continue to treat the linguistic and the social as essentially independent entities.
482
John J. Gumperz and Jenny Cook-Gumperz
3.2. Style as an index of distinctiveness Judith Irvine’s 2001 paper shifts the focus of attention away from language use as such to propose a more broadly conceived semiotics of style, constitutive of linguistic ideologies of distinctiveness that characterizes human societies in general. From this more abstract perspective, speaking style counts as one among several means by which individuals set themselves off from others, in ways that are similar in communicative effect to dress style, consumption patterns, and other options that make up “life style” – the ways of living in late modern societies. Such a concept of style involves a notion of the social, which is not bounded by extra-communicatively defined a priori regional, social or ethnic categories. Boundaries are reflected in individuals’ ways of speaking and acting. Speakers can be shown to trade on this knowledge and its distribution as communicative resources. This new position reflects not only different views on the relationship between speaking and society, but also views of verbal communication that move beyond the specifics of sentence bound grammar to ground usage in a broader Jakobsonian linguistic perspective on semiotic processes. We therefore need to reconsider the means by which current social categories are constructed, to show how on the one hand they are represented in what we say at any one time, and on the other hand are being changed or reinforced through speaking practices over time. Irvine’s key argument is that a style gains communicative effect by being seen as part of an ideologically based “system of distinctions in which one style contrasts with other styles within the context of that system and its social meaning contrasts with other social meanings in the system” (2001: 24). Styles in other words are not inherently meaningful by themselves. They should not be studied in isolation but should be examined in terms of a) their relation to other styles and b) to the semiotic principles of stylistic differentiation in what Irvine calls the “continuously evolving sociolinguistic systems” of which they are part. The communicative effect of styles is mediated by ideology (defined as that aspect of culture by which we evaluate ideas, objects and actions). Sociolinguists generally agree that ways of speaking index the social formations with which they are typically associated. According to the Peircian theory of semiotics from which current notion derives, an index can only lead to social action if it acts as a sign, and a sign requires an interpretant, that is, it must be meaningful and understandable to at least some of those individuals whose reactions it is designed to affect. Indexes in other words
A postscript: Style and identity in interactional sociolinguistics
483
rely for their communicative import on someone’s interpretation, and this involves both propositional content and knowledge of the surrounding social world and its history. The social and the cultural therefore do not entirely exist apart from talk. Together they become intrinsic ingredients of the semiotics of human action. As a consequence understandings are culturally variable, that is they are neither universal nor entirely predictable from social positionings such as ethnic identity or socioeconomic class without consideration of local history and cultural tradition. The notion of style as a marker of distinctiveness is shared by several of the chapters in this volume where stylistic distinctions are constructed in the course of people’s shared history over a time period and their current adaptations to local conditions. The organization of social styles leads to variable stylization practices in which positioning is in opposition to other’s use of linguistic resources for indexing social identity as well the aesthetics of verbal form. Interactional sociolinguistics analysis enables in-depth examination of such variable stylization practices.
4.
An interactional sociolinguistics of style
Interactional Sociolinguistics (IS) is best described as the application of interpretive methods of discourse analysis to gain insight into social/cultural issues that tend to arise in today’s social environments by systematically looking at how speakers and listeners involved in these issues talk about them. Typically, IS research begins with the ethnographic study of everyday local practices, participant observation and interviewing in selected settings, to gain insight into communicative conventions and ideologies of interpersonal relations. The initial aim is to locate naturally organized situations (Garfinkel 1967) where the practices to be examined are likely to come up for discussion, and to collect tape or video recordings suitable for in depth study. For purposes of in-depth analysis extracts from these recordings are then divided into speech events. Broadly defined an event can be treated a any sequentially organized string of speech exchanges marked by a detectable beginning and an end that presents evidence of the event’s communicative outcome (Gumperz and Berenz 1993). Central to (IS) is the assumption that all communication is dialogically grounded in that it involves active collaboration among two or more individuals. Interlocutors’ interpretive assessments depend to a significant extent on the interactive processes through which communicative work gets
484
John J. Gumperz and Jenny Cook-Gumperz
done; meanings are finely tuned and negotiated. As empirical work on discourse and conversation has long shown, participants in such exchanges respond to what they assume others intend to convey. They do not base their interpretations on the denotational meaning of individual lexical items or on sentences extracted from discourse. IS argues that to account for the observable facts of interpretation we must broaden our view of how verbal communication works, and adopt a Peircian semiotic perspective which argues that understanding rests on symbolic signs that communicate via grammar and lexicon, as well as on indexical signs defined as a association between sign and context (Silverstein 1996; Lucy 1996). When seen in these terms communication is always intentional in that participants respond to their perceived understanding of the other’s communicative intent, and everyday language relies on simultaneously conveyed symbolic and indexical signs, one working in conjunction with the other. Understanding, in other words, goes beyond grammar and lexical knowledge and depends on additional inferential processes incorporating among other factors wider culturally based presuppositions. We have to assume that at any one point in a discursive exchange members must, if only in very general terms, agree on what activity they are engaged in, how it is framed and what the likely outcome are, as well on the conventions that underlie participants’ ongoing interpretations in the course of the interaction. Without such agreements there can be no conversational involvement. The conversational management process also relies on additional verbal and non-verbal signaling processes that are referred to as contextualization cues (Gumperz 1982, 1992) along with grammar and lexicon. Inferential processes are basically indexical, that is what is heard and seen at any one time is evaluated in order to retrieve the contextual grounds of what is communicatively intended. Since they are acquired in through everyday culturally communicative practice indexical signs are by definition highly culture bound and sensitive to subtle shifts in contextual presuppositions. The above perspective differs from commonly accepted notions of understanding in its treatment of cultural knowledge as an essential input to the interpretive process. Culture here affects interpretation in two ways. One, it provides background knowledge that we rely on to formulate tentative interpretations. Second, interpretations once made are then validated on the basis of the way they can be integrated into the event as a whole. Some researchers use the term social meaning to refer to indexically conveyed information, but we believe it is useful to draw a distinction between deno-
A postscript: Style and identity in interactional sociolinguistics
485
tational meanings, meanings can be extracted from context bound talk and listed in dictionaries, and indexes, based on culture and context bound inferences that are either validated or rejected by what happens in the course of the event. In this way understanding in communicative practice draws on elements of background knowledge (Garfinkel 1967), opening the way for further interpretations that go beyond first order indexing, by showing how listeners and speakers build up second orders of indexicality (Silverstein 1995). We use this latter term to refer to inferences made by participants in the course of a communicative event that construct an envisagement of an activity or communicative exchange. The construction process here involves additional culturally based knowledge indicating how specific aspects in the discourse are able to move beyond stereotypical categorizations of persons or situations to reach out into more abstract social worlds. As we have argued above, interactional sociolinguistics enables stylization practices, i.e. the processes by which speech varieties come to function as the means by which individuals set themselves off from others, to be explored analytically. Resources for stylization include among other things linguistic, prosodic, rhythmic and timing constituents together with gesture that act as contextualization cues in everyday talk. Other resources for stylization are semiotic particulars such as embodied features of dress and posture. Style switching or shifting relies on some or all of these working in co-occurrence with each other to achieve communicative ends. It is this issue of co-occurring constituents that makes styles of speaking creatively variable and marks a distinctive interactional sociolinguistic approach. We provide some brief examples of this process below. Other chapters in this volume also exemplify the process. The following two examples demonstrate how speaking styles operate to position participants in a bureaucratically mediated social world, showing how they fit or differ from the social categories made available to them. In other words the ideologically inscribed distinctions of language operate as resources to enable members of a group to select or highlight certain features of speech that others bound by the same ideology recognize. In example (1), professors in a large research university take part in a bureaucratically typical oral assessment of a student’s performance. In example (2), American Indians resident in a small California town, several of whom hold professional positions in the region, meet informally with two researchers to discuss their concerns over local interethnic relations.
486
John J. Gumperz and Jenny Cook-Gumperz
4.1. Example (1) As our initial example we present four excerpts from an American university oral Ph.D. examination for an older woman graduate student. The formal interview section of the proceedings has just concluded. Participants are casually seated around a table in a seminar room. Lee, the candidate, has left and is waiting outside as the professors discuss the completed dissertation: Adam, the chair, who is also her graduate advisor, Sherm, the most senior department member, James, a member of another department, and Pat, a junior woman faculty member. The talk, for the most part, takes the form of a casual discussion, marked by frequent false starts, clauses left incomplete and much hedging. Given this overall frame we further distinguish two modes of speaking: a) ‘on record evaluative style’ (bold type) with comparatively slow tempo, contoured intonation and relatively high incidence of technical terminology, and b) ‘off record style’ with more rapid tempo and less pronounced intonation contours. Consider the following initial comments by James, the outside member, who sums up what he has to say as follows: Excerpt (A)1 1 J:
I would certainly be in favor of, you know, (ral) passing her. (acc) Obviously with the kinds of suggestions we'll have for revision and so forth.
The passage begins in off record style, but with the phrase passing her the tempo momentarily slows to more measured pace and contoured intonation, of the kind that distinguishes on record evaluative talk throughout the transcript. Immediately afterwards the off-record talk sets in again. The effect is to set off the above phrase from the preceding and following discourse. Adam, the chair, then follows up. He begins with an informal personal anecdote about his own Ph.D. examination (not reproduced here). Then, after a brief pause he continues, picking up on James’ passing her in the preceding turn: Excerpt (B) 1 2 3 4
A: J: A: J:
So you say pass. Oh yeah. And that is ... that’s a neutral term. I’m not sure.
A postscript: Style and identity in interactional sociolinguistics
487
Note that grammar and lexical meanings cannot alone account for the dynamics of what “is going on” here. Clearly participants go beyond denotation to infer what is intended. But how are these inferences constructed? How are they grounded in the interaction? We assume that as faculty in the same institution all examiners are familiar with the basic examination procedures. They expect for instance hat evaluations are first proposed and then collaboratively and negotiated and agreed upon, unless someone dissents. Also, participants generally draw a distinction between formal assessments that enter into the written record, and informal opinions about the candidate that may for example affect how faculty members describe the candidate’ ability. Such procedural knowledge forms part of the general background knowledge that underlies interpretation. Note that although Adam’s turn has the surface form of a statement repeating information and ventrilocating the intonation contour of James’ prior evaluative turn. But James’ reply “oh yeah” in turn two treats it as a request. A likely inference is that James understands Adam as intending to convey something like: “What kind of a pass do you mean, how well has the candidate performed?” But since James does not elaborate we can assume that he does not want to be more specific. Adam's next remark in turn 3 begins in on-record style as if he were expecting a formal assessment. But then after a brief pause Adam repairs, shifting to off-record style with “That’s a neutral term” which can be interpreted as a request for further clarification, but this time an informal one. As we pointed out above, committee members share an understanding that academic assessments when first given may often be hedged. They know from past experience that the final evaluation will ultimately be collaboratively achieved and how they should be interpreted for the record is then determined cooperatively through further negotiations of the kind illustrated here. But additional forms of socio-cultural and linguistic knowledge also affect the outcome of the negotiation process. For example to comprehend any one utterance we must assume that in absence of cues to the contrary, the speaker intends that it should fit in with the agreed upon communicative goals of the encounter. Such assumptions of conversational cooperation motivate listeners to search for information given in preceding talk, as well as for other types of background information relevant to understanding of what has just been said. All of these affect the inferential process by re-
488
John J. Gumperz and Jenny Cook-Gumperz
trieving interpretive frames suggesting how message segments can be linked to create a thematic whole. One way in which contextualization conventions function is to serve as guide posts for monitoring the progress of the interaction. We use our knowledge of grammar and lexicon, along with contextualization conventions and whatever background information about settings and participants we have to decide what discourse task is being performed, what activity is being signaled and how it is framed. We then build on these predictions to identify the communicative intent that underlies particular utterances. Contextualization conventions channel interpretations in one direction or another. The interpretive assessments made at any one time are contingent assessments both for analysts and conversationalists. Once made, they are either confirmed or disproved by what happens subsequently. If they are confirmed expectations are reinforced, if they are disconfirmed we attempt to reinterpret what we have heard and change our expectations of the goals, outcomes and/or speaker’s intent. In the present situation where the examiners are members of the same academic community, we expect to find a fairly high degree of sharing of contextualization conventions and shared background knowledge. Our analysis reveals the degree to which interpretation of what goes on depends on this sharing and how the cues signal that the candidate is being evaluated. Adam’s anecdote has introduced a more informal, off-record speech style where the shift between on-record and off record styles conveys additional information that enters into the interpretation. In order to show how the stylistic parameters, once established, become essential to the interaction, we concentrate on two more passages. In the first of these James responds to Adam’s preceding turn. Apparently he is now ready to provide more, albeit informal, detail on what he thinks about the performance: Excerpt (C) 1 J: 2 3 4 5
Well/ I think..my sense is that..uh..she has done../A GOOD JOB/ ...and she certainly has..uh.../ I mean..this project for her is being everything and more I think..that a dissertation...should be/ ...and at the state of her professional development I think. a..a GOOD JOB / and she ought to be commended//
The above passage provides another illustration of what participants recognize as the on-record style, marked here by relatively slow enunciation and
A postscript: Style and identity in interactional sociolinguistics
489
the use of technical evaluative terms. The capitalized “a good job” which appears in the first line and is repeated in the last serves as a key evaluative expression. On each of its occurrences it is pronounced with the same unique prosodic contour, consisting of an increase in volume (here marked by capitals) and fall-rise fall intonation contour on “good” followed by fall to lower pitch fall on “job” which set off the phrase from others in the preceding passages as a formulaic expression. Such prosodic treatment suggests that more is intended than mere emphasis, but exactly and possibly intentionally what that is, is not spelled out in words. Excerpt (D) marks a highlight of the evaluation portion of the defense. Note that Sherm's contoured opening statement “it's a-fine job” ventrilocates the prosody and rhythm of James' formulaic “good job”. We can infer that he agrees with James's 'on record' evaluation or is at least willing to let it stand with all its ambiguities. His follow-up remarks, which take up the remainder of the first three lines below, begin by picking up on what James had said about the length of the dissertation. They are delivered in off record style with accelerated tempo that includes a chuckle suggesting that it is intended as a humorous off-record comment. Pat the junior faculty member’s laughter supports this. In turn 5, Sherm then goes on to what he marks prosodically as more substantive matters by returning to a rhythmically contoured on record style with short phrases, frequent pauses, and false starts with comments often preceded by introductory formulaic phrases that lexically mark the passage as an evaluation (line 5; 12; 22): Excerpt (D) 1 S: oh ...it's a FINE JOB//...by my/..lights uh.../ I would have 2 wished it was shorter//[laughs] (acc) I have some sympathy for 3 the twenty five/...page psych../ dissertations// 4 P: (laughing)...Right/ (clears throat) 5 S: Uh...the uh...uh...// this/...the one comment I'd 6 have/... ha-has to do with her writing/...uh these up// 7 ...uh uh her/...the dissertation was written/...was 8 written with the frame // these are the extent theories// lets 9 use these/...to derive hypotheses/ and get some data/ and cast 10 them against/..against the theories// (sigh) uh that's/ 11 that's/ fine// uh..uh.. but it also...uh/..al imit//...uh..because 12 it leads her/...for example not to/...ask..such questions as 13 the kind of things I was pushing her on a little bit uh/..what 14 alternative meanings/ might be given to/.. the uh..the/.. the 15 class/..variable// ...other than the socialization //
490
John J. Gumperz and Jenny Cook-Gumperz
16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33
it is true that/ in this literature/.. the class variable is interpreted as a socialization variable// ...but that’s not/... necessarily the/..case if you start/... from the more general question/ ..of how can we explain radicalism/...rather than the more particular question/..of.. ...given the theories currently used to explain/..radicalism// ...uh uh...uh she could I think/..write/.. some of this stuff up/..within the more limited frame//it'd be better it seems to me/ if she would/...expand her vision a little bit/...uh...so that she's not necessarily limited// ...although/ ...let me take it back//...in the longer run I would want her to do that// ...in the shorter run I'm not so sure// ...It may be wise to/ ...restrict her vision/...in order to ...uh...to/ ..to get some things done/ ... and now I think it's important for her/ ... to uh...to quickly move/ to get at least one piece out and in the literature/ just to give her the confidence that she can do it//
Given the speaker’s position as the senior member of the group his comments carry the authority that makes the interaction cohere as an oral examination. Yet analysis of the written transcript alone does not account for the talk’s communicative effect. In performance we see the interplay of several verbal and non-verbal channels, with both rhythmic patterning and contouring coordinated with head nodding and symbolically appropriate hand gestures. For example, in referring to what Lee, the candidate, could have done apart from what she did, Sherm makes a gesture of bringing his finger tips together in a flat tent-like arch; and then, as his slow rhythmic delivery continues, he resolves the problem he has set up in his talk. As he does so, he flattens the arch. Here we mention the kinesics only anecdotally. However the point is that discursive practice goes beyond lexical content by providing a rendering which has kinesic and gestural features that gives the impression of a fully developed summing up, and provide the members of the group with a sense that there has been a careful discussion of the thesis as an academic accomplishment. It is by means of such strategies that the speech legitimates the event as a properly conducted examination. From the key prosodic, paralinguistic and lexical strategies through which the communicative effect is achieved and from the chunking of the talk into phrases, we can infer that the speaker Sherm is thinking out loud.
A postscript: Style and identity in interactional sociolinguistics
491
The relatively high number of hesitation pauses, self corrections and asides, as well as the many incomplete clauses, tone groupings and pausings, all frequently violate our syntactically based expectations about clause boundaries Connections are frequently not lexicalized so that many individual clauses make no sense by themselves. The description dwells on the factual aspects of the candidate’s actions. Little direct evaluative vocabulary is used, so that seen from the perspective of propositional content, it is not immediately evident that the speaker is “doing evaluating” as such. One might of course paraphrase the first few lines as implicating that the candidate took existing theories for granted and that the work is limited because her theoretical position kept her from exploring alternative solutions. But it is left to the audience to infer such information. The passage appears to cohere mainly through its prosody and phrasing, just as it is transformed into an evaluative, ceremonial statement in part through its kinesics and body rhythm. Some of the incomplete utterances could be conflated to make two or three major points, in a manner more consistent with the performance style commonly associated with oral examinations. To construct such contracted summaries of the argument, however, would be to risk losing sight of the ceremonial/indexical character of the actual performance, and thus destroy its effect. To repeat our earlier remarks, in highlighting the role of prosody and the setting up discursive oppositions between on and off-record style we are not claiming that contextualization cues such as style switching convey ‘meanings’ in the semanticists’ sense. The claim is that contextualization cues in co-occurrence with other linguistic signs and background knowledge, lead us to frame the interaction in such a way as to favor certain classes of interpretations. Specific inferences as to what is intended are always locally negotiated. Excerpts (A) and (B) illustrate this point. When Adam uses contoured style to ask James to clarify his statement he is – given the sequential positioning of his question – implicating that he is interested in an on record judgment rather than in the specifics of James’s own personal opinion. In this, as well as elsewhere in the defense, what looks like a potentially “uncomfortable moment” that could lead to an overtly expressed differences of opinion is avoided, because the paralinguistics of the performance indirectly bring the organizational particulars to the surface. And by following up with the on-record committee talk Adam, as chair, implicitly reminds the other participants that it is now time to bring the formal proceedings to a conclusion. Such reminders are however in no way lexicalized and not made part of the official proceedings. To
492
John J. Gumperz and Jenny Cook-Gumperz
have done so would have been to engage in a controlling act with potentially altogether different interpretive consequences. We could say therefore that prosodic and other contextualization cues facilitate the smooth conduct of the defense. We can go further and say that the participants, in recognizing the on-record/off-record opposition, and in the systematic ways in which they react to each others cues, are cooperating not only in producing an academic evaluation, but recognition of group co-membership as academic gatekeepers. As Erickson (2004) refers to it, they recognize each others performed social identity or situational co-membership. This comembership then provides a warrant for any further use of on-record features to be seen as evaluative, and for further strengthening of the group’s bonds by off-record laughter and personal anecdotes.
4.2
Example (2)
The material in this example was obtained in cooperation with the defense team in a high profile Northern California trial, in which the initial verdict had been overturned by the court and a retrial ordered. Arguing that the case can only be understood on the basis of knowledge of local history, culture and interethnic relations, the defense asked the researchers to help with collecting background material. In order to do this the researchers participated in local discussions about relationships between the local Indian community and the White population organized by local residents for their benefit. The discussion takes place in a recently completed housing development belonging to the Karuk tribe, and friends and relatives of the defendant are present. The key speakers are all women. Speaker A is a community worker employed by the Karuk housing association, speaker B is a local dental technician and speaker C is another community member. The data consists of three excerpts from a longer three hour discussion touching on a range of issues concerning interrelations with the local community. Linguistic research in American Indian communities has traditionally concentrated, and to some extent continues to concentrate, on the grammatical systems of specific American Indian languages in order to save them from extinction. In recent years however linguistic anthropologists have turned to sociolinguistic investigations of the English spoken by Native American groups. Several initial findings of this work are of particular
A postscript: Style and identity in interactional sociolinguistics
493
significance for this discussion. Many English speaking Indian communities, particularly those who live in reservations separate from the surrounding white groups, tend to speak varieties of English that show characteristics of discourse level stress, rhythm and intonation and modes of argumentation reflecting those of parent American Indian languages. In addition Native American English rhetorical practices govern among other things the use of silence and matters of discourse organization: who speaks to whom, when and under what circumstances. Modes of indicating agreement or disagreement and the like, are to a significant degree carried over as well (Basso 1986). The transcript begins a few minutes into the discussion as the community worker tells an anecdote about what happened when the Karuk housing association was making plans for the construction of the homes. Excerpt (A)2 1 A: 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 B:
like when we purchased this LAnd out here; on the ROad up here, a:n; there w's a big WRiteup;= in the PAper,.. an people were get'n real’.. kinda’ ..LEery lo ya know, o:h the're gonna have a reserVAvation, ya know,