sfrucfura/ functions in music WALLACE BERRY Pnjssor q' Music Universiy Qf British Columbia
DOVER PUBLICATIONS, INC., NE...
1692 downloads
5955 Views
61MB Size
Report
This content was uploaded by our users and we assume good faith they have the permission to share this book. If you own the copyright to this book and it is wrongfully on our website, we offer a simple DMCA procedure to remove your content from our site. Start by pressing the button below!
Report copyright / DMCA form
sfrucfura/ functions in music WALLACE BERRY Pnjssor q' Music Universiy Qf British Columbia
DOVER PUBLICATIONS, INC., NEW YORK
Copyright © l976, 1987by WallaceT. Berry. All rights reserved underPan Americanand International Copyright Conventions. Published in Canada by General Publishing Company, Ltd., 30 Lesmill Road, Don Mills, Toronto, Ontario. Published in the United Kingdom by Constable andCompany, Ltd. This Dover edition, first published in 1987, is an unabridged and corrected republication of the work originally published by PrenticeHall, Inc., Englewood Cliffs,Newjerseyg in1976. Correctionshave been made by the author specially for this Dover edition. Manufactured in the United States ofAmerica Dover Publications, Inc., 31 East 2nd Street, Mineola, N.Y ll5Ol Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication
Data
Berry; Wallace. Structural functions in music. Reprint. Originally published: EnglewoodCliffs, NJ. : Prentice-Hall, 1976. Withcorrections by the author. Includes bibliographical references andindex. 1. Musical analysis. I. Title. MT6.B465S8 1987 781 86-31912 ISBN 0-486-25384-8 pbk.!
fo Maxine
contents preface to
the do ver edition,
preface to
the first
xi
edition, xiii
introduction, 1
CHAPTER ONE
tonality, 27 Introductory notes Introductory comments concerning tonal and linear functions The concept of multileveled function further explored The ideas of primacy and hierarchy among pitch-classesand pitch-class-complexes; genericand particular tonal systems Tonal systemsof compositions: regions and interrelations The concept of a generic tonal system exploredin theory Primary and secondary tonicsand their structural and auxiliary functions; multileveled multiple! tonal-harmonic function; tonic and dominant forms Essential and auxiliary linear functions of pitches and pitchcomplexes, their hierarchic basis often determined by tonal and cadential factors Tonal order as an inflation of harmonic order and succession Tonal fluctuation and techniques of immediate succession by which it is effected Tonal organization as a pattern of relative stability opposed to relative flux Tonal rhythm The questions of tonal intersection, direction, and distance; intervals of fluctuation; high-level chromatic successions and nondiatonic tonics; the interchangeability of modes and the equivalence of parallel tonics Concepts of tonal progression and tonal recession Concepts ofharmonic and melodic progressionand recession; complementarities and counteractions of element-successions VII
viii contents Some examplesof quasi-tonal order in melodic and composite functions Melodic curve Some further observations concerningmelodic analysis Some particular issues ofharmonic analysis in later styles Procedures ofanalysis; symbolic representations ofmelodic and harmonic functions and afliliations Gregorian chant, Veni creator spiritus Two preludes from Fischer, Ariadne Musica Bach, Drauf schliess ichmich in deine Hande, from Motet, Komm,Jesu, komm Beethoven, SymphonyNo. 2 in D, Op. 36, fourth movement; representations ofthe tonal system 'andtonal rhythm Wolf, Das verlassene Miigdlein, from Gedichtevon Miirike Ravel, Le Martin-Pécheur, from Histoires Naturelles Bartok, String Quartet No. 2, Op. 17, third movement Webern, Four Pieces, Op. 7, for violin and piano, No. l Berg, Four Pieces, Op. 5, for clarinet and piano, No. 4 Summary notes on the universality and significance of the principle of hierarchic tonal order Concluding notes
CHAPTER TWO
texture, 184 Introductory notes Textural progression, recession, andvariation as structural factors Types of musical texture; problems of classification and terminology Some further considerations ofterminology and aspects oftexture Texture and style Textural rhythm Qualitative and quantitative values Density and dissonance Interlinear independence and interdependence Imitation, a universal feature of many polyphonic styles; multiple counterpoint The activation of simple textures The complementa.ryand compensatory dispositions oftexture in relation to other element-structures Some textural functions in delineation of form Textural processesin progression toward intensity, in recession toward cadence, and in anticipation of thematic statement
COHIBDIS Texture as space Motivic texture; the provocative effect of unusual textures Levels of analysis and of hierarchy in the textural structure Gesualdo, Or, che ingioia credea viver contento from Madrigals, Book 4! Telemann, Fantasy No. 4 for violin alone, first movement Bach, Denn das Gesetzdes Geistes, from Motet, jew, maineFreude Brahms, Symphony No. 1 in C minor, Op. 68, first movement Un pocosostenuto!, introduction Dallapiccola, Goethe-Lieder for soprano and three clarinets No. 1, In tausend Formen! Serialism and texture; texture as a product of chance operations Concluding notes
CHAPTER THREE
rhythm and
meter, 301
Introductory notes Fundamental concepts of rhythm Rhythm as activity and motion Rhythmic pattern as motivic The rhythms of element-successions A theoretical approach to the consideration of meter as accent-delineated grouping The concept of meter as, by definition, subject to fluctuation Meter as one manifestation of grouping in music Meter as opposed to the notated bar-line Impulses and their functional differentiations Functions of the cadential conclusive! impulse Criteria of accentuation Further comment concerning diverse impulse functions Levels of metric structure and analysis Metric irregularity: horizontal and vertical noncongruity The problem of preconditioning metric structure and syncopation Stability and flux; metric progression and recession; compensatory and complementary functions in relation to other element-structures Beethoven, Symphony No. 8 in F, Op. 93, first movement Chopin, Prelude in E, Op. 28, No. 9 Webern, Three Pieces, Op.ll, for cello and piano, No. 3; and Five Pieces, Op. 5, for string quartet, fourth movement Some twentieth-century problems in rhythm and meter; recent developments in serialism of durational units-theory and practice Concluding notes
X COI7f6I`lfS APPEND/X ONE editorial notes,
425
APPEND/X TWO translations, 427
index of
musical examples
index of
subjects, names,
and citations, and terms,
437
433
preface tothe doveredition
It has been more than a decade since Structural Functionsin Music was first
published. Thus, I should perhaps affirm at the outset of this new editionwhich is not a revision-that although my views and particular correlative analytical approacheshave undergonecertain refinementsand redirectionsof formulation evident in a number of publications during the intervening years, thefundamental ideasand methodsexplored in this book remain, for me, validand useful.These includethe important concept ofmusical structure and significative shaping as in one critical sense! an undulation between tendencies ofprogression andrecession. This books three essaysconstitute one testimony among many to the deepening preoccupationin the professional communityof musictheory with allied andoften parallelquestions aboutstructural unities, organic substance, and modesof expressivelydirected fluctuation in all of musics cofunctioning elements. Sinceits appearancein 1976,Structural Functions has beena factorin, and has directly stimulated, abundant discussionof various and frequently innovative approaches to studies of the shaped elementsof music. Such proliferating studies-complementary or refutative, and often speculativehave appearedin the form of conference papers,unpublished researchby young professionals,including many student theses,and a substantial stream of published work. Indeed, probing inquiries into musics melodic and harmonic dimensions, texture, rhythm, and other elements continue, as they must, without end. The complexities ofpreliminary and derivative questions about the expressive andintellectually provocative properties of music are such that the theorists quest is assuredly notfor final answers, whoseunattainability we know enough to know, but rather for deepened channels of understanding consistentwith our responses tomusic assensitive musicians. I wish in this brief prefatory statement toexpress thehope thatthis book will continue to be a basisfor respondentand pursuant investigations, asone of a growing number of theoretical works treating related issuesin disparate ways, yetalways to the end of better understanding ofstructure and effect in music. It is, to be sure, a matter of gratification to me that the general
xi
xii preface
to the dover edition
accessibility of Structural Functions in Musicwill besignificantly broadened as a result ofits republicationas aDover paperback. WALLACE BERRY Wncouveg 1987
preface tothe firstedition
Major premises and objectiveswhich are the basisfor thesestudies are fully set forth in the Introduction; the work can be characterized, simply and in summary, as an inquiry into tonal, textural, andrhythmic structures in music, and into conceptual and analytical systems for the study of these fundamental elements.While assuming the readers understanding of basic principles of -tonal form and analysis, this book carries ideas concerning tonality, texture, and rhythm-their processive functions and structural configurations-into the analytical discussion of many works and extracts ranging from early modal styles to recent times. Much important theoretical work that has provided foundation for my thinking about tonality conceived as a principle relevant far beyond the conventions of the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries!, and by which I have been informed and
motivated, will
be evident
to readers
who are
familiar with traditions, and with certain recent procedures of inquiry, in music theory. Such sources and points of departure which include the theoretical works of Schoenbergas well as important literatures embodying and reflecting concepts of hierarchically ordered structural elements! are cited at appropriate stagesin this book. At the sametime, I have soughtto bring about a presentation of ideas in conformity with certain essential underlying premisesand in the interestof consistentexposition ofa pervasive point of view which is, essentially,mine. The studies in texture and rhythm have few if any significant, direct antecedents; the work in those two areas Chapters 2 and 3! is largely in original formulation. Many of the ideasand approachesset forth in this book have been the bases forgraduate and to some extent undergraduate courses andseminars
in musictheory inwhich, atthe Universities of Michiganand BritishColumbia, I have beenprivileged to explore with inquiring and highly committed student collaboratorsthe lexical,methodological, andconceptual systems and problems of the three areas of concern. The responsiveness, butmore the challenges andinitiatives, of students in these studies,and in research stemxiii
xiv preface
to the first edition
ming from them, hasbeen ofthe greatestbenefit tome in the preparationsfor this book and the cultivation of
ideas central to its content.
A seminaron textureproved tobe thesource of at leastthree extensive research studiesof which I am aware, and that on rhythm and meter the source of others at early and advanced levels of graduate study, not to mention numerous searching papersof more limited scope. The dissertation of Dr. Anne Hall Texture inthe ViolinConcertos Stravinsky, of Berg, Schoenberg, and Bartok ; Ann Arbor: University Microfilms, 1971! brought to impressive refinement theoreticaland practical systems inthe realm of texture, applying them in probing analysis of significant extracts from the four subject works. I wish it were possible to list the names of other student contributors, some of them engaged in important research at this time. During crucial stages in this work the longer periods of its inception and conclusion! I have enjoyed accessto the resources andfacilities of the Music Division of the San FranciscoPublic Library and the Music Library of the University of California; my indebtedness tothe staffs of both is very substantial indeed. And I owe special thanks to Ilene Silverman, of the Music Library at the University of Michigan, who helped me in many ways during the year in which the book was completed. My colleagues, Professors IleneOlken, John DArms, and Otto Graf, advised me with respect to translations of texts in certain works cited as examples. A number of publishers have kindly granted authorization for the reprinting of copyrighted materials, and while these areacknowledged in onpage citations in the text I should also take this opportunity to express my appreciation to all of them, and especially toBoosey andHawkes, Inc., and Theodore PresserCompany, for permissions toquote from restricted works. Obviously, a book like this is quite literally impossible without such permissions.
My wife, to whom this book is dedicated, has helped in many ways, even in such tiresome chores as proofreading, but particularly, and critically, in her quiet encouragement through pressured times, and in her everlasting patiencewith my brooding preoccupationsduring such times. I thank her for a companionate, sustaining presence, and for sharing with me so overtly the exhilaration of achievement, whatever its measure, as this work has evolvedin many places towhich we have travelled, and where we have found composure in memorable natural ambiences, during these past seven years.
Finally, I am grateful to have learned a great deal, through much reading, discussion, and analysis preliminary to and concurrent with the preparation of this book, especially aboutJosquin, Beethoven,and Webern, and therebyabout whatI think to bebroadly relevantfactors inthe experience of music. WALLACE BERRY 1975
strucfura/ functions in music
introduction
Outside of simplest genres,it is unlikely that we ever attain full understanding of a particular musical experience, so complex are its elements, their actions,
and interactions.
This book
seeks to move toward
a better
understanding of structure and experience; it does soin systematic exploration of the elements of structure and their important interrelations, laying out a variety of approaches to the analysisof directed successions ofevents involving tonality, melody, harmony, texture, and rhythm-each of these treated throughout much of its range of potential operations. These four studies thusseek togive comprehensiveexposition to particular syntactic processes inwhich music can besaid to have meaning,and to illuminate in penetrating analysesof works of many kinds the procedures by which structural elements in nearly all music function expressively. Amajor concern is the consideration of relations between specificformulations and expressive eH`ect in significant works, and a number of concernsare treated in conceptual, methodological, terminological, and analytical systems relevant to such particular structural formulations. One sets out in a project of this kind in the hope, to use Edmund
Wilsons hauntingwords, ofbeing cured of someache ofdisorder, relieved of someoppressive burdenof uncomprehendedevents. I cite thesewords not because Ican hopeto have achieved with consistency soelusive anobjective, but becausethey reflect so eloquently the intensity of purpose and commitment to which a writer on serious mattersis dedicatedwho seeksto put forth systems ofanalysis and thought by which understanding can be induced. To a large extent, explorative emphasis centers here in theories of rhythm and texture, structural parameters towhich relatively little attention is given in the existing literature of music theory. A large-scale scholarly, philosophical, analytical, and systematic penetration of these two vital factors in musical structure and experience is an undertaking of awesome difficulty; yet, any broad, probing effort toward systemsby which rhythmic 1
2 introduction
and textural processes can be investigatedconstitutes, tothe extent that it is successful, aneeded anduseful stepin the direction of better understanding of theseand related critical factors in the musical experience. Although concernswith rhythm and texture are, in the somewhatmore pioneering efforts they represent,of central importance in this work, related and interactive tonal, harmonic, and melodic systems are examined as parallel shaping factors in musical structure. Moreover, by recurrent reference tointerrelations among element-systems, reciprocal and analogical correspondences areindicated in which the actions of individual elements are seen to project expressiveshapes of progressive, recessive, static, or erratic tendencies. Progressiveand recessive intensifying and resolving! processes are seenas basicto musical effect and experience. In the effort to subject to theoretical expositionand analytical explication a consistent, fundamentalview of .syntactic relations in music, a strongly permeative value is, by implication, embraced: a belief in the importance and necessityof logical analysis of the musical experience, of the study of objective data derived in the analysis of structure and experience. That experience canbe regardedas thesum of responses attributableto particular musical processes,the action-reaction complex set up whenever there is perception and enhanced whenthere iscognition! of musical stimuli issuing in contexts in which syntactic relations are cultivated and controlled as the result of disciplined creative acts. I strongly aflirm that belief in logical analysis, andin the necessity andworth of the pursuit of rational inquiry into the musical experience. At the sametime, I see that mode of inquiry as one in which conjectural hypothesisand intuition where intuition is the creative fusion of acquired knowledge and experience! are_ vitalin triggering necessary questionsand answers,and in suggesting interpretationswhich can be examined for plausibility and, at times, susceptibility to empirical verification.
Thus, the many analysesin which theoretical premisesare throughout this book brought into focus are directed to consideration of the vital question: How does musicspeak, andwhat is the nature of its language? Or, to what specyic irylection.r can we besaid torespond with understanding and _keling ?The study of particular syntactic techniques and procedures, howevertentative its conclusions,can render the expressivecommunication of musical meaning less mysterious than it is oftenthought and sometimes wistfullyhoped! to be; The distinction intended isthat betweentwo levelsof awareness in theexperience of music, twolevels goingbeyond simple hearing: thecognitive levelis regardedas thatinvolving understandingas opposedto that of simpler affective sensory apprehension. Thisdistinction isrelated tothat betweenknowing 'andfeeling, insightand innocentreaction; both kinds offactors arecritical in the musicalexperience.
introduction 3
indeed, the belief that logical insights can be had into relations between ltructure and effect underlies all productive aesthetic inquiry. It is a belief proudly affirmed in this book. The business ofmusical analysisis to consider the nature of functionsand expressive #set in the tones and rhythms of which music is made.
Surely it is clear that any serious investigationof structure and effect, to the extent of its validity, must significantly inform the critical evaluation, stylistic understanding, and interpretation of music, while laying important bases fordecision in musical creativity. Beyond this general statement one can do little more than affirm with the most positive emphasis that the understanding ofstructure and effect in all musical elements andin their interactions isof very decisive importancein all serious, professionalendeavor in music, andto the listener-participant as well. Moreover, a theoretical system which affords useful commenton the relation of structure to expressive effect will suggest many significant and necessary parallels linking music with other, especiallytemporal, art forms. " The performer, for his special part, must fully understand the functional-expressive basisand significance of energizingand subsidentelementproeesses which it is his challenge to portray and project. However, I believe the path from analytical insight to interpretive decisionis oneof considerable complexity. Conclusions drawn from the analytical undertaking do not lead directly and unequivocally to particular interpretive decisions; rather, the performermust often-perhaps usually-make difficult judgments within a range of plausible solutions for example, whether in performance to underscore bythe slight adjustment oftempo anessential recessive process!. For now, in the interestof achievinga rigidly defined, circumscribedfundamental purpose, I have had to be content with a restricted number of suggestive references toproblems of interpretive decision in the examples towhich this book refers. Nonetheless, suchreferences occur,especially in Chapter 3. I believe a great deal of understanding of musical process,in its essential terms, to be accessible tothe involved layman or amateur. Indeed, many of the most persuasive factorsin musical effect and function are delineative of shapes andprocesses thatcan be demonstrated, given necessary theoretical and analytical calculations, relatively simply. A related view holds that many of musics most immediate and compelling strengthsderive from the shaped actions of elements of primitive substance and effect e.g., those of dynamic changes, or timbral differences!. Nevertheless, the thorough analysisof all the elementsof structure in their confluent and con2See also relevant statementsin the Preface tothe authors Form inMusic, 2nded. Englewood Cliffs,N ._].:Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1986!.
4 introduction
tiguous operationsat all relevant hierarchic levels isan issueof sophistication and complexity-and one that must constitute a significant and essential part of the competenceand experienceof the professional musician. In an important sense, this book is concerned with groupingof many kinds and at diverse levels~event-groupings goingbeyond thoseof form,the articulation of units by formal cadential, thematic! procedures. In general, structureis discussedas toits functional and expressiveconsequences within an intensity curve delineated by groupings and controlled associations ofevents underlying nearly all composed music. In music that is composed asopposed tomusic of random operations or random consequences!, actionschanges, events!involving various elements lines of pitch change, tonal and harmonic succession, rhythm and meter, texture, and coloration! are so conceived and controlled that they function at hierarchically ordered levels in processes by which intensities develop and decline, and by which analogousfeeling is induced. Element-actionsmay converge in collaborative, directed lines of change; or, probably more commonly, certain prevailing lines of change function toward a particular expressive endwhile others, subordinate to the essentialfunctional tendency, are counteractive.
Examples of universal musical processes canbe indicated in observations ofthe sort: the music iscoming to a close, or the music is advancing toward a peak of intensity, or the music is in a tentative state, with something of 'special consequence about to emerge, and comparable cognitions. In the first of these instances,the processof closure, predominant elementactions of necessity followa course of decline but at the same time others may and often do resist that essential structural tendency. For example, crescendo, or increase in textural density, or, in cases of tentative internal closure, even increased textural diversity, may result in relatively active cadential process: li92
Essential to this concept is the principle that the senseof motion in music passage from one event or state to another! largely, although not exclusively, dependson change within one or more element-successions. From this premise, it follows that where there is not change there is in critical respects immobility. For example, in the fixed repetitions of an ostinato pattern at the samepitch level there is motion at only the lowest level of structure-that of profiled configuration within the pattern itself Eventfulness in music demands that iteration without change, at any specified level, occur only within
limits: to
the point of such limits iteration
can be
introduction 5
intensifying, while beyond theselimits, diflicult to define, tedium and tautology result. Motion in music may, in principle, be erratic, but its usual condition is one of directed activity-courses of change-in lines of _growth ordecline at various levels. A line of decline at one level maybe observedto be subsumed within a broader line of growth at another.! The punctuation and proportionalization of such linesof changeconstitute animportant aspectof rhythm i.e., are expressive ofelement-rhythms and meters which may or may not converge!. The successions of element-events trace shaped, controlled profiles-a concept arising analogously in the sense oftemporal paths and, wherelines ofaction crossfields ofpitch, of shapes in space. Musical structure may be saidto be the punctuated shaping oftime andspace into lines of growth, decline, and stasishierarchicalbv ordered. Of course, not all elements actin commensuratesignificance, or even act at all, in given instancesof suchconcurrent linesof change.For example, it is clear that the processof digressionfrom and resolution into a primary tonal systemwill have little or no significancein certain styles; or the action of melodic line in rising and falling, conjunct and disjunct, pitch successions may in particular instances be in part irrelevant or utterly inapplicable. In the analysisof the interactive and hierarchically related! element-actions in expression ofparticular functional processes the initial question is, thus, that which seeksto determine within whichparameters contributive actions occur. As to general relative importances of such actions in perception and cognition of functional-expressive effect,we will make conjecture from time to time~speculating, for example, that such immediate, direct, and primitive actions as changesin dynamic intensity or textural density have more palpable persuasiveforce than a relatively elusive factor like that of tonal fluctuation. But, in general, the questionof relativesignificances ofcorrelative actions in cognitive experience is a difficult issue which this book does not attempt to explore. Element-actions in music function in certain basic formal processes or activities which can be rather simply stated, assumingit is understood that there is an infinite range of possible andpalpable degreesof change, rates of change, and specific configurations of interrelations among contributive elements-and hence of expressiveeffect conceivable and potential within such formal processes. Such fundamental classificationsof formal process are those of ! introduction-often involving dissonant prolongation as a primary factor of expectant intensity in precarious balance with quiescent, 3Presumably, the limited capacityof pure iteration for intensification isaccountable to the rising expectationof inevitabledigression.
6 introduction
tentative conditions such as relative textural sparsity and slow tempo; ! statement-normally in a context of 'relativestability; ! restatement-often involving variation, but again in relatively stable conditions; ! transition and development-both normally relatively fluctuant; and ! cadence-in an environment of relative decline and deceleration. Someaspects offormal technique are characteristic of particular styles and, of course, particular systems; thosemost representativeof broadly prototypical formal procedures are explored in many existing works. The present work addressesitself to theoretical discussionand analysis of certain growth-connected and decline-connected! intensity-activity shapes inwhich functional and expressivelines ofchange arevery commonly conveyed. Although certain formal processes areby definition associated with particular functional tendencies e.g., that of the cadential processwith recessive action!,in general, progressive andrecessive linesof change occur variously in diverse formalcontexts. Indeed,music isconstantly involved in both concurrent and contiguous event-relations! in a dialecticin which opposing tendenciesof growth and decline and their correlatives up-down, far-near, dense-sparse,simple-complex, etc.! are in continual interplay at different hierarchic
levels.
Formal processesand element-actionsof growth and decline are thus both discretely identifiable and interdependent asaspects ofform and structure, and the study of lines of changing intensities yields significant understanding ofparticular compositional techniques bywhich formal processes are carried out. The primary concern of this book is the analysis of structure apart from formal processas such,but it cannot fail at the same timeto note that particular functional tendencies are critically allied to essential and universal techniquesof form. The idea of music as dialecticalbin balance between intensifying and resolving tendencies, and involving complementary or counteractive relations, brings to mind a moving, terse statementby Pousseuron the third of
Weberns ThreePieces, Op.ll, for cello andpiano discussed as to metric structure onpp. 397-400of thisbook!. . . . it is through the positioning of well-defined and clearly perceptible units, articulating themselves bymeans of reciprocal effect, each one limiting and defining the nature of its fellows. Thus an available space is established among the units, an emptiness within which they may reign calmly in all their abundance. Symmetry and asymmetry, determination and indetermination, equality and inequality, are thus more than simply contradictory principles excluding each other in an absolute manner; rather, they are complementary properties, each conditioning the other and mutually dependent. An excess inone sense These includethe authorsForm inMusic, citedmrlier.
introduction 7 or the other leadsto the same pathologicaldisorder. Only a correct proportion, a balanced tension which may be realized in an infinitely varied manner! can engender afree order, vital and significant, representingmultiplicity and communication, individuality and recognition?
Pousseur istalking about relations of units; yet, the remarkable spectrum of expressive possibilitieswithin the range of musical techniques is immediately apparent when one-thinks of further potential relations among units of dwrent but concurrently functional! elements, and of the cofunctioning, complementary or counteractive, relations of such element-units at diverse hierarchic levels of structure. The musical situation and its experiencepose inthis light a challenge of great complexity, yet one of beguiling interest and inescapable significance. To see istructural function in music as it concerns lines of intensity
change is to see,in general,three possibilities: that ofincreasing intensity to which we apply the term progression!, that of subsiding intensity to which the term recession is applied!, and that of event-succession involving unchanging degrees ofintensity stasis!.° Of the latter, it is noted that true immobility is probably inconceivable at the lowest structural level; even a successionof sound events of equal qualities can be viewed as consisting of attack-decay shapes in each of which it may be reasonable topostulate recessionin theory and experience. At the same time,the principle of stasis,even whileit cannot be said to have absolute applicability throughout a leveled structure, is of practical importance. For example, repetition of a motivic unit in which parametric factors by which the motive is characterizedremain unchanging to the extent that this is possible in performance which is not mechanical! is, for all practical purposes, staticat levels beyond that of the motive itselli The concept of musical motion iscritically allied to the concept of progressive, recessive, and static events andevent-complexes. Tothe extent that motion is a useful concept in musical experience,it may be said to reside in factors ofthree kinds,of which the mostimportant is that involving changing qualities in contiguous sonorousevents.
Henri Pousseur,The Question of Orderin NewMusic, in PerspectivesNew qf Musab, V, l 966!, pp. 104-5; translationby David Behrman. °It is assumed thatcertain structural conditions generallybut in random contexts only accidentally!are expressive of intensity,others ofrelease. Such conditions areevocative of feeling-of anxiety, of calm, of exhaltation, etc.,not really explored hereas such,but implied. _ This underlying assumption isin somedegree subjectto conditionsof probability, predictability, andfamiliarity within the respondentsrelation to a particular style, system, or rhetoric. Thus, anyassumed intensity scale orstate within a givenstructural parameter depends inpart bothas tokind anddegree onthe understanding or intuition of probabilities within the terms ofa particularstyle andsystem.
8 introduction
First, in a purely temporal sensea successionof even equal! sound events, suchas onethat consistsof evenly spaced strokeson a drum lacking dynamic or other changes, is felt as moving in time. Thus, passage through a temporal Held is felt as a kind of motion-a stream of eventfulness whichcourses throughtime. Even when a stretch of time is vacant
unpunctuatedevents, by inasQ !, itis bounded by events at itsextremities whose connection-a relation reaching across the vacant temporal field-denotes passage as motion of a kind. A secondand far more critical factor of motion in musical experienceis that associatedwith successionsof sound events havingchanging qualities. As suggested, suchchange in element-actions isnormally traceable in, at given levels, consistentdirections of increasing or receding intensity,but it may, at given levels,be or seem! arbitrary and erratic too. In any event, the succession ofa sound eventby another qfdW:rent qualiy'or qualitiessay, two events of different degrees ofloudness! conveysan analogicalimpression ofmotionof a distance between disparate qualitative states havingbeen traversed. Thus, in a sense,in music, change ismotion. A third factor has to do with the illusion of a spatial Held in music delineated by the pitch ambitus inherent in the spectrum of perceptible frequencies; this ofcourse can be viewed, within the scope ofthe secondfactor described above,as motion implicit in change within the element of pitchline, of melody. But obviously it has special significance and is usefully regarded asa distinct factor. To the extent that succeedingsound eventsof changing pitches are felt as coursing over an analogical spatial field a broad leap between twopitches isfelt as going farther than a succession linked by a conjunct intervallic relation!, the eventscan reasonablybe saidto describe motion within that field. Thus it is that lines actively cross or are related diagonally or triangularly in musical texture. With reference to all of thesefactors, this book embracesa concept of motion in music presupposing,in its direction and control, a value which inheres in significant musical expression ofall times. It was earlier stated that directed, functional-expressive elementsuccessions havethree conceivable tendencies: progression,recession, and stasis that of aimless or arbitrary lines of change in some music would constitute a fourth, although its consequenceis likely to be one of relative stasis atgiven levels!. In any real musicalsituation severalelements arelikely to be active, and these do not necessarily-in fact are not likely to-correspond with consistency indelineating progressive,recessive, orstatic concurrent lines of change. Moreover, when element-successions move in parallel, or complementary, directions, theyoften do so, assome analysesin this book will show, at differing rates of ascent or descent in the intensity lineat differing rhythms of element-change.
introduction 9
Within these concepts, musical structure if form is seen as the thematic-developmental scenario! can be regarded as the coryluenceof shaped lines of element-succession which either agree are complementary! in intensity direction or disagree are mutually counteractive, orcompensatory! in direction. Within the range of musical elementssubject to control within these procedures,some are of course of greater relevance to certain styles than others; but the underlying concept of the confluence of functional element-actions as fundamental to
musical structure
and effect is of crucial
significance in all styles. We may posit, for example, certain common kinds of element-action having function analogous tothat which is clearly pertinent within the loudsoft - 1 ;! spectrum of effect.Some seemrelatively clearly demonstrated in identifiable musical experience; othersare of more conjectural functional significance. Une thinks of rising pitch, shrinking units acceleration!, increased textural quantity, increased compressionof texture, crescendo, harmonic succession ofincreasing tonal distance, increasein the overall spatialHeld, Huctuationtoward morepenetrating coloration,increased rate of attack, and dissonance, amongother musical states commonly associated with
increased intensity.
Such atentative and preliminary discussiontakes noaccount, ofcourse, of the question of rates ofchange, which may be gradual or sudden, or anything within these extremes, with profound consequences for expressive effect. And it has beennoted that confluent actionsoften differ in rates of changein angles of descentor ascent.The factor of rate of changein element-successions, ofthe degree ofincline or decline in the angleof ascentor descent, is a highly important and pervasive aspect ofrhythm in music, one of the most telling factors in the perception of motion and eventfulness. As a matter of fact, temporal
segments in music are identifiable, in
one sense, as
discrete stagesin lines of element-change, as rhythmic units of one kind, their progressive or recessive shortening or lengthening acceleration, deceleration! expressing intensification and release of further functional significance within the rhythmic parameter. Implicit in all of this is the thesis that contiguous sound eventsmanifesting changewithin any parameter and in any degree resultin functionalexpressive effectof, to that extent, intensity change. Put another way, the premise is that no change distinguishing contiguous sound events can be neutral with respect tointensity. Thus, pitch change, however slight, is suggestive ofmodification in the degreeof intensity, as areany and all changesin tonal reference, harmonic content, rhythmic activity, textural complexity 7It is by now clear that intensity is regarded asa product of qualitiesiorstates involving usuallymany concurrentlyoperative parameters;as suchintensity can only be defined inthe listing,conjectural ordemonstrable, of such qualities.Particular intensifying conditions ofmelodic, harmonic,tonal, textural, and rhythmic structures arerecurrcntly discussed as a fundamental,continuing concernin this book.
10 introduction
and quantity, metric structure, and coloration. Of course,in accord with the necessary conceptof hierarchic levels .of structure, it can be stated that change ata relatively low level is of inferior structural significance relatively neutral! in contexts ofa higher level-i.e., in broader implications. Thus, the pitch successiona-b-a is in a sense, atthe level of the total unit, unchanging; the progression to b, in the total context, is in a sense neutralsubsidiary to an underlying, fixed pitch event. But at the circumscribed level short of resolution a-b- ! the modestascent inpitch and minute tonal departure! has identifiable functional-expressive significance which can be felt as important in a leveled structure and experience. A theoretical formulation in which vital factors of musical meaningare attributed to controlled directions of growth and decline in lines of elementchange requires,as suggested above, hypothesisand observationwith respect to the connotations of various qualtities in the dialectical intensity-release scale of values.
Further such assumptions arestated, in a summary way, in the following outline; a number of them will be regarded ashaving a kind of commonsense logic e.g., complexity = intensity, or the forte-piano dialectic!,others have the support of traditional acceptance e.g., dissonance= intensity!, while others emerge asof more tentative significance.At the same time, the theory of functional-expressive significancein controlled lines of change within structural element-actions with shaped tendenciesto and from points of intensity at different levels hasintrinsic importance apart from any specific formulation of connotative valueswithin the intensity-release scale. While any premature statement in the direction of classification of intensity values within elements of music is inevitably too simple, it can be useful ifread assuggestive only,and of very restrictedscope ofconcern. These ideas are extensively developedin the three studies to follow. In Fig. 0-l specific manifestations of recessive tendency are not indicated but are of course oppositeto the kinds of events characterizedas progressive. Reasoned premisesconcerning the functional-expressive connotations of events and kinds of events! and concerning their interrelations and confluent parallel or resistant tendencies andinteractions are indispensable in analysis. And since inthe complexity of real music thelikelihood of comprehensive verification, or even susceptibility to verification, is uncertain, analytical judgment is commonly one of carefully reasoned, interpretive The question of ambiguityin particular element-qualities requires distinct, special consideration. Inambiguous conditions, by definition, the cognition of structure must be considered uncertain, and functional-expressive significance would depend onthat cognition. Where apparently ambiguous conditions are understood as ambiguous, in a context in which ambiguity is not a normal state, the effect is presumablyintensifying. Thisproblem is treated further,but not extensively, inChapter 3.
introduction Fig. 0-1. Somepremisesrespectingintensity valveswithin the spectrumof qualitiespertaining to eachof certainfundamentalelementsof musicalstructure. Element Melody,a line of contiguous pitches Harmony,the line of harmonic succession Tonality~the line of tonal reference Meter, the successionof accent-delineatedunits
Progressive action: Up; leap expecting closure, especially when dissonant; instability of tonal or other felt tendency Away from tonic; dissonant; inverted; complex forms; chromatic deviation from primary diatonic resource! Awayfrom primarysystem, in relation to tonal "distance" and assumingreferential adherence of primary I; chromatic succession and expansion Towardshorterunits; asymmetry and fluctuation; clarity of more frequent accent acceleration!; toward instability, departure from relational unit norm Acceleration in rate of occurrence at given level Greakr interlineardiversityand conflict; increased density; wider spatial field
Tempo,or rhythmic "pace" Texture,the line of changes in numbers and interactions of components Increasedsonorousweight and penetration strings Timbre,eventsinvolving did b .!;I d coloration, dynamic level, higher registerssharper "focus" of intense registral change, articulation color; more percussive,stressedarticulation
choice. Within any associationof partly counteractive confluent elementactions,the analytical judgment of prevalencei.e., what tendencydominates and is germane to broad structural efFectand intent, is of course extremely critical.
Stasis is nonaction, to whatever extent this can pertain, within any parameter at any given level. We have noted that stasis,being "unnatural" and expecting the intervention of change, is short of the point of tautology intensifying, and usually counteracted by eventfulnesswithin someopposing parameter.
Within tonal structure, for example, stasisdoes not necessarilydenote neutrality of intensity: indeed, prolongation e.g., of V! is common as intensifying technique. But intensifying stasisof tonal condition almost invariably involves dissonance,often gradually increasinglevels of dissonance,as a distinct factor significantly complicating an apparent condition of tonal immobility. Dissonanceis, by definition, active and mobile and must be regarded, in one sense,in its discrete significance. One seriousaspectof the oversimplicity of Fig. 0-1 is its incompleteness in the listing of parameters of potential action; at each stage in this book primary attention is given to the fullest possible statement and illustration
of
12 introduction
areas of potential functional-expressive action within each broad element classification. This is carried out with exhaustive attention to quoted examples, and to the presentation, development, and illustration of many techniques of analysis applied to these examples atall stagesof discussion. Lines of element-change controlledand directed along profiles of appreciably increasing or decreasingintensity are common to all music except that limited literature of very recent time in which concurrences and conHuences of sound eventsare unforeseen,and of purely orsignificantly arbitrary association.
Of course, the associationof tonal distance with intensity would have radically diminished relevance tothe extent that a style isatonal; dynamic progression and recession in loudness levelsare of unlikely significance in fifteenth-century polyphony; or, in twelve-tone music,the predetermination of pitch-class PC! content in simultaneous aggregates mayalthough it does not necessarily! result in diminished significance in functional-expressive contextual control of, say, dissonance-consonance values within that parameter and, correlatively, increased significance in other element-structures. Moreover, that the mannerin which elements are articulated i.e., the techniques ofelement-change! differsamong stylisticapproaches canbe noted, for example, in strikingly different applications of the rhythmic factor of tempo change in Baroque asopposed toRomantic styles,or of melodic and textural change invocal asopposed toinstrumental polyphony. These areonly a few examples ofparticular relevances of particularactions inparticular styles. Still, it is a soundbasic premisethat the shaping oflines ofelement-change isan all but universal factor in musical function and expression;and certain elements are applicable across virtually all historical-stylistic boundaries, notably those of texture and rhythm.
Techniques ofprogression andrecession inlines of change are,then, in different ways significant in the relation between structure and expressive function in some importantdegree inall music in which controlled contextual function is a compositional objective and aesthetic desideratum.With this assumption, analytical effort must be directed to those elements which are functional in the particular style or medium, or particular work in question. The concept of progressiveand recessiveactions within confluent element-structures suggeststhe useful basic principle that, in an important °It is clear, too,that the particular mediumof expression significantly conditionsthe prevalence ofcertain elements over others,or to the exclusionof others.Thus, forexample, in solomedia thetimbral spectrumis limited to registral,dynamic, andarticulative differences, inaddition tosuch devices as mutingor otherpreparations ofthe particular medium -a fundamental distinction ascompared with the broad spectra ofmore heterogeneous media..
introduction 13
sense, there are dissonancesana' resolutionswrithin allqfmusics parameters. The clear relevanceof this idea is apparent with; even a little thought: thus, for example, anupward leap in the pitch-line, a highly active perhaps imitative! texture of competing lines,relative ambiguity or in relation to a referential norm! asymmetry of metric relations-any of these can,like other, comparable conditions of instability and expectancy of restoration of simpler states, beconceived asdissonant, as evocatiive ofintense feelingand subjective involvement in potently absorbing actiomsand interactions taking place within the projected medium of sound events. These actions and interac-
tions, pertinentto the elements of tonality, lharmony,melody, texture,and rhythm, asto furtherparameters within each ofthese, area principalsubstance offunction and expression inmusic. Levels ofpitch structure, as of other erlement-structures,can perhaps usefully be conceived asanalogically of relative distance: the most fundamental as background in an imagined three-dimensional field, the most immediate as foreground, in focus as one regards the structure at close range. The metaphoric conceit of relative focus is helpful in engendering the image of structural depth in this sense, and in conceiving of various middlegrounds coming into increasingly sharp exposureas detailsblur, and as increasingly comprehensiveevents are the objects of attention. The idea of referring to identifying! such levelsas to spans fy'context is at times also useful:half of a bipartite form thus can represent that level at which its own inherent, delimiting funda.mental elementsare in focused exposure-that broadly middle level which is nearer than superior elementfunctions extending into contexts beyond that of rwrence, in this case one of two major divisions. More foreground levels are thus identifiable as to more limited temporal units, or spans. Inthis sense,the level of the phrase would mean that level at which those events of primary structural basis in the phrase arein exposure-that level whoseprimary events go out of focus in favor of elements of broader significance as one regards broader contexts more basic levels! through events of the relative foreground. Or, the level of the phrase is the level of those eventswhich delimit the phrase as toits most basic content while more fundamentallevels-extending ultimately to that of the whole-are comprised of eventswhose implicationsreach beyondthat oi, in this case,the phrase,or other, limited referential context. In the succession oftwo phraseshaving harmonic content I, IV, V - I, IV, V, I, the phraselevel action structure at the level of the phrase! isI, V - I, while the action at the broader, two-phrase levelis I-I, the comprehensivetonic prolongation. It is my belief that levels ofdepth, providing the basisfor important analytical constructs, haveexperiential validity within all elements ofmusical structure. The idea of leve1 also arisesin other connections: for example, asto the abstracted, leveled hierarchy by which a tonal system, particular or
14 introduction
generalized, can be represented, or the multilateral leveling of pitch events asnoted in Chapter l. Moreover, a conceptual basisfor identifying structural levels in meter as to temporal spans one of which is taken as the referential metric unit! is discussed in Chapter 3; in that respect, metric structure at the level of the phrase is that whosemetric unit is the phrase. The concept of hierarchic levels ofstructure is of great importance. To put it in the light of the above formulations: every individual element-event has immediate local, foreground! implications; those that are more fundamental have broader implications as well. Hence, the concept of hierarchic levels in all element-structuresarises naturallyand inevitably, and it must be seen asapplicable to the functioning of eventswithin any given confluence of action.
The theory of hierarchic levels is not only of very critical importance in the study and analysis offunctional-expressive effects of element-formulations, but it also has important implications for theoretical treatment of individual structural elements, especiallyin matters of classification and terminology." Thus, for example, the difficulty of modulation as it concerns theextent and perceptual significanceof tonal reference tonicization! is greatly attenuated when it becomespossible tospeak oftonal shift having referential significanceat agiven level and not at others. The problem of tonal shift remainsone ofcomplexity and, at times,ambiguity; but in at least many instances it proves to be of significant clarification to seethe tonicization of secondary pitch factors asrelevant to particular levelse.g., thoseof the phrase and smaller units! but not to higher levels. A like approach to the discussionof fluctuation as manifestat given, identifiable levels
of structure can be found useful
in the
theoretical treat-
ment of other structural parameters too-for example, meter, coloration, and texture. Consider, for example, broad potential implications within the symmetry-asymmetry polarityin metric structure, asopposed topurely local effects of subordinate fluctuation within this particular parameter; and a comparable polarity within the realm of texture is that of the homorhythm-
1°Thc conceptof 1eveled structure derivespreeminently fromthe analytical approaches ofSchenker topitch structuresin tonal music, butfrom thework ofother theorists too-for example, that of Hindemith in his conceptof step progression or Schoenberg in his importantconcept ofmonotonality. Schenkerian approacheshave nostrict or direct representation in this book, butsee Felix Salzer,Structural Hearing: Tonal Coherence in Music New York: C. Boni, 1952! orSalzer and Carl Schachter, Counterpoint in Composition New York: McGraw-Hill, 1969!,in addition to many other sourcesin an expanding literature,some ofthem cited at appropriatelater stages in this booksee page 113!. Hindemiths concept of step progression is setforth inThe Crajt J Musical Composition New York:Associated Music Publishers, Inc., 1937!, Book I; Schoenbergs theory ofmonotonality ispresented inStructural Functions ij HarmonyLondon: Williamsand Norgate, Ltd., 1954!. Other sources of comparable interest are noted in appropriate, later contexts.
introduction 15
polyrhythm dialectic, with its varying levels of significance.! In analyses which make up much of the substance ofthis book, the issueof hierarchic structure is recurrent and of fundamental importance. We have noted the importance of hierarchy in such qualitative distinctions asthat which observes stasis at one level e.g., the level delineated by a pedal point! and activity at a lower level e.g., that of fluctuation over a fixed or elaborated, prolonged pitch event!. Similarly, application of the concept of hierarchic level can bring about decisive and critical refinement of a theoretical idea like dissonance: in comparison, for example, of dissonance function of purely local importance e.g., the foreground melodic event in
conflict with surrounding pitchfactors! andthat of broader importance e.g., the expansive dissonance prolongation that often preparesa stablearea of thematic statement!. And the underlying concept of hierarchic levels serves to inform and establish certain functional distinctions
between elements
themselves: note,for example, the clear significance ofthe conceptof hierarchic level in the frequently differing implications of harmonic as opposed to tonal events,the latter a hierarchically superior extension or amplification of the former.
It thus follows that recessive andprogressive linesof element-change must finally be evaluatedas to level offunctional signyicance ; within this necessary approach,broadly viewed actions ofone classificationwill be seen commonly to contain lower-level actions ofan opposing or corresponding,but subordinate! classification. Let the reader contemplate briefly some of the element-structures in which the extension ofimplications to the broadeststructural level can most easily be grasped-conceptually and often perceptually. Thus, in any multimovement work the succession of primary tempi constitutesa broad line of changewhich is the highest manifestation ofthe tempo-structure.Or, the broadest levelof metric structure in any work of suchmajor formal divisions i.e., movementsor comparable units! is evident in the proportional differences andinterrelations manifestamong suchdivisions. And the coloration of a multimovement work constitutes,at broadestlevel, aline of color changein differences involving areas morethan detailsof spectrum! of critical functional-expressive effect.More diflicult is the fact that the broadest lines of tonal successioncan be seen in many instancesto correspond to to be an augmentation of! those ofrelatively local tonal or harmonic and melodic! successions.
It seemsto me that there is no reason todoubt the perceptual, experiential as well as conceptual, significance of these broadest configurations in
element-structures, especially those functioning within element-structures of most immediate and uncomplicated apprehension-within the soft-loud, dark-brilliant, slow-fast, long-short, and comparably apprehensiblelines of change. In carrying out expositions and analytical illustrations of major
16 introduction
categories of element-structure and element-content I have at times been compelled to stop short of discussionand application at broader in a sense simpler! levels, those for example of intermovement relations. But it is my assumption thatthese conceptsare applicable and apprehensiblethroughout the structuralhierarchy, especiallywhere theyconcern musicsmost directand palpable areasof action and interaction. In any concern with the functional consequences of contextual associations and interrelations, there
arises the issue of randomness, whether overt or
the result of the blind predetermination of musical events. Kfenek embraces unpredictability as a premised value underlying total serialism; in a somewhat ambivalentstatement aboutinspiration Einfall! as spontaneous and uncontrolled therefore of random consequences!, Kfenek correctly notes that it is in fact conditioned by prior understanding and experience.In that light, he saysof the composer of totally serialized music: In order to avoid the dictations of such ghosts i. e., recollection, tradition, training, and experience!, he prefers to set up an impersonal mechanism which will furnish, according to premeditated patterns, unpredictable situations.
But the surprise which is thus assured isone in which no contextual norm of probability is demonstrable or relevant; hence, an environment is established inwhich all events, and no events, are surprising.The affective value of surprise,important to any composer,must derive from a context in which a particular event is not what the percipient is conditioned to expect.! Roger Sessionsasks oftotal serialism: . . . what is being organized, and according to what criterion? Is it not rather a matter of organizing, not music itself, but various facets of music, each independentbr and on its own terms or at bestaccording to a set of arbitrarily conceived and ultimately quiteirrelevant rulesof associationIP
The affective value which inheres inisolated from contextual relations! sonorous events should notbe overlookedor discountedin any comprehensive view of musical expressionand experience.Whether isolated-sonorousqualities canproperly be said to convey meaning," there seemsno doubt that "Extents and Limits ofSerial Techniques,in TheMusical Quarterly, XLVI, 2 960!, p. 228. 12Problems and Issues Facing the Composer Today, in The Musical Quarterly, XLVI, 2 960!, p. 169.Italics added. "This dependson whethermeaning is considered torequire syntacticrelations, or whether a kind of meaning inheres in any singleevent or element-complex eliciting affective orother! response. Meaning in the experienceof art is commonlyconsidered to depend oncontrolled contextual interactions ofelements: once two eventsoccur intemporal
introduction 17
phenomenal-perceptual attestation and experience speak persuasively of the affective force implicit in such eventsquite apart from contextual interactions of the kind which are the prevalent concern ofthis book. The primitive impact of the naked musical event-its intensity, its timbral quality, its general pitch locus, itsdensity if it is a tone complex!, and the like, divorced from syntax, must be regarded as an aspect of musical effect which is far from adequately understood, yet of a level and substance surelyinadequate to full experience ofa temporal art form in cognitive, intellectual dimensions. Implicit throughout these studies,and from time to time made explicit in them, is the belief that controlled contextual function of element-events isof
fundamen/tal value and, even,of necessity in musicalcommunication where the respondent is engaged in apprehensions of functionalordered relations as aspects of experience. The view that purely superficial sound qualities in individual isolation-or in adventitious association-have aprimitive communicative potentialsuggests some kind of noncontextual significance,even though an important aspect ofany meaning in the isolated event say, a big tutti attack in the orchestra! is normally the expectation it arouseswith respect to likely subsequent events. At the same time, however, the most important aspect of musical experiencederives from the interaction.: and interrelation; of contiguous and concurrent events of diifering qualities within contextual procedures determined by controlled lines ofprogressive andrecessive successions. Such a concept of meaning is not applicable where event-confluencesare random and arbitrary. A subtle factor in this area of critical judgment is the problem of element-change ofa rateand degree so extremeas to, in effect, nullify the effect and perceptionof controlled change, evenof changeitself A useful analogy here isin the rapid spinning of a color wheel,which becomeswhite beyond a certain speedof rotation.! Where change e.g., of register! is fast, constant, and extreme, many percipients testify to the dulling ¢ct of uneventfulness which is the ironic consequence ofsuch action. This point is quite independent of whether element-successions in a situation of
this kind
are, in
com-
positional intent, random or arbitrary, but it is a relatedquestion becausethe phenomenon ofuneventfu1 change is in fact characteristicof many products of total serialism.
This book includes within its range of musical illustrations several instances ofrandom and highly serialized i.e., serialized beyond the single contiguity, thepercipient assumes a contextualsyntax to be thebasis fortheir association, and fortheir further role in a largercontext. Toldthat suchfunctional contextualinterrelations arebeside thepoint, heis left to the limited experienceof individual events whose association hasno appreciablerationale-events cooccurringin an apparently contradictory situation in which they have noappreciable functionalinteraction or interrelation but are nevertheless run together.
18 introduction
element ofpitch! operations in order to provide some expositionof specimen techniques andin order to provide the basisfor. analyticalcomment. But it is important to concede at the outset 'the bias by which successions ofevents having no controlled or even foreseencontextual associations,relations, and interactions are regarded as antithetic to systems ofmeaning upon which the musical experience, in virtually the entire history of the art, critically depends. Theissue mightbe characterizedas oneof starting-going-stopping in a given musical structure as opposed to beginning-prog'ressing-recedingending.! When a soundevent succeeds another purelyfortuitously, contextual interrelation is precluded unless it is accidental, or unless preselectionis within a narrow range of planned consequences!; andchange which leaps far acrossa spectrumof possibilitiesin rapid succession oftentends to nullify the effect and the effectiveness! ofchange. It is an interesting fact, too, that music in which important decisions are left to the performer is guided, and often contextually directed, by whatever creative gifts andintuitive experience the performer may bringto hisparticipation. But music in which major elements are prescribed in predetermined serial operations i.e., in which few if any significant elements are left for determination on the basisof the needs ofcontext! is the most random of all, in that contextual relations arelittle if at all foreseen, muchless abasis by which events are determined. The highly serialized pieceis thus in a critical sense morerandom than the overtly random piece in which fundamental decisions areleft to the performer. Of courseit is recognized thatthe intentionsand creativepurposes ofthe total serialist may havenothing to do with determined-in-context associations of events,nor can he be censured forfailure in an aestheticdesideratum and objective hedoes notembrace. On the other hand, it must bestated that two common protestations-that performer-directed chance music issimply an extension andfurther manifestation of historical improvisatory procedures, and that extreme serialismis merely another systemicbasis forcontrols analogous tothose ofmodality or tonality-are decisively rejected. In no traditional system are contextual associations adventitious or predetermined in any critical degree: in traditional improvisation, structure is rigidly planned and specified in crucial, prevalent parameters; and in historical systems of modality and tonality creative choices asto contextualaffiliations ofrhythmic, textural, and relatively foreground pitch events and event-complexesare of broad and decisive latitude,with functional consequences incontext, rather than predetermining systemic influences,the considerablebasis for creative choice and the determinants of content in the particular piece. Tonality as a systemthus predeterminesthe content of the particular musical instancein only the most general terms within which expectations are aroused and fulfilled!, and only with respect to certain structural elements; and contextual associations
in tonal music are determined as to the
introduction 1.9
functional-expressive needs of specificcontiguous andconcurrent conditions, not predetermined. Even while an underlying assumption ofthe value of contextual determination is conceded, thepresentation ofspecific worksand extractsin which this value is called into question will inform the reader of disparate compositional practices,while calling attention to works and ideas that may by their own persuasion leadhim to conclusions verydifferent from those stated in the foregoing commentary,with respect to an area ofconcern fraughtwith heavily contentious
and controversial
issues.
It is/irnportant to emphasize that theserialization ofsome elements in no way precludes the contextual determination of others. Moreover, any predetermination in classical twelve-tone procedures is commonly of PC pitch-class! rather than of specific pitch, so that, for example, melody is preconditioned in only one of its aspects. The serialization of PC content in classical procedures in which rhythms and other element-actions areleft for contextual determination has produced a vital literature, even at times with tonal bases, as examples inthis bookdemonstrate. Importanttwelve-tone works are cited and discussed in illustration of this books fundamental premises andin documentation of the values it espouses.' To the extent that the composer does not determine or significantly condition or even foresee! the concurrencesand contiguities of events in planned contextualrelations, analysiscan only describe proceduresand show their applications. It can, in theory, attempt to describe expressive-functional consequences of random procedures,but any effort to do so, in critical comment basedupon premisesof value likely to be contradictory to thoseunderlying suchworks, isirrelevant to the extent that the contextual confluencesof events are arbitrary and
uncontrolled.
The problem of the apprehension ofsignificances ofevents whoseassociation in particular contextsis purely or essentiallyadventitious issometimes discussed as aproblem of overload in the channel of communication ofinformation, with reference tothe concepts of information theory:. . . the absence of a stable stylistic syntax, archetypal schema, audible compositional order, andpatent natural patterning resultsin a level ofredundancy so low that communicationis virtually precluded. Leonard Meyer, Music,The Arts,and Ideas; © 1967 byThe University of ChicagoPress; p.290; all rights reserved.Emphases added.! See tooEdward Conesdiscussion ofthe problemof what he refersto as synoptic comprehension inthe musicalexperience Musical Form and Musical Peyizrmance; New York: W. W. Norton andCompany, Inc.,1968; pp.88-98!. Atone pointin thisexcellent essay pp. 95-96!, Conemakes thestatement that. _ _ non-determinedmusic, whetherthe sequence of eventsis left up to the performeror to pure chance,may imply a continuumthat often seems tocombine thepurely musicalwith the quasi-dramatic. Thetwo determinismsof formula andfortune leadto the same result:the arbitrary way in which suchproducts begin and endand thefortuitous nature of theirinner connections ensure thatthey canat bestbe experienced onlyas surfaces. Emphasis added.!
20 introduction
This books discussions ofstructure and analysis in harmonic and melodic successionsare less extensive than one might wish, but there are many sources,reflecting a variety of approaches andpoints of view, to which the reader can refer in these connections, and it has seemed important to concentrate heavilyon problemsof structurenot widely treated in the existing theoretical literature, especially thoseof texture and rhythm. Moreover, the treatment of quasi-serial cellular associations ofpitch and PC complexes islimited, and the treatment of this aspect of structure centers primarily in complexes having tonal or quasi-tonal significance; again, the reader is referred to supplementary resources. And it has provedimpossible toextend this book to the point of including a discrete study of structural functions of coloration of timbral differences, of orchestration, etc.!, although there are many parenthetical references to these factors in connection
with central
issues of concern and
treatment, especiallythat of texture. In view of this, it seems wellto make a point of the fact that one vital aspectof coloration, that expressedin dynamic levels, often goes very far indeed in accounting for the nature of musical experience andmeaning. If its attention in these chapters is not equal to its importance, it is largely because itsfunction is direct and obvious, and because ofits comparative accessibility offunction and significance. In the organization of this book, it is true that the treatment of discrete elements in individual chapters imposessomewhat arbitrary lines of distinction. But overlapping concernsare explicit, for example, in the books concept of rhythm as including the rhythms of all element-changes e.g., tonal rhythm!, or in the obvious relation of texture and harmony, and in many other instances and connections. With the understanding that this book is about music, none of whose structural
elements often assumes inde-
pendent manifestation,it will be acknowledgedthat there is a methodological advantage and common didactic purpose in individuating at provisional stages identifiableand classifiableelement-actions forsingular emphasisand for explication of conceptual and practical ideas relating to each such element-structure in
turn.
On the other hand, a very important manifestation of the conceptual unity and interdependence ofthis entire range of studies canbe noted in the conscious effortto treatin anabfsis certain recurrent examples from chapter to chapter, and to refer asoften aspossible toassociated events involving cofunctioning elements otherthan thoseof immediate concern. To some examplesan effort is made to apply comprehensive, althoughsummary, treatment at various stages; each chapter cites examples of substantial scope treated in full at least asto operationsof the particular element under primary consideration. The most important ofthese havingbroad applicabilityis GeorgePerle, Serial Composition and Atonality: AnIntroduction to the Musicof Schoenberg, Berg, andWebern, 3rded. Berkeley andLos Angeles:University of California Press,1972!.
introduction 21
The procedure by which a basic, highly diverse, set of musical references wasestablished foranalytical at times recurrent! treatment had been an essentialpremise fromthe earlieststages ofthis study. In fact, the selection of works to which to devote attention was the first problem undertaken. Implicit in that initial undertaking was the challenging assumption that a thesis ofbroad importance in the structure of music had to be, and must be shown to be, applicable to an ample samplingof works representing a great breadth of chronology, mediumlsystem, and stylistic disparity. Only occasionally has it been necessary to omit the quotation of music to which significant referenceis made; usually this is becauseof necessary reference to lengthy excerpts whose quotation has proved impractical, or because ofrestrictions of copyright. On the other hand, referenceis often to music readily available in widely circulated, published anthologies. While there are within the range of twentieth-century practice some illustrations of new techniques, this book would be highly untrue to itself if it conveyed animpression of special advocacyof contemporary or recent music in which ncvel means areevident at the expenseof twentieth-century music oi, in the particular element of concern, more traditional persuasion. It is somehow aparticular mark of the present time that musicians tend to devote disproportionateattention to works that constitute manifestoes,or are in some sense apparentlytrailblazing, when in fact there is much of great interest, force, and resourcefulness in music of traditional confidence within conventional means-music that is
bents and assured unfortunately less
studied in view of its relative accessibility. Again, one can only plead the limitations of space. Whatis ofinterest isthe discoveryof relations pertaining between structureand expressiveeffect in all music that is strong andinteresting; indeed, the restlessassertion of novelty often finds its outlet in mere notational appearances.
Notational problems are a primary reason for the lack of attention to electronic music-not only the problem of the accessibilityof scores,but the absence ofany generally intelligible mode of graphic representation of specific elements of structure. But I wish to state at the same time my conviction
that the principles of contextual shaping to which this book is devoted are fully applicable, within a context of vastly expanded resources notyet well understood in perceptual and cognitive implications, to electronic music. Analyses ofmusical extractsand piecestake manyforms in these studies, and that breadth of technique of application is altogether intentional. I "These includeBurkhart, Anthology _hr MusicalAnabsis, 2nded. New York: Holt, Rinehart andWinston, Inc., 1972!; Davisonand Apel,Historical Anthology of Music,2 Vols. Cambridge: Harvard University Press,1949!; Starr and Devine, Music Scores Omnibus Englewood CliH`s, N._].: Prentice-Hall,Inc., 1964!;Parrish, ATreasug ofEarbv MusicNew York: W. W. Norton and Co., 1958!; andBerry and Chudacolf, Eighteenth-Centugw Imitative Counterpoint Music : farAnabrsis Englewood Cliffs, N._].: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1969!.
22 introduction
strongly rejectthe conceptof any fixed orthodoxy of analytical procedure and representation, believingthat the approach dependson the nature and basis of inquiry, and may in specific areas take,equally, any of a number of forms. Thus, someof the analyses inthis book take form in descriptive statements of an expositorynature; others are graphic or symbologicalwithin a substantial range of possibilities-some innovative, arising necessarily out of newly formulated concepts.Throughout this work I have soughtto fulfill the analysts obligation to make clear all lexical and symbological basesfor graphic, expository, and other methods. Analytical sketches, of which there are many and many kinds, go beyond the text in analytical comment and statementand must be reviewed in necessarysupplement tothe text. The question of how to prepare synoptic sketches ofpitch structures is oftentreated informally, again within the intent
of latitudeand fiexibilityof specificinode ofexecution; there is, however, an abundance of illustrations, and suggestions ofsymbols and procedures are given. In general, it should be kept in mind that sketches ofpitch or PC! content representfor eachexample, sometimes within a limited, specified context and restricted realm of inquiry, an interpretation of structure and functional processand affiliation. This applies not just to pitch structures but to all the elements examined;in Chapter 3, too, the hierarchy of metric functions is represented with careful explication of devicesemployed. In any study of this kind, the problem of terminology is a difiicult and considerable one.It has been my hope to counter this problem at least in significant degreeby the best possibleconsistency andclarity of terminological selection and definition.
The index lists all crucial and problematic termstogether with numbers of pageson which their definitions can befound. In many instancessignification is probably self-evident, given a few stated, underlying terminological assumptions, butI have tried to give the reader thebenefit ofany conceivable doubt
Brief expositionof certain most essentialand recurrent terms andusages might be given herein passing,but it should be read astentative and preliminary. I use the term elementwith reference to any of that set of primary structural parameters within which events oflike significanceand character take place in music; thus, the element of tonaligy isprojected in tonicizations; that of harmony in chords orvertical simultaneities! ; that of meloafy in contiguous pitches orattacks occurring within an identifiably continuous linear stream, voice, or stratum; that of colorin timbresor sonorous qualities" within a given "These are determined by physical characteristicsof the sometimes alteredor manipulated! soundingmedium, by registral locus,by articulative mode ofproduction of sound, andby degreeof loudness.
introduction 23
vocal or instrumental spectrum; these arefor the greater part conventional usages.
The element of texturemight be said to consist in events bywhich the interrelations of lines or other cqfunctioning components areconditioned, but the
textural element is alsoregarded as inclfuding such factors as density and space. The most complex elementof all, that of rhythm, isin a sense theproduct of actions of all other elements; constituent parameters of tempo andmeter are seen asof great importance. Naturally, all of these termsand conceptshave full presentation in the chapters which follow. The terminological premises notedbriefly above,and givenfull explication at later stagesin this book, yield certain derivative terms which I have adopted ordevised inaccord with underlying theoreticalconcepts. Ahyphenated term like element-structure refers to the shape delineated by changing qualities and intensity levels assuming thereis change! within a given element, or -to theflat shape of unchangingevents within that element.Terms like tonal structure aresimply specihcations of the generic form, elementstructure; but comparable specificationsare also used to denote structures arising within constituent parameters. Thus, tempo structure, and related terms, are employed from time to time. Similarly, terms like element-action, element-event, element-change yield the more specificforms of which metricevent and harmonic action mightbe cited as instances.[The hyphen occurs in generic forms, and in terms, especially noun-noun forms, which I have devised e.g., pulse-tempo!, and where the associationof component terms might not be self-evident.] Equally fundamental are the terms progression, recession, succession, and to a lesser extentstasis; in fact, these terms representin the simplest consolidation a primary conceptual basis for this book, as we have seen.Forms like element-succession, harmonic progression, tonal recession, and the like, are in constant use.
I use the term function to refer to the processive,structural role of an event or succession succession denoting a generic conceptsubsuming progression and recession!. Ifthe concept of structural function is in terminological reference commonlyallied to that of expression, is it becauseI cannot regard them as distinguishable in the object-subject relation of actual musical experience: functionis the role, or nature of participation, of an event in the import of expressive contentand significance; element-function is the basis for source of! expression; the functional-expressivemeaning and substance of the event are its perceived, cognized characteristics or those presumably intended! and relations to affiliated eventsin a given work at a given level. It is my view that every musical eventin contextsto which these theories are applicable i.e., all except those in which contextual contiguities and simultaneities are fortuitous and arbitrary! has discernible, rationally interpretable, functional and expressivesignificance. The expressive content
24 introduction
or potential of any musical event rests very decisively on its functional role within a contextual process;an events functional significance in the structure! is indissolubly allied to, and the primary basis for,its expressiveimpact in the experience!. It seems to me that the only sensein which these are experientially separable concepts assuming consideration not merely of objects, but of experience! isthat in which the isolated musicalevent whether it is in isolation, or is construed in isolation in the absence ofapprehension ofcontextual relations! might be said to have purely expressiveas opposedto _functionalexpressive import." Since this book is concerned with the analysis of contextual relations in shapedcombinations of events, it employs commonlythe hyphenated formfunctional-expressiveaddition in to didactic useof the individual terminological components. When I wish to refer to the eventwithout necessaryregard to its expressive contentor functional role I often usethe term prejection, denotingsimply that which issues fromsource to percipient in the musical experience-the stimulus itself, whatever its functional-expressive contentand significance, and apart from such significance. Finally, with respect tothe confluent successions of concurrent elementactions, I employ fundamentally the terms complementary and compensatoy sometimes such associated terms as parallel and counteractive!to denote relations betweenelement-actions whichhave, respectively,allied or resistant functional tendency at somelevel. Within this concept, the term neutral isat times necessary,having to do with stasis or nonparticipation-nonfunction within agiven element-structure, with respect to agiven directional tendency, a atgiven level. In description of situations characterizedby the confluence ofseveral element-successions, multiple terms suchas tonal-harmonic succession texturalor metric intensificationare useful, even unavoidable.
The above summary of some basicterminological propositionsis, to be sure, not intended as a glossary; for explicit discussions ofthese andmany other terms the reader is referred to the text of the book itself and to the index.
The foregoing is intended simply to call attention to lexical items of most general importance. While discussingparticular structural elements andconsequences ina very broad selection ofworks, this book points out and illustrates many approaches to analysis. It should pose no unusual difficulty for the reader equipped with understanding of fundamental conceptsof music theory, and it is my hope that thesestudies succeed in laying out in theory and in application somevital ideas and methodsin supplement to the work of many others Compare Cones immediate apprehension as opposedto synoptic comprehension in the sourcecited earlier footnote 14!.
introduction 25
along distinct but often complementary linesof inquiry. Important, relevant theoretical sources are cited from time to time.
fl*
By no means do I regard extramusical systemsand communicative structures, applied analogously or adopted assources oftheoretical models, as lacking important usefulness in the study of music." But the present studies byand large follow from direct encounterswith musical works themselves, fromwhich observationsare drawn with respectto functional elements and expressiveeffect, without recourse to parallels in extramusical systems and without employment of systemic analogsand models of other kinds. Understanding is sought in direct discoursewith the object itself-the score page andthe sound images ofwhich it is the symbol, and in exploration of the experienceof what psychologists callthe stimulus object, the musical work transpiring in time. Of course much work remains to be carried further; the problem of logical considerationof pathsfrom analytical insight to decisions ofperformance isonly one area of need that comes quickly to mind. I have also implied that, in -my view,thorough studyof subjectresponse to the single musicalevent in isolation in the widest possible rangeof combinations of parameters! must ultimately play its part in the understanding of the musical experience; for, if the syntactic operations and relations of confluent eventsare expressiveof meaning at the most sophisticatedlevels of apprehension and comprehension, the naked qualities which inhere in the single event unconfused bycontextual disorder! presumably haveevocative powers in themselves insome pure and primitive sense. Adancer will respond with related movement triggered by the affective content of a single sound.! The qualitative impact of the individual event is intrinsic rather than syntactic-with this I mean notjust the event out of context, but the event forwhich no plausible functional relation to surrounding events can befelt or deduced, orthose aspects of the event, contributing to its import and impact, heard and felt apart from contextual associations. Thus, the qualities of single events e.g., articulation, density, loudness!often have in themselves unusualaffective importanceat low levels ofstructure. I have the feeling that purely surface qualities-those divorced from or regarded and felt apart from! contextual implications-are underestimated in the study of musical experience,probably becauseof the apparently unintellectual nature absence of the intuitive or deliberate, cognitive act of relating! of the appreciation of affective valuesin isolated events ofpure, local surface. Of further areas of potential exploration, one feelsthat, for example, subjective association of musicalevents withextramusical experience-association triggered by analogical parallels of various kinds-may have greater "Perhaps themost importantcurrent sources of suchanalogs areset theory,probability and information theory,Gestalt psychology, and linguistics.
cH/wen onus
ton olity /n troductory notes
The concept of the hierarchic ordering of pitch content has in one mani-
festation oranother served as abasis formusical structure since theearliest
stages the in Western traditiorg is It theintent of this chapter to introduce some ideas about tonal order in music and to illustrate
and discuss certain
approaches inthe analysisof tonality and tonal successions according to the concepts presented. The practice of hierarchic systems oftonal order, by which the concept of tonality is engendered,is of truly monumental significance. Mans cultural history is marked by few achievementsof comparable magnitude; indeed, it can be argued persuasivelythat nearly all music of the Western tradition is structurally conditioned by some kind of expression oftonality. The discussion that follows restsupon the broad definition of tonality necessary tosuch an argument.
Tonality may be thus broadly conceivedas aformal system in whichpitch content is perceived asfunctionalty related toa specijic pitch-class or pitch-class-complex ey resolution] often preestablishedand preconditioned, as abasis for structure at some understood level of perception. The foregoing definition of tonality is applicable not just to the tonal period in which the most familiar conventions of tonal function are practiced roughly the eighteenth and the nineteenth centuries!, but through earlier modality and more recent freer tonal applications as well. The tonalsystem consists of a hierarchic ordering of PC factors, with the tonic final, axis, center, etc.! the ultimate point of relationship which tonal successions arecontrived to expcct. In the tonal period of conventional common practice the primary system consistsof hierarchically oriented degrees of the diatonic scale and the tertian harmonies erected on these The terms pitch-class and pitch-class-complex symbolized PC and PCC! are used to denote pitchindependent ofspecific registraloccurrence, ora complexof suchpitches generically understood. 27
28 tona/ity
degrees. Inmodal styles,corresponding systems are the modes, with normative points of tentative and fi-nal cadential repose. In more recent stylesin which tonality is relevant a system may but need not! consist of specific scalar formulations PC collections! of these or other kinds, with derivative
melodic andharmonic configurations disposed insuch away asto express and give primacy to a particular tonic or, tonics. Often
in fluctuant contexts, particular
such tonal content is reminiscent of conventions of the tonal
period, and its validity in experience may in significant part be based on powerful conditioning in the all but universal practice of systemsof tonal order in Western music.
There are of coursemusical idioms, especially ofthe current period, in which tonalityl as aformalized systemis lessrelevant-or irrelevant, and the less relevanttonality in- a particular style the more musical structure must depend on other factors, even extramusicalfactors, exceptwhere considerations of structure are put aside altogether, asin somealeatoric music. More-
oviiftyles in which tonalfluctuation isconstant and extreme inlatitude may find the structural function of tonality impaired, resulting in a changed experiential significancefor the sense oftonic or vacillating tonic. Although no amount of argument cansettle thequestion ofrelevance of tonality in music describedas atonal, it is important that the question be raised in the light of specific musical instances. The many examples of
twentieth-century sources in the following series of analyticalpresentations, and the discussion oftonal referencesor allusions affirmed to be implicit in them, will amplify the question and suggest possibleinterpretations. These analyses thusinclude referenceto music later than as well as earlier than! that in which conventions of tonal-harmonic order prevail. Questions of the establishment of tonal reference by harmonic-melodic
succession, andof the nature of tonal fluctuation, like that of conjectured tonal allusion in antitonal styles, are necessarily somewhatsubjective, and the concernin the analyses whichfollow will be to cite methodsarising from theoretical propositions stated, and to present plausible conclusionsarising from such methods and hypotheses, rather than to achieve any kind of inflexible truth about the examples andideas explored.Effort is directed here to an understandingof the techniques bywhich tonality is expressedand to formulation of interpretations of tonal implications which are demonstra2The termsconvention and conventional will refer specificallyto triadic and othertertian harmonies, and melodic successions having these as theirframing bases, as wellas predictable successions of harmonies bywhich othersare seenas exceptional or deceptive in the period of major-minor tonality.Conventions of tonal expression are thusparticular means of aflirming the tonal hierarchy in practice asopposed tothe broad principle of/zierarchic order in thesense inwhich it can beseen'to begenerally applicable.
tona/ity y
29
ble in the circumstances discoverable in the analysis of a given work. In discussion of tonicizing factors those which support the emergence or existence of a particular tonal center, or central PCC! primary interest resides in techniques by which the basic tonalorder iscfirmed andestablished.
/ntroductory commen ts concern/'ng Iona/ and //'near functions Melodic and
harmonic functions
are of two kinds: the first of these has to do
with position, identity, and hierarchic statusin eachof the system components of the particular tonality in question tonal function! ; the second isthe role of the event in the melodic-harmonic linear stream linearfunction!, also hierarchically defined with respect to a given level of reference.Linear function is the relation of an event to the structural relatively essential! linear frame or basis, orits auxiliary subsidiary, elaborative,embellishing, prolonging! relation to an event ofhigher order. We shall suggest thatthese functions are multidimensional-tonal function as to relatively deep or superficial levels of tonality, and linear function in a comparable sensebut also in the fact that a given event is associated oftenwith more than one other event in different ways, even at the samestructural level of reference. Of linear functions, the neighbor auxiliary embellishes astructural point in harmonic or melodic succession and is oftenderived in voice-leading ofone or more stepwise adjacencies, common-tone associations, or other proximate relations". Often a neighbor separates two appearances of the structural harmony it elaborates. A second typeis the passing auxiliaiyoften a succession of passing events!in which movement, up or down, links two harmonic or melodic points in an action filling space beyond that which is normative in the neighbor association. In a passing configuration, implicit in one or more voiceswill be relatively conj unct descentor ascentin a passing stream normally filling intervals of a 3rd or greater. Distinction between neighbor
e.g., /92 or 92/ !and passing e.g., ,___---"' ! configurations involving linear auxiliary functions is, then, a matter of voice-leading; where harmonic"as opposedto monophonic textures are concerned, theconsideration of voice-leading gives primary attention to outervoices, and especialbf to the bass. An event may be auxiliary at a broad level, essentialat a more immediate level; or a neighbor auxiliary at a very local level may be seenas part of a broadly passingstream at a higher level. For example, aV cadencewith
Q second scale degree! in theupper voice is theessential structural aim of succession atthe level of the phrase it concludes; at a broader level the ca-
30 tona/ity
dence isperceived as auxiliary toits I and l! consequent. Thus emerges the concept of multileveled linear function paralleling that of multileveled tonal function.
Example l-l points up the issueof linearfunction asrelated andcompared to tonalfunction, andit argues that the two kinds of function do not reflect, as is sometimessupposed, mutually contradictory or exclusive concepts.The analysis isbased on the premise that except in relatively rare tonally non-
functional elaborations, tonal function is of significance and in evidence and an experiential reality! in foreground, immediate contextsat the same time that the seriesof harmoniesmay beof significanceas aspace-filling linear stream of passing orneighbor auxiliaries. The exampleis a severe reduction and abstraction of harmonic content in mm. 3|-12
of the fourth movement
of Beethovenssecond symphony.The essentialsuccession is iv-V, the former appearing in two positions linked by a complex succession ofpassing auxiliaries related by tonal function aswell asby chromatic successionthe in lower voice and other, comparable linear successions!.Following the process ofsubdominant elaboration, V undergoes prolongationby neighbor auxiliaries, chromatically related, above and below. This sketch, vividly illustrating the two aspects of harmonic and melodic! function which are a principal premise for this study, is indicative of the fact that linear andtonal functionsnearly alwaysappear incomplementary conjunction in tonal music. In the Beethoven thelinear force of chromatic ascent is of course inescapable,but at the same time the tonal functions indicated are plausible, perceptible, and never far removed from the primary system; The concept of multileveled linear function is unavoidable,and itscomparability tothat of multileveledtonal function will be readily apparent.The multileveled aspect oflinear function hasto do, as wouldbe expected, with the diferingfunctions pitch of eventsat various levels ofstructure-a multiplicity of functioncrossing structural levels. Forexample, apitch event whichis of central essential!function within a phraseis seento be auxiliary to more fundamental factors at broaderlevels, asin the instance described above; oran eventwhich is a neighbor auxiliaryat a very immediatelevel isseen tobe a factor in a larger, passing complex ofevents. Theconcept ofmultileveled linearfunction thusconcerns the nature of the function itself as viewed or heard! within differing levels ofperspective. Linear functionhas afurther aspectof multiplicity, or ambivalence:that in which a particular eventis afiiliated with more than one related event.For example,a particular pitch might function asupper neighborauxiliary to another andat the same timeanticipate be a factor in the prolongationof! an essential pointwhich follows.Or a particular pitch event mightfunction asauxiliary to two relatedpitches inthe succession g-a-f-g-a, thef is part of the neighborencirclement ofg andat thesame timea lowerauxiliary ofa!. Thiskind of dual,or multiple,functional relationmay beoperative ata singlelevel ofstructure; it has to do with the fact that an eventin a linear succession can bemany-sided inits roles.This might be termed multilateral linear function in keepingwith its character, asdescribed. Multilateral linearfunction thusconcerns the diferent events qfqjiliation ofa givenfactor ata given level. Themultiplicity of linear functionis, then,of two dimensions. See pp.37-40 forfurther referenceto theseconcepts; added treatment ofthese issues is alsoto be found in discussion of examples tofollow. -
tona/ity 31 Ex. 1-1. Beethoven, Symphony No. 2 in D, Op.36. Abstraction ofapassage in the fourthmovement. 111.303
;ZZI~¢ 92" 92'!
I `e' ' 92./V i
_; .,
I _ __ __p_.
e 92/
_
iv, IV *D,d2 iv
IV rv/Iv vii N2
V iv/ iv vii l
V/ iv iv VI
IV V A6!
g: i
V/iv IV
vii/V lg
iv vii/V
v? V
`i
V/ iv! C,c: V
I vi
Vi
vii/ii? ii?
Specimen identifications of multileveled tonalfunction seepage 52!. *Some romannumeral symbols are simplified. m.302 fa /"0 Jf Fl.
3
;I
/"0 f_`
_ »_ f
O.
=£C==§== Ob. :in-ll"l -H
3- _
lan, l
=a"I:Ii =
?_
I i==-_-_I §== i= Q!-_I
'° Bn.
_
»» - 1 1£1 1 »_ . __
zz '|.= °
.» _
- .7
- ff
._ '° 0' _ =Bi==i==i:11i, h1'li==
~
= i;hm~vI'nIi1;=i==r~ I U 1111! I Z
1o
Vln.
I
=:==§:===§===
.¢
-I7 °
if °
=Hiv.:==1'_1i,I=11'1s= I I 1 I Q.Ie I *o -0 lmhmwzgr-11==i==r-i1_1i:r'
' ff
~ ° *f ~'
:';:i|T1|'Q1H2I I ll !=1__» :1 Vla. ::t'x~'1l_;n:=l==l1»1 I
$111114 . nr 1111.1
il Ve., 3'-£;fI1U|l=1JI'Y1l=== _ ' Db. §-_|92'7 r
=-f7'_-.--!5r'2--22ll -
` if
'Q
:Q-I;
` sf
tona//ty 11
ntinued.
Boi /*___*
//_é /_X
¢ fm
1:1-` E'
uf
A2 _
il J
1? `
= ?_.E¢=-.,,.;_i '.;191 4' 4? °
ff
ff
Eg Q f ,E :r..E=:rE
f'- 3 =g;. u-,---rr-= r==:r * ff
ff ff
ff
tona/ity 33
Fl. Elf" T a #V ob.
f/F J
i"
5V
il T
Hn.! 2/ if
62
i; 9
;
u Q
Timp- 'G39¥l;,;3&192:JZ
V., jllfj Effie ji @ 5is at at 1 wa' @ if Vi V f f i VC-» f
f ll
ll fvci
Dbwgag é.; fqi Db-JT!
they include brief, but felt, local~tonicizations ofiv/iv c! and V A!, in addition to that of iv g! itself, the primary harmonic basis f`orthe entire ascending passage. Romannumeral analysesare given at severaltonal levels,including tonic and subdominant, the former of broad significance, the latter of signiiicance atthe immediatelevel of the passage itself, reflectingthe principle of multileveled tonal function. The example demonstratestwo fundamental classes of harmonic and melodic! activity: that of prolongationof iv,IV, then of V! The conjunctionof twolike symbols of upper-and lowercase, separated by the comma, denotes asituation inwhich majorand minorforms orsystems are conjoined, andin which both are relevant.
34 tona/ity
and that of movement in this case, recessive succession, overall, from iv to V!, as well as linear functions both passing andneighboring. The reader may wish to make comparable study of other passages fromthe same movement, harmonic fluctuations of comparable features-for example, mm. 220-21, 317-21, or 240-44.
A further example ofharmonic abstractionis givenin Ex. 1-2 in resumé unfortunately without quotation of the too extended extract on which it is based! of the reaffirmation of T; I5 and V following developmental fluctuaEx. 1-2. Beethoven, Symphony No. 3 in El., Op.55. Synoptic harmonic abstraction ofan extractfrom thefourth movement. m.277 m.292
m.303 m.3l5
.£%='?=¥¬1'I'|T¥1='»{§'T?'==3 lT?= .IL ..92|vf 1-1| _-_¢ 3,- _ ...ID T 1:-'Ti 2 ' "I -M 1 ,- _ _-l3__ | lil-', ,_ -m 1111 _=
`,v ~._.v 92..v 92.1 92.v ' le F
IILQ - L . 3-1--1:0-_ lf* -'VIYQ -Il _ C -ll!I BI C :ll-1.1
Q
§'5 92-V E 92.v
[sm SD
Wedge-like configuration toward V
st SD
D]
,l
note chromatic role ofmodal variant of tonic!: @§_'=-%2 ,.
Q *Undergoes 21measures of unbroken prolongation.
I
I
-IIEQH
tion in the final movement ofBeethovens Eroica Symphony. T: I is stated powerfully in a seriesof utterances,given in the one-staffsketch, elaborated by neighbor auxiliaries someof which tonicize thediatonic harmoniesii, IV, and vi, with ultimately a more extended referenceto Ab, the subdominantnormal medium for direct reintroduction of T: V-and with linking, passing auxiliary harmonies embellishing the recessive harmonicmovement from IV to V. All
of this is summarized in the sketch, to make theessential harmonic
directions and functions visible, and thereis addeda brief synoptic representation of the contrapuntally potent, largely chromatic, wedge-like configuration toward the V. Although it is not representedexcept by an inserted note, the V undergoes no fewer than 21 measures ofinsistent prolongation plus fermata! in a typical atmosphere ofgrowing intensity before the I is reaffirmed in thematic
statement.
The Beethovenextract showsa primary tonal systemon Eb expanded by referencesto the diatonic tonics f, Ab, and c. Again, purely linear factors are obviousand extremelypersuasive andthey are accounted for!; but tonal 51. e.,I of the tonic T! system,here Eb.
36 tona/ity Ex. 1-3 continued. 1,117 l Y.1:"l1:Y'i" ,_ lrnlllll I lil 11 lN 'lLlP'_ g,/ 92_,
7 lrtv' 1 v -f
"__, Li
-'_ i
'1 |
-HI: , I.gn-»';'_,*.,,, :1 4 im.: ini
|
'
gf: _
92_,_ ¥_/v v1 v/v t: R/t: V
III? I!
prolonged andembellished bya seriesof chromatic auxiliaries. They become, in mm. 29-32, virtually nonfunctional,°-i.e., their positions aspassing chromatic harmonies in embellishment of the structural harmony does, in this context, eclipse any potential inference of tonal meaning in the stream of diminished 7th-chords. This is because of the parallel succession inwhich any inherent potential for contrapuntal interaction and afliliation, or leading-tone action in support of potential tonics ofreference, isavoided. This is very different from the Beethoven examples,in which despite the force of consistent linear
succession in elaboration of
essential harmonic
content,
tonal tendenciesare fulfilledor suggested in foreground actions atimmediate levels of structure.!
If relative significances of linear as opposed to tonal functions are a matter of degree, it is nonethelessa valid analytical judgment that at some point tonal function is superseded inexperiential eH`ectby the force of relatively straight linear function in which potential leading-tone relations are unfulfilled at any structural level of perceptible connection, and not at other points, where suchrelations arecontextually demonstrable.The juxtaposition of the Beethovenand Chopin examples isuseful in illustrating this distinction.
The Chopin analysis mustalso giveattention to the root succession ata more essential level: BI;-Gb-Eb-a
triadic formulation
with its
own logic.
The sequentialrelation at the outsetof the succession F-Bb,Db-Gb! imparts °Again, with respect totonal significance; thus, in mm. 29-32,despite thepattern of tonal shifts D ?-G?-A ?-D?-E ?!inferable fromand inherentlypotential in the succession of diminished 7th-chords,and the .logic of 5th movement in that succession, thepassage involves direct, parallel chromatic succession from one diminished7th-chord to the next. The chords areof aspurely linearfunction asit is possible toimagine, lackingtonal significance of evenimmediate functionalvalue. Thus,the parallelismof voice-leadingpreempts and precludes expression or suggestion of tonal function inherently,if ambiguously,potential in the chords.Such pureparallelism ofvoice-leading isa primary technique oftonalb nonfunctional harmonic usage; itis to be foundin examples,especially offantasy or improvisational style, inthe eighteenthcentury, andof coursebecomes common as anelaborative device in late-nineteenth-century impressionism.
tona/ity 37
a further underlying, logical unity.' The chromaticism of the vocabulary of the example is enhanced by chromatic succession ata number of points. In Ex.l-3, to set the stage for the series of passing diminished 7thchords, the primary tonic is impaired by cancellation of its leading-tone in the intriguing, repeated allusionto the Gb passingtonic t: III ?!. Compensatory reaflirmation of the primary tonic at m. 33 isconsequently ofenhanced, strongly resolving effect.
The concept of mu/t/Yeve/ed function further
exp/ored
The conceptof tonal functions asambivalent, in accord with that of tonics of reference ofvarious levelsof structural significance havingrelatively local or broad implications, is of continuing necessity in the analysis of tonal-harmonic function.
Similarly, the linear interpretation of functions of pitch materials their dispositions in lines-their horizontalness and contiguity in time! concerns aleveled structure except in the most microcosmic contexts.Often many levels can be characterized, ranging from the extreme background the total form as an all-encompassingimpulse!-the broadest and highest hierarchic architectonic level, through numerous intermediate grounds, to the mostimmediate, local context ofreference treatedin traditional theory as foreground, the actual surfaceof music. It is vital in
the consideration
of leveled structure in
music that
the
surface, orforeground, be kept always in view, while regarded in its proper hierarchic status.
The reference to hierarchic
status as low in
level bears
no implication of relative importance. Indeed, one might well observe that in music of normative stylistic terms i.e., music in which norms can be identified-nearly all music!, background structure isof moregeneric content the more broadly it is viewed; theHavor, character, and unique expressive consequence of the particular work resides in very important respects in the particularities of elaboration in the foreground by which broader structural factors are realized and amplified. Analysis is far too often content with delineation of broad structure as THE structure, leaving untreated the vital, arresting patternsby which the great work of music projects and extends in time the prototypical basis. We have been concernedin discussionsof pitch structure to this point, and continue to be concerned, with functions-including tonal functions and identities_of pitch complexes within very local areas ofreference aswell as more essential functions, insistingthat immediate tonal implication is very critically a part of the experienceof music. 7Consider as well thehorizontalizations F-Bb-Db, Bb-Db-Gb, andGL-Bb impliedroot of the primary dominantat the end of the passingstream ofdiminished 7ths!-Eb.
38 tona/ity
The concept of multileveled, or multiple, tonal function is the basis for discussion ofthe concept of tonal fluctuation modulation!, in which it is asserted thatthe signyicance of fluctuation in any given instance corresponds to the breadth of structural level over which an emergent referential tonicprevails. Similarly, the concept of chromaticism, as it concerns alteration of the factors ofthe diatonic scalein melody and harmony, depends fordefinition on the concept of multiplicity of level and of tonal reference: thus, a particular PC may be chromatic in relation to the tonal system at one level e.g., the primary system C,where an F# appears in tonicization of the V! but not at another level in the same hypothetical situation, not at the secondary levelrepresented asthe immediate context of the tonicization of G, where the Fll is diatonic!. Important, then, is a procedure ofanalysis whichexamines aparticular harmonic or melodic line in stages ranging from the particulars of its appearance on the score page to the general structure described by its most essential points. For example, even a small unit of melody can be viewed varyingly so that at one level a particular factor appears relatively essential while at another level relatively auxiliary subsidiary, elaborative!. The differing conclusions derivedin this way are not of course contradictory; rather, they are complementary and interdependent views.! To put it another way, a melodic line of severalformal units reveals, inanalysis, anincreasingly complex hierarbhy of functional significances: anessential pointwithin a phrase may appear to have an auxiliary relation to a more basic point when the phrase is viewed in a larger context, and the high point in pitch within a single phraseis likely to be interpreted asa subsidiaryhigh point in the analysis of that phrase in combination with others. Two examples follow, each involving a kind of linear analysis of pitch functions having 1eveled significances. The high point of the first phrase of Ex. l-4 is subsidiary to that of the second phraseof the group. This the expansion ofspatial compass!is, along with motive mirror, the chief means by which the content of the opening phraseis varied in the second. Thereis thus a conjunction of formal units into a larger unity by relative superiority here of pitch, but often of duration, stress,density, etc.! of one event over a related eventin a linear sequence.The significance qf bothpoints intonal function
l,
should also be emphasized; and ahigh-level conjunct successionhigh of
points and structurally important cadential pitches, clearly implied despite registral variance,is shownas part of the representation in Ex. l-4. In all references to linear functionof pitch materials, theterm essential is orcourse to beunderstood as meaning of the essenceand notin the more restrictedsense of necessary.
tonality 39 Ex. 1-4. Brahms, Trio in C minorfor piano,violin, andcello, Op.101, thirdmovement. -1 Andantegrazioso
J J Q92_»Lfl'
P
/A
4 /-J: T' "` +. ,_ T
~ ~_._ __ ____ ____ _____. _~_..
_____________
. TE
' EE
5! 1
~@ I-
The phrasefrom the Sequenza IIfor harp by Luciano Berio° Ex. 1-5! is a concise melodicunit, its cadence achievedin part by rhythmic means. The exampleillustrates afurther aspectof hierarchicrelations amongmelodic units within a single phrase. The structural basis is felt in the succession of principal and subsidiary high and low points toward the cadential D, which can be interpreted as analogous tothe traditional tonic. Ex. 1-5. Berio, Sequenza I/ for harp. m.l4* __
g gl
I
I Iii //PP 1 lPP f iff' #ff | gi/,
Pf
PP
II
*Each measure is a fixed durationalunit at 40MM. © 1965, Universal Edition. Used by permission the of publisher. Theodore Presser Company, sole representative United States, Canada and Mexico.
The quotation, with supplementary explanatory symbols,makes these functional interrelations visible. The melodic line assumes akind of wedgelike character seen in this light, its course very purposefully controlled. Several observationsneed to be made in extension of the analysis: both °In the index will be founda listingof citedcomposers and their years. Each vertical bar-line corresponds to onemetronomic pulsationat 40MM. Time distributions withinthe unit are represented spatially, andapproximately, inthe notation of pitch events. Beriouses accidentals only to indicate pedalchange. Accidentals given in parentheses inthe quotation are thoseapplicable by virtue of already established pedal settings.
40 tona/ity
cadence PCs D, A! are immediately preceded by leading-tones whose resolutions arefulfilled in octave displacement;the recessionof low points is diatonic, the sequence ofhigh points chromatic; the stressed notes,marked forte, constitute in a middle register a descent diatonic! toward D, complementing that of the succession oflow points; each subsidiary high point is preceded by a rest and/or upward leap; and a rest precedes theprecipitate cadential succession. It is of course vital that
these relations, functions, and
interactions be understood in performance. Some ofthe examplesof recentmusic tobe presentedin later analytical discussion in this chapter will be concerned with instances of hierarchic linear arrangement and order in which tonality is not, or is less, relevant.
The ideas of pr/macy and hierarchy among pitch-c/asses and p/rch-c/ass-comp/exes; generic and part/cu/ar tona/ systems
The discussion and analytical exploration of tonality, however broadly conceived, tends to center in those particular conventions by which the tonal period is identified. Propositions respectingexpressions oftonal order in earlier and later styles areoften introduced in analogous relation to that most immediately understood conventionalpractice. In the projection ofa primary factor within the PC contentof a musical work or style! much of the weight of historical practice suggests thechief importance of its relation to the pitch a semitone below and, in some styles, above! and the pitch a 4th below or 5th above. The leading or leaning force of the proximate pitch in semitonal relation, and the strong natural relation of the 5th and its inversion, emerge throughout tonal practice of all kinds as preeminentaffiliations by which the central factor in the hierarchic order is approached and understood. The primacy of the tonic is a function too of its role as the ultimate point of cadential arrival. The hierarchic tonal .gzstem can be referred to as oneof genericapplicability-i.e., the tonal systemof a style might be conceivedas that embracing, for example,a diatonic set togetherwith vertical PCC derivativesof common usage and expanded by subsidiary collections of chromatic affiliates usual in such a given style. Thus, the tonal system ofa style would represent the normal range and ordering of PCmaterial reasonably to be expected. In the same way, the tonal systemof any class ofworks might be theoretically conceived. Of more immediate concernis the concept of tonal systemas it applies to a particular musical instance, aparticular work and the experience ofthat The word tonic willbe freelyused to denote thePC or PCC at the centerof the hierarchic orderin all styles in which suchorder is a relevant principle, including those which predateand postdatethe periodof major-minortonal conventions.
tona/ity 41
work, as opposed to the theoretical system genericto the class of which the work is a member. The generic systemmight be, for example,the phrygian scale together with predictable secondaryleading-tones attending and inflating, or tonicizing! certain of its degrees, orthe diatonic scale ofC comparably expanded. The tonal system specificto a composition representsthe total resourceof PC content basicto that work, and the hierarchic ordering of the content asexpressed the in formulations cy' thatparticular work. We will see ofcoursethat particular tonal systemsreflect stylisticnorms; but at the same time the analysis of the tonal system ofa composition will illuminate its tonal structure by making visible the particular directions, range, and byimplication! rates of _fluctuation by which its primary system isexpanded and enriched. Such a particular system may include chromatic factors e.g., Neapolitan harmony! as significantly projected secondarycenters, may be seen to extend a particular modal
ambitus beyond the norm, may be seen to
have some kind of anomalous second-order component e.g., recurrent cadential G within the phrygian context, or predominantly plagal action within the framework of eighteenth-century tonalstructure!, or may be seen as, for example, a primary system consisting of a trichordal or tetrachordal collection rather than the more extendedresource commonto a given generic system. Methodswill be suggested herefor the analytical representation of tonal systems,methods by which the tonal image of a work can be portrayed and its latitude and directionsof fluctuation shown. In the representation of a particular
tonal system the rate of fluctuation
may be inferred in
a general way; but the rate of fluctuation is a rhythmic factor to be treated precisely in subsequent portionsof this book.
Tona/ systems of compos/'t/'ons.' reg/'ons and /'n terre/at/'ons
The specyictonal system may be defined as the collection of tonics tonicized PCS and PCGS! and the supportive encircling, leading, preparing! pitch factors oriented toward each of these."
Example l-6a is agraphic representationof the tonal systemunderlying the expositionand developmentin the first movement of Mozarts Sonata in F for piano, K. 332. It shows aprimagf system on F, and secondary expanding, auxiliary, inflating, embellishing! systemson C, Bb!, d, Eb, G!, and a!; those shownas parenthetical are of distinctly inferior status. Thetonal range expanse, scope,latitude! is immediately evident. Although the ascending order of the chart represents thechronology oftonal rwrencein the piece, it could also be arranged to representa hierarchic ordering based on such factors See pp. 134-38 forseveral representations of thetonal system ofthe fourthmovement of Beethovenssecond symphony.
42 tona/ity Ex. 1-6a. Mozart, Sonata inF forpiano, K.332, firstmovement. Representation the of tonalsystem andextracted key tonal eventsin expositionand development.
a! '
A
, 92__/,
G, g!
A 92/'
-1, P 1f|nuu _;: ' I 1' nr v1 E -inn;
Eb 6
s
C,c gigs: as I7
2 11-rf; -:-ai-'
R/d ` n B/-B
Bb! |
Q 92.Jr
FQ
§___""Dr"°°5
I,
'
| 3;
92/v
IY s
T SD
D,d sm r!
' #2
p
Orcéer refegence
Allegro Z_`
5@ 9292ll ' n I1
ff~ 5
__92
J =» 1 _ 9292.J .- V ass }`-
5=
/=
EEEEEEEEEEEEEFEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEE!
m. 22
Ei? 35%
E
tona/ity
l
1g7é/ f_92 ll ll
f /'ba
f V
Jihizjr fb; bf
ESFC VPJ|'P_| If E fP
fP
[if
44 tona/ity Ex. 1-6a continued. m.1l7
g7
fP
fP
fP
f' J
fP
f
Eggs-f."i% 3 I m.l27 92
°s
l
1¥'
1
"' "E
Pf
as frequencies and durations of tonic occurrence cadential and prominences of such occurrences. Curvedarrows representdirections and paths of especially dominant-tonic! tonal affiliation within the system. The whole is a visible, synopticimage of the tonal resource; it could ofcoursebe extendedto represent the total harmonic
resource as well.
Example l-6b is a further kind of representationof the same example and its tonal system. While relative importances of tonics are suggested by note values, specific durations of tonal reference arenot represented. It is important to emphasize ina graphic exercise ofthis kind that the notation represents PCsonly, not specific pitches,and should of coursenot be read as a pitch-line.
The underlying tonic and dominant components" are offirst-order and second-order significance, respectively, asmight well be expectedin the style of which the exampleis an instance. A tentative representationof fluctuation to and from points of relative distance isgiven below the exampleas further indication of the structural importance of fluctuation within the expanded tonal system. Specific typesof tonal function embellishing, passing! arein evidence here and will
be discussed further later.
Of course,
the relative
superiority of any given tonic of reference isa questionof structurallevel: the central tonic, F, is thus preeminentat the broadest level;C, the dominant, is The term component .9»stem or component refers toa level within the total system-a related system,itself hierarchicallyordered andcapable ofexpansion bysuperimposed inferior systemcomponents. Thus, in the Mozart Ex. 1-6! ,d is a systemcomponent ofrelatively inferior status.
46 tonality
The concept of an emergent secondarytonal system can be seen in a representation of the modestly expanded primary system asshown in Ex. 1-7a. It indicates a tonicized iv as second-ordersystem componentand Ex. 1-7a. Josquin, Tu pauperum refugium. Synopsis oftonal system.
Q uSMa» E I
I'
g
r1== uli 1 1 |19 ll! ; 5 f ¢_ GP=_ 1|
1| CXO |10 li
__ Qsd ilhi10| ,
t
suggests themildly tonicizing effect of the plagal approaches tothe cadential VI as well. The secondary regionon A is tonicized in the second phrase and elsewhereby application of the G# leading-tone. The applicability of the principle of tonal expansion bringingabout limited excursion beyondthe fundamental, primary system is obvious, and its representation as shown provides animage of hierarchic tonal order which can properly be viewedas a foreshadowingof complex tonal structures of later music? In Tu pauperum rqfugium, tonal structure is of course also evident in cadential functions,
and the
hierarchic order
of cadential
functions is
of
fundamental significancein study of the tonality of the piece Ex. 1-7b!. Examples in this chapter reflect theconcept qf tonaligf broadly dqfined, as is evident in the most cursory examination of sources.That concept applies to early music predating the tonal period! no lessthan to tonal music of the twentieth century: it is implicit and essential inthe idea of a scalar or other PC resourcehierarchically disposedin cadential and other focal applications. Indeed, in that senseit is evident in liturgical chant no lessthan in sixteenthcentury polyphony. Beyond that general concept, much music of modal systems canbe seento foreshadowthe conventionsof later tonality as well: the functions of root relations of chords and of primary and secondary
tonality 47 Ex. 1-7b. Josquin, Tu pauperum refugium. Succession and hierarchic order of cadentialcenters. Pronounced texturalcontrast Varied reprise
§¢l.l..% .
--ll Strong i
elaborations
Hierarchic orderof cadentialcenters: third-C
second-a first-e leading-tones in the applications of musieajicta,which show in incipience the means of later, broad tonal expansions by chromatic extensions of fundamental diatonic collections. And the tonal principle of opposition of areas of fluctuation with stability, and of ambiguity with focus of tonal orientation, is evident in many highly provocative sixteenth-century works.
The concept of a generic tona/
system explored
in theory
Since the intent of this chapter is chieHy to explore tonal principles and specific tonal systems manifestin individual works, the concept of a generic tonal systempertinent to a classof compositionsis discussedonly limitedly in a forthcoming illustration. The deduced norm-defining tonal system of a class ofworks, while not a major concern here, can constitute a useful point of referencein the study of stylistic trends, terms, and evolutions. T wo relevant, interestingsources mightbe mentioned,one concemedwith early manifestations oftonality, the other with later manifestations.Edward Lowinskys paper, Tonality and Atonality inthe Sixteenth Century Berkeley: University of California Press,1961! illuminates aspectsof tonal order, as well as what the author calls atonality" and floating tonality in sixteenth-centuryworks, in provocative studiesin which tonality is conceived asa tonally centered organization p. xii!. Examples of sixteenth-century tonality are discussed often in the light of emergent devices common tolater major-minor conventions. Atone pointLowinsky notesthat a net of cadences on varying degrees related to the tonic and organizinga wholework into various sections comes closer to definingtonality p. 15; emphasis added!. Another sourceof substantialinterest andrelevance tothe concept of tonality broadly viewedis RoyTravis, Towards a NewConcept ofTonality ? in journal qfMu.ric Theory, III, No. 2 959!, pp. 257-84. "The theoretical presentation whichfollows is related to,but significantlydistinct from, the chart of the regions suggested byArnold Schoenbergin StructuralFunctions of Harmony London:Williams and Norgate, Ltd., 1954!, Ch.III. The reader isreferred to Schoenbergs theory as astatement ofmajor importancepromulgating atheoretical, generic concept oftonal systemapplicable toderivative representations of the tonal systems of individual works.
48 tona/ity
What follows Ex. 1-10! is a theoretical representationof a chromatic, panmodal tonal system onC, capable of courseof transposition throughout the equal-tempered pitch resource." It is a theoretical system of proposed generic significance as opposedto one Ex. 1-6! in which systemic content derives from a particular context.
A symbology of tonicdesignation akin to Schoenbergsis adopted here. It represents tonicized factorswithin an expanded tonal system by symbols which go beyond the usual letter designation ofPC to indicate the relation of a secondarytonic to the primary tonic. Thus, such symbolsas T tonic!, ST supertonic!, M mediant!, R relative!, and like symbols, as well as lowercase correspondents to representminor systemcomponents, will be used.The affix /T or /t will be used whereconfusion might result as in usesof the sym-
bol D as dominantto distinguishit from that standardletter designation of pitch or PC! ; otherwise itis understoodthat the symbol is one of direct reference tothe primary tonic e.g., st is understood asst/T! _The slashalso shows other kinds and levels of afliliation within the system: e.g.,r/D relative minor ofthe dominant!, etc., as wellas relationsfarther removed:e.g., r/D/st,etc. Again, /t or /T is understood as the concluding term, explicit only when necessary topreclude ambiguity. The symbol r/T or r = relative minor of the primary major tonic; R/t or R = relative major of the primary minor tonic. M or M/T = E, GH, Bin C major; M/t = Eb, G, Bb in C minor; etc.! It is important to recognize that in a representation of this kind these symbols representtonics-actual or potential centersof systemcomponents or in Schoenbergsterm! regions of tonality, not merely harmonic factors.In that sense,SD can be viewed as an extension of the principle of IV and a succession T-SD-Dan extension or inflationof thesuccession VI-I V. Similarly, to indicate relations as well as simplePC identities, symbols like smziv in C, iv/vi or aziv! or D/Tzii in C, ii/V or Gzii! are sometimes used. Relationsof considerableremoval from the primary system SD/SD/N, where N denotes theNeapolitan! are sometimes shown;although such relations can be adducedwithin an infinite breadth of systemicpossibilities, it is important to make distinction between relationsof a theoretical nature like those adducedin the following chart, Ex. l-10! and thoseof a contextual, or practical, basis which are the chief concern of particular analyses!. Example l-10 includes alisting of relations bywhich all PCs within the chromatic primary system canbe relatively closely identifiedwith the central tonic, C. These are listed as nearest relations,i.e., relations cjjifwest terms. Although the N is noted occasionally,in view of its common membershipin many practical individual systems, the relations given are otherwise through diatonic links.Thus, for example, the identification of f il as st/D/r or m/D/D "Other, related generic systems could beadduced aspertinent to phases ofstylistic experience withinthe tonal period: e.g.,that of the unexpandeddiatonic majoror minor collection andits derivativeharmonies, orthat of the bimodal collection withoutfurther chromatic expansion.
tona/ity 49
suggests that its nearestrelation to C easiestdiatonic accessibility!is asseen in Ex. 1-8.Again, note that Ex. 1-10 represents tonal, notmerebf harmonic factors ,°the summary given as Ex. l-8 should similarly be read as consisting oftonics-tonicized harmonies, diatonic system.
Ex.1-8.
rather than
simple harmonic
successions within
a
3 kI iw, QQ., E o ;9292_ ___§____ j
T r/
T D/
r/T st/D/r/T
gs j W s 92/
T D/T
s
D/D/T m/D/D/T
Key signatures are listed as including the tonally vital leading-tone of the minor system a practice shown in Ex. l-6b!. Although
key signatures are
one indicator of tonal distance, thisfactor alone can be deceptive, asis noted in the discussion oftonal distance which follows later in this chapter. A very critical factor in tonal distance or proximity! is the immediate adaptability of a major triad as dominant to T or t-what Schoenberg calledthe interchangeability of mode"-a principle responsible in very significant degree for the expansibility of tonal systemsin conventional tonality. Implicit in Ex.
l-10 area theory of chromatic harmonic succession and a theory ofmultileveled or multiple! harmonic function, bothof thesetreated further as thischapter unfolds. Since theconcept isinvolved with questions oftonal ratherthan purely harmonic range, all degreesin the primary chromatic system areregarded as of tonic function; hence, eachis consideredthe root of a major or minor triad and its theoretical significances andaccessibility derivedfrom that assumption. The minor triad has suchpotential significancesas r, sd, st, m, or sm. The major triad may have tonic significanceas R, D, SD, or SM. Where potential system components are hypothesized, diminished and augmentedtriads 3Schocnberg, Structural Functions, Ch. VII.
50 tona/ity
are notconsidered be to ofpotential tonic function, i.e., to be capable of functioning as system centers. Analyses oftonal fluctuation, as suggested in the chart, might well show the particulars of relation demonstrated in specific contexts;the symbology proposed has the advantage of showing ultimate relations to the primary tonic as well as immediate functions within secondary tonal systems.Such a representation mightbe asin Ex. 1-9, whichincludes abranched diagram in which the dominant systemcomponent region! appears asan outgrowth in the given context! of the primary system, and the third component as an outgrowth again, in the given context! of the second. Thefirst-order system primary! is shown at the base as T. The designation of relations by this principle avoids the often dubious identification oi, say, a Bb triad in expansion of the system of C as lowered subtonic and suggests for it such a plausible, precisecontextual relation as SD/SD. Ex. 1-9. T: V D/T: IV r/D: iv
-I
V/V! -V
etc. V/iii! - vi _ ii
- etc. V/vi! etc. -V - etc.
Finally, in extension of a chart of this kind Ex. l-10!, or in representation of any modification or qualification of this hypothetical, generic, tonal system, further relations-relations of greater complexity, of greater distance, of more terms-could of course be adduced. For example, one might go further, in theory and without reference to any particular contextual derivation, to note such potential relations asr/SD/SD for the minor triad on G, or SM/r/ D/r for the major triad on A, etc.-all such relations potential and plausible in functionaltonal contexts. Implicit in the functional chromatic system is the principle that the chromatic factors have a diatonic basis, i.e., derive as alterations of factors in the basic diatonic collection on T or t.
tona/ity Ex. 1-10. A view of a theoretical, generic tonal system,and hypotheticalproximate dlaton/e re/ations linkingeach ofits potentiallytonicized degrees with Cc Mode of Tonic Examples Tonic Triad Degree Signature
of Close Relations of Diatonic Succession
mo
m/R/t, st/SD/T
Mo
D/T
mU
_W ' _ m/T
-1 e
MU
D/sm r!/T -1
M -l-
R/t
m ii
sd/sd/sd/t -1 e
m
st/T
M ""-;_`__
D/D/T
m ;,
__
M 92
r/D/r/T -1 __
nl D
e
¢
SM/sd/t +N!
'__-_'_-l-___-__ I
M
T
m I!
33'---`_`__
M M_
.2
IT! U1
! ____._______
M-. m M m ` M m M.
m/D/T
!
lg 55' EL li
I! II _
Z D/D/r/T SD/SD/T, D/R/t sd/sd/¢ r sm!/T -1 D/st/T, D/D/D/T m/D/f/'r sm/t
eI
°
52 tona/ity Pr/mary and secondary ton/'cs and the/r structura/ and auxi//'ary functions; mu/ti/eve/ed mu/I/Qo/e! tona/-harmon/c function; tonic and dominant forms
As implied in Ex. 1-6b, tonicscan berepresented inmuch the same waythat structural and auxiliary embellishing harmonies and, in melody, individual pitches! are represented incommon techniquesof linear, reductive analysis. A fundamental difference must, of course, be kept in mind: a stream of tonics in such a graphic, hierarchic representation canin no sense beinterpreted asa sounding line of actual pitch events. Vital to this considerationis the concept ofstructural level. The primary, or central, tonic is of course always of first-order importance as the term primary suggests!. At the same time,secondary tonicsof referencemay be structural inimplications within low-level contexts over whichthey prevaile.g., F at the level of a single phrase over which it has tonal predominance!. But the idea of low-level predominance doesnot pertain to the cadential tonic representing within the sameunit of reference tentative a departure from the primary or other superior tonic, where departure expectsreturn and conditions the interpretation of the tonic of departure, if hierarchically inferior, as submissive, andthe superior tonic asgoverning." The concept of level of tonal structure yields that of multiple tonalharmonic function, or multileveled tonal-harmonic function, a concept by which it is proposed that the true analytical interpretation of tonal function in any given harmonic or melodic! event is often one of numerous terms. The multiple function of tonal harmony is suggestiveof an ambivalence not necessarilyambiguity! in the significanceof any single harmonic event or successioninfluenced by the emergenceof a secondary tonic or complex of suchtonics. At one level,tonal meaning is traceableto the most immediate tonic of reference; otherfunctional meaningsderive from references toother systems operationalin the given context.A number of the examples analyzed in the following pageswill illustrate this concept. The full explanation of a secondary tonalfactor involvesa rangeof tonal functions which, with experience and understanding, can be felt in a way that lends harmonic events within multileveled systems anextraordinary depth of signyicance,and consequent expectationof ultimate, sometimes veryremote, fulfillment within the primary, expanded system. If in a specific tonal structure there is one, and only one, primary system Schoenbergs monotonality!,2° it follows that there is among "Some of the examplesto follow will demonstrate,however, particularcontexts in which the tonic of departure ultimatelyprevails-tonal structures, that is, which setout initially in subordinate regions ultimately absorbed within a culminating, differentsystem e.g., somesongs ofWolf!. 2°Schoenberg, Structural Functions, Ch. III, especially p. 19.
tonality 53
secondary systemcomponents a hierarchic arrangement particular to the musical instance see Ex.1-7!. Given the termsor norms of a particular style it can be predicted that, for example, the dominant or relative might well emerge as second-order systemcomponent. In certain tonal styles other orders are somewhat predictable, often occurring at intervallic distances of the 5th or in other idioms the 3rd!, just as in modal systems PCsof likely cadential prominence can be preinferred. But beyond suchnorms the particulars of tonal system are a vital factor in the character of the individual work and its expressivepotential. The concept of secondarytonal levelsis of course thebasis foridentification of specific harmonic events as secondary dominants, secondary subdominants, etc., and for such a term as secondary leading-tone. All of these havetheir counterparts: primary subdominant, primary leading-tone, etc. We have already made useof the terms primarytonal gystem, primary tonic, secondagf system and .gystem component, etc., as well as expanded tonal gfstem,the latter denoting all primary and secondarysystem componentsparticular to a given composition or class of compositions!, the system componentscollectively viewed.
Those triads of the diatonic system most likely to assumethe role of secondary tonicin music of the tonal period are of course theconsonancesthe major and minor triad forms. Such functions as T:vii°, t:vii°, or t:ii° and III* 2' will function as secondary tonicsonly when modified to appear as major or minor triads.
When t:vii° and t:III+
are tonicized as major triads they normally
involve thelowered 7,a violation of the primary leading-tone,and have a consequent weakening effect on the primary tonic. It is for that reason especially thattonal-harmonic functionsof the minor mode commonly derive from the harmonic form of the scale. I. e., the evolution of major-minor tonality out of modality involved more than any other singlefactor the inflec-
tion ofthe7 asa leading-tone.! For thesame reason tonal change toward the minor tonics relative major R/t! can be regardedas far more disruptive of the primary tonic and its predominance than fluctuation into the dominant region, whose tonic triad unless tzv! has solid diatonic status within the primary system." The leading-tone triad in the majorand minor modes, respectively, and thesupertonic and mediant in the minor mode. 22In otherwords, fluctuationin the direction of R/t t:III tonicized!, exceedingly common intonal forms,is considered here topose theparticular problemof unusualdisruption of the primarysystem incancellation ofthe primary leading-tone! ascompared with the equallycommon fluctuationin the direction ofD/t or D/T, whosetonic isdiatonic, and highly supportive,in relation to the primary center. This will be discussedlater as one manifestation ofdistance between relative systems as comparedwith others of greater apparent distance. Prevalence inthe tonalperiod offluctuation andtonal expansionin the
54 tonality
just as particular chord forms function as tonics tonic forms:major and minor triads! , soparticular chord forms functionas dominants.Dominant forms are those in which the two chief factors of dominant action and potential relation are in evidence or clearly implied: ! the leading-tone and ! the potential for root relation a 5th above the affiliate tonic form. It must be recognized that when the leading-tone, i.e., the note a semitone below the tonic affiliate, is the apparent root of the chord, the particular dominant form in question isa variant of the very closelyrelated form built on the note a major 3rd below that leading-tone.Thus, is
virtually indis-
tinguishable fromand interchangeablein function with .! A resuméof the group of dominant forms is givenin Ex. l-l 1, arbitrarily relating to a potential tonic affiliate D. All have potential dominant function, primary or secondary. Listed forms of III* and I6 are of course 4
understood as highly dependent chords-tentatively delaying the usual dominant major triad of resolution and, in fact, inseparable from auxiliary anticipations of! the dominant triad. To these forms could be added still Ex. 1-11 . A resuméof commondominant forms.
* * ai Q V V1
vii° vii?
Vs
Vo III*
i or U2
*Often considered to havethe dominantdegree as implied root,thus analogous to the corresponding V7 and Vgforms. 1'Usually infirst inversion,the dominantdegree aslowest note. Iln second inversion: delaying,dependent on,succeeded by the V.
others: for example, the dominant 7th-chord with its 5th raised or the dominant triad
with the
same alteration-a
favored device
of the
later
nineteenth century, especially ofBrahms. Of the following dominant forms, the dominant 7th-chord and the diminished 7th-chord on the leadingtone must be recognized as the most important ; it is understandable that such dissonantforms would have priority over the simpler dominant triad because of their strong drive toward tonal resolution. The diminished 7th-chord is favored over the half-diminished because of its versatility in direction ofR/ tmay wellbe aresidual tendency remaining frommodal traditionsof cadential Huctuation lackingdependence on the later leading-tone functionby which major-minor tonality is largely defined.
tona/ity 55
resolution to both maj or and minor triads; other aspectsof its versatility are treated later.
In Ex. l-12 certain factors of apparent tonal significance are quickly evident: there is reiteration of the major triad on C at the outset; there is fluctuation from the diatonic system Fit in m. 2, Bb in m. 5!; both of the accidentals introduced
in the Huctuant portions
are soon cancelled; and the
original C is reaflirmed in these cancellations and by appearance of its dominant in
m. 9.
Ex. 1-12. Beethoven, Sonata inC forpiano, Op.53, firstmovement.
Allegro con brio Z-$_
_. =i¢n|m:_ ll..1V l _ 1 ' -I
1 I3 _
1 H Q: 11 _H _ _ -'=== li!
1_ Z _
|¢1111;111_ 1 .-at-1-7-ri-1-1-71
1- 1:-1-1-1-1-9-I
m.4
/'i
:E _
-' - -_----= -. Ei. _
e=
m.7 /_92 KT- is r92 ~I!-11'!1$1l92:~l .4 IH 1 §|1.
U; xi1=g_:$ :=
E=
|513-D-sennnzrsnnnn-:Hui
'Q
° ====
» li
1 -= H l 1:1 _ _#_-' _- 1
:Er -=
--=-=1 1'-- '= == 1 111-1-1
111-1 1
A 25 -
#nh ° __Il 1
- '-4
- 5:2
IQ
1_
_:lx-_ --_Z-._
'
CTCSC. "'°' Ui I i-1-_-Q.;-_TQ-1-1--_-_--_1_-Ifiuiijnii 1_---I*_------ini!--Z!-= l1lllll1--
-1-I H-Q!-IQ
b 5l'= Tonal Huctuation thus occursprecipitately, and the early threat to the primary tonic, C, is one of the factors conveying a sense of unrest in the opening passages of this work. The first accidental, Fil, is in a veg!local sense a leading-tone, resolving to G, its tonic, as canbe seenclearly if mm. 2-3 are, solelyfor analysis,considered in the contextof the secondary system of G. Since theprimary system is G, not G as examinationof the entire movement would confirm!, the F il must be interpreted asa secondaryleading-tone, theG asa secondarytonic;
56 tona/ity
thus, theseevents havetonal importin somedegree. Theprimary tonal system, on C, is thus expanded andembellished bythe inflated significance attached to its dominant i. e., by the presenceof a secondary dominant,by the tonicizing of C:V!. The above analysis ofmm. 2-3 as expressinga secondary tonic, auxiliary to C, suggeststhat the opening harmony, the tonic of C, has an ambivalent, dual function-again, in a very local sense making possibleby simple diatonic meansthe referenceto the secondary level,G! ; it is also a subdominant in the secondary system on G, hence a secondary subdominant.
The progressionfollowing m. 5 is analogous in important ways: it is a sequential repetition of the opening, a step lower; it is a reference to the secondary tonic,F, involving the samesecondary functions IV, V!. There is an important difference, however:it is not derived diatonically. That is, the introduction of Bb involves chromatic succession mm. 4-5, Bll becoming Bb!, expanding the systembeyond the preceding tonicizedV. The consistent chromatic line in the bass within and surrounding this successioneases the approach, indeed makes it seem inevitable. In broad analysis ofthe Waldstein opening, theseprocesses mightwell be capsulatedas momentarydeviants swiftly brought into focus asembellishments ofthe primary tonic. An analysisof the tonal structure and systemunique to this work accounts for the inflation of that systemby system components auxiliary neighboring, encircling! in relation to it--D/T and SD/T, .gymmetriealbf ordered around the primary C, each prepared by a secondary IV the SD/T:IV alien to the primary diatonic system, moredistant, and chromatically derived!. The entire process leads to an unequivocal T:V7 by
persuasive and inexorable chromatic descent inthe basslinking l and 5. Example 1-12illustrates auxiliarysecondary tonal systems in expansion and embellishment of a primaryrystem. Secondary systems canbe seenin general to have functions of two discernible principal classes: some embellish the primary system, with such embellislzing secondary sjystems succeeded by reaffirmation of the primary tonic; otherscan beseen tolink disparatesystems. Such passing ortransitional secondary tonics alsoembellish and expand, of course, but they have the particular function of participating as connectivelinks in transitional, fluctuant successions. parenthetical A gfstem, which can have either function, is one which has a tentative presencewithin the procedural relations of tonal expansion,but which does nothave morethan a very nebulous, implied manifestation e.g., Bbin Ex. 1-6!. A parenthetical systemcan often be identified as important in defining the relationsby which tonal fluctuation occurs, while its perceptual signficance is uncertain or clearly negligible; see, amongothers, Exx. 1-39 and 1-47.! 23The Al;of m. 8 hasno tonalsignificance; itsimply altersthe modeof thesecondary tonic, F, continuing thebasss chromaticdescent tothe dominant by which the primary tonic ispowerfully reaffirmed.
tonality
57
In the Beethoven,G and F appear as embellishing secondarysystems symmetrically neighboring the primary C. The issueof the levelof structure taken as referential is essentialto the characterization of function of a system component: thus, for example, in the Beethoven,G is passingat one level, embellishing neighboring, surrounding, encircling at a broader level. At the broadestlevelof structureall secondary systemcomponents can be interpretedas expanding theprimarysystemandelaboratingtheprimarytonic. The third of Liszt s Transcendental Etudescontains, just before its conclusion, a provocative expansion of the primary tonality conditioned by a largely chromatic descent in the basslinking F:I with F:V Ex. 1-13a!. Ex.1-13a. Liszt,Transcendental Etudes,No. 3 in F Landscape!.
ritenuto ed appassionato assai
tona/ity 59
In the Liszt successionreferences areremote from the primary system and the means of tonal fluctuation extreme. There are firm primary tonal pillars framing the passage I, V! and the fluctuation is swift and brief; yet the auxiliary tonics emergeas distinctsecondary passing functions, enriching the primary system chromaticallyin the works final developmental episodes. As examples for analysis reflect progressivelylate styles in the tonal period, increasinglyprovocative questionsmay arise,since extremesof range and fluctuation
are more common in
later nineteenth-century
music. The
parameters arethose offrequenqy qf tonal change the quantitative facet of tonal rhythm! and distance and volume qfehange-both vital questionsof style. Tonal expansion, especiallyas it concerns thesecond ofthese considerations, might be symbolizedas in Fig. 1-1. Fig. 1-1 . Symbolic representation of tonalexpansion. 61-! primary
Expanding resources within_the system and within other individual system components
+ Expanding range of fluctuationinto secondary systems; increasing-number and distances of components
Tonal expansion
In more Huctuant stylesof highly chromatic idioms there aresometimes extremely remote and rapidly driven fluctuations in the tonal structureeven at such critical stages asthe beginnings of works, creating tonal forms which might be representedgraphically asin Fig. 1-2, thelevel portion showing the ultimate stable expressions relativefocus! of tonality after widely ranging fluctuation at the outset. Indeed,this isa shapingprinciple by which a great deal of later tonal music is conceived. Fig. 1-2. Symbolic representation of tonal structure broadlyreceding towardfinal pointof relative focus.
L
tona/ity 61
unlike many instrumental as well as vocal works of highly chromatic idiom less commonand generally less severe Classical precedents of courseexist! in which the primary tonic is very richly expanded-its prevalence even impaired-at such crucial points, or at great length in the forms internal stages.
It must be emphasizedthat the sketch belowthe Brahms quotation is contrived to show tonal, not merely harmonic, succession.
Essential and auxi/iary //'near functions of pitches and pitch-comp/exes, the/T hierarchic basis often determined tonal and cadentia/ factors
by
A central premise of the present study is evident: harmonic and melodic analysis is,in an aspect complementaryto that of concern with tonal function, the interpretive identification and evaluation of auxiliary and essential linear functions which make up the stream of successions. While of course not the only approach to harmonic and melodic analysis, norits only important aim we might note, for example, the importance of study of style characterization!, the identification and evaluation of linear functions are nevertheless basic. The identification of some harmonic or
melodic factors
as essential
basic! and others asauxiliary elaborative! is not a matter of absolutes.The conclusions reached,based upon a complex of factors less objective than those by which tonal function is determined, constitute an interpretation of linear .functions. Linear function is strongly conditioned by tonal function as well as, at lower levels
or in nontonal contexts,
rhythmic prominence,
reiteration, stress,cadential position, and like characteristics ofevents. There are, of course, someinterpretations which are readily seen to be ill-founded while others derive from persuasive logic.And in the midst of such extremes many analytical interpretations represent a range in which right and wrong are in somedegree inapplicableconcepts. Onlyin some instances is there reasonfor insistence upon a right interpretation to the exclusion of others.
In the identification and analysis ofessential harmonicfunctions weare inevitably much concerned with the analysis of cadencesand their relative strengths. Cadentia]events areinvariably of fundamental, essentialfunction at somelevel, thelevel ofsignificance dependingon thestrength ofthe cadence in relation to others, its strength a product of such subtle cadential qualities as registral placement, distribution of harmonic factors, metric prominence, linear approach to the cadence point,and especiallythe positions ofthe cadential harmony in relevant primaiy and seeondaty tonal systems and the place ofthe cadence
62 tonality
in form conclusive asopposed to preliminary!. A predominant preoccupation of harmonic analysis is thus the analysis of cadence-its functional identity and character, the motions toward it, its relation relatively affirmative, tentative, etc.! to cadences which precede and succeed it. These are vital, crucial considerations in all harmonic analysis of whatever nature or premises, andwith respect to whatever musical styles. In very much conventionally tonal music, harmonic analysis will reveal a broad prevalence of the primary often triadic! harmonies: tonic, dominant, and subdominant i. e., I and its chief auxiliaries a 5th removed!, and especiallyof the first two of these.In such tonal music the vast majority of harmoniesdesignated asessential in the basicframe of structure must be I and V-the latter, when tonalmusic isviewed inbroadest terms, an auxiliary support andembellishment ofthe former, for which it is the principal medium of tonicization. Conventional tonal composition thusgenerally manifeststhe central structural functions of I, IV, and V; in fact, we tend to regard others as tentativeor allied forms of these. Thus,we think of and hear! II as functionally interchangeablewith IV having considerableoverlap of PC content,
and oftenoccurring infirst inversion with fl as bass note!; weregard VI, preceded by V, as a tentative substitute for,
I-a delaying
action which
brings aboutresolution ofthe leading-tone and introducesl but otherwise suggests furthermotion toward more conclusiveresolution. And we consider VII analogous in function to V-almost indistinguishable from V,, and III, often III,, as V-like in function because ofan overlap of PC content which
duplicates theleading-tone andthe 5 itself The centrality of I, IV, V in conventional tonal composition is necessarily acrucial basis for distinctions among relatively essential and relatively auxiliary harmonic events in the linear stream; and the discussions whichmake up much of this chapter are concerned with this underlying premise. While any and all dimensions andparameters of musical articulation are potentially signiiicant in the interpretation and eH`ectof essential and auxiliary functional distinctions, precluding any simple,general ranking and accounting of such factors, the following broad observations can be useful if they are not read as absolute and universal in their implications. ! Cadential harmonies areessential inthe linear functional hierarchy, although at relatively broad levels ofstructure weakercadences areauxiliary to more affirmative, tonally and formally more decisive cadences towhich they are related.
! The
primary tonic and dominant are very often, in conventionally
tonal music, the essential substance of the structural
frame. However,
indi-
vidual tonic and dominant harmonies can, in low-level rhythmic formulations analogousto thosein Ex. 1-15, functionas highly subordinate auxiliary events.
tona/ity 63
C 'ESM-tt EI I Y' iV
! Many
V
factors extrinsic to actual, specific PC content can at lower
levels affect, even determine,
the structural
value of a harmonic or melodic
event: e.g., stress, reiteration, duration, etc. Ex. 1-15 is pertinent to this point.!
! In nontonal styles,such factorsextrinsic to actual PC content, and the factor of cadential occurrence, areof increasedsignificance. The general superiority of tonal primacy, where tonality is relevant, to other manifestations of structural value can be illustrated and empirically demonstrated in the imagination or in improvisation of a frequent situation in tonal music: that in which a dissonantfactor a dominant 7th-chord, an appoggiatura, a dominant triad over tonic pedal, etc.! is of great relative duration i. e., of decisive agogicsuperiority! in comparison withits cadential resolution. No ear would deny the superior structural value of the higherlevel resolution, the ultimate aim of motion, the medium of absorption of preceding dissonantenergy and intensity. Finally, all evaluation oflinear like tonal! functionmust restupon evidence intrinsic tothe particularcontext inquestion. The first prelude of Bachs Well- Tempered Clavier, Vol. I Ex. l-16! illustrates harmonic prolongations and auxiliary embellishments. The issue of distinctionbetween superiority of tonal and linearfunction andmetric accentual! valuemust beintroduced verytentatively atthis point, to be developed further in Chapter 3. We are not, in the present context,saying thatmetric oraccentual value necessarily inheresin thechord ofcadential resolution by virtue of itstonal primacy.In Chapter3 we shallwish tomake afirm distinctionbetween metric structure, inwhich unitsare delineated by accentdissonance, density, duration, stress, etc.! and the kind of structuralbasis whichis our concern in the present chapter.In the present frameof reference,the superseding cadential eventultimately I! is thesupreme tonal-harmonic-melodic factor uponwhich all other eventsof pitch and pitch-complexare suspended in elaborating, prolonging actions.
But it is notnecessarilymedium a of metric accent,or a delineator initiator! of metric
unit. Indeed, within the elements ofpitch content the primary tonal event I, or 1! is the object ofrecessive action. We shall take theposition in Chapter 3 that articulationsin which elaborating PCand pitch events areseen as grouped infunctional associations around more basic, structuralevents interact,as do purely formal-cadentialgroupings ofevents, interestingly andvitally in musical structurewith often counteractive delineations of metricaccentualb' pnyected! units and unit relations.
64 tona/ity Ex. 1-16. Bach, Prelude No. 1in C We/l-Tempered Clavier, Vol. 1!. Representation of linear andtonal functions. Sequential patterns, each repeated once
m.1 ,-
_ --.,I_"
m.19
i/% -é
Q 7%
VQ
TI E
: II Q 92
Q ..
if-`: Q
ti 92.Z T!
_;_.:;_|. 1*r-PP : Q.,_IV p '
»1
R ~ .' _
. ~_,
E 92__'
Xp: `/1
'2
/
§92_di "_llrl!!!_ E
E_ 92-' _aj !` ' Z '_
i C T!:l V!
I vi
v/v v
ii/V! G D/T!: IV ii V
v ii
I! ii
Vl
II [d st/T!: N vii° i] representation of multileveled tonal function
*Note bassdescending diatonic scale cl-c!.
I i_. AC-I ze; I uii v
iii f I-ll _iS II
ES; 4 11 I' I_
7 U*/V
I_
ii; Y' 4 Y'
iii 1'
___l7_i' 4 --_ UI
____ ___ --I II
»
Ya _lf I__-
m.3 --.4 1' Y. 1' 7
_IP2'GT' Y_
Lia ei ;__,,»r, ° Yi o' 3, I
--- ____ _Y I' _I/ 2 I
_ I.
' 1:
17-9;"|' 7_9;"f
92-/ |57
.
tonality
m.7
m. 11
m. 15
66 tona/ity Ex. I-16 continued. m.l7
II
2° I
. J ."J;'ilg ° ` The function of iii and V, in the prolongition-embe1lishment ofthe 25
opening Iis particularlyclear inthe voice-leading, in the easy adjacency of pitches of the two auxiliaries. Reading ahead fromthis relatively brief pro-
longation whichserves to establish the primary tonic!,we cansee thatthe ultimate aim of the ensuing succession is the toriie of m, 19, toward which
there isgeneral descent, made visiblein the analytical sketch,The factof linear descentsuggests a melodic recession complementary to that of the tonal-harmonic coursetoward I.! The diagonal lines drawn through the bas; of the Sketchindicate the succession ofpassing auxiliary harmonies linkingth; essential point; at either extremity in this overall succession. Auxiliaryelements ofrhythmic-metric emphasis might be indicated by the symbol 1 , used in other, comparable
situations.! Twosequences the in descending, passing stream are bracketed and they of course represent ahighly standardized convention of the style. Harmonic content beyond m. 19 consistsof an expansive aflirmation of I
by a broad cadentialformula encirclingI with its primary harmonic supports: I V/IV!-IV-V/V-V prolonged
for nine measures, 23-31, eight of
these over the 5pedal!-I. There are two secondary tonal references,one to the dominant, the
other to the supertonic,the latter derived chromatieally,There arealso subsequent, ephemeral secondary references to the snbdominant region, e.g., mm. 20-21, and again to the dominant, m. 22.!
The romannumeral symbols show whathae. emerged as amost important factor in the theories ofanalysis developedhere; the deep tonal significance multiple, or multileveled, function! of harmonies, For example, the
tona/ity 67
minor triad on A, followed bythe C: V/V, is shownas viin the primary system, as ii in the secondary system,and as ii/V in the primary system; indeed, it has allQf these signifcances within a leveled perspective. The later D minor triad is both i in the secondary systemand ii in the primary system-a tonicized primary supertonic. The dual or multiple meanings ofharmonies in tonal contextsare thus here reemphasizedas a fundamental premise. The designation by roman numeral symbol of a harmony in two or three lights e.g., the Czvi, ii/V, Gzii! is not an inconsistency; it is a realistic representationof the range of functional significancesattending a harmony of both primary and secondary contexts of tonal reference.The identification of such multiple functions is a reflection of the depthof tonal-harmonic meaning and of the way harmony is heard in tonal music. The concept ty" multiple function isnecessarythe in fullexplication qftonal-harmonic signyicance. Tona/ order as an inf/at/'on of harmonic order
and succession
lt- is well to pause here to make explicit the concept of tonal order and hierarchy as directly analogous to, and an inflation of, harmonic order, hierarchy, and succession. Harmonic rhythm thus has its counterpart in tonal rhythm and all parameters ofharmonic structure their counterparts in tonal structure.The inflationary process bywhich IV or 92 VJ IV
becomes SD is oneof tonicization; fl becomes the basis not merelyfor a harmonic factor, but for a secondary tonal system expanding the primary tonal system.
Thus, there is an important sensein which, as a hypothetical example, a five-part rondo might be seenas an inflation extension, prolongation! of the contrived
harmonic structure
illustrated in
92 ,D/t: vii-I isdz I
Ex. 1-17.
I
68 tonality
An example of tonal expansion isgiven in Ex. 1-l8a. In Ex. 1-l8b, the succession oftonicized factors tonal events!can be seen asa broadly, richly inflated underlying harmonic succession. Ex. 1-18a. Chopin, Prelude No.9 in E. Largo *
...S /'T
|_
I'
- _ _lla _
_
""*""5§5§£5?£5§ =EEF-§E5§§5§é= 33 33 cresc. ------
_ - ,,_,,,q
lsrmlli / XBl|'ll!Il 'lil "I'
J°
-!
-oo
--
69
tonality
theless,it is IV of the primary system.The restoration of the primary tonic is accomplishedby enharmonic-diatonic successionseep. 73! in which a/:i acts pivotally as E:iii. Example 1-18b shows without fidelity to original voice-leading! the entire passageas an elaboration of E: I again with linear expressionof the factors of the prolonged tonic its root, 3rd, 5th!. The passingsecondary tonics are identified as embellishing tonal functions. Ex.1-18b. Enharmonic-diatonic succession
A I',a~/gg R/sd/SM /t m /T! F,f SD,sd/SM/t! C SM/t! F.,e T,t! .
The "vagrant" diminished 7th-chord is important in the progression: the harmony at the end of m. 7, notated with E!, looks back to the tonicized F through which the music has passed;notated with F>, it looks ahead as a dominant to A>.
Tonal fluctuation and techniques ofimmediate which itis effected
succession by
Tonal fluctuation is, of course, denoted by the appearance of accidentals, whether they represent shifts to primary, embellishing, or passing systems. tially tonic or dominant harmony may occur purely as a sonorouslinear factor having no significanttonal function. On the other hand, if a dominant form is suggestiveof a tonic prevalentin local or broadcontext,or if it hassufficientdurationalor other emphasis,it may havefunctional effectwithout associationin the immediatecontextwith the expectedtonic of resolutionor its deceptivesubstitute,i. e., without normal root movementor expected resolutionof the leading-tone.The conceptof nonfunctionalityis applicableto only low levels ofstructure, and thejudgmentthat a particularfactor is "nonfunctional"is, inevitably,often interpretive.
70 tona/ity
In theory, T: IV can be tonicized by I without accidentals, but this is rare
and, without7, notreally convincing.2"! Accidentals may, of course,appear l»
without tonal significance, as when the normal raised7 of the harmonic minor scale appears, whenpurely modal changes occur,when altered nonharmonic tones are used, or when there are cancellations of any of these alterations lackingtonal significance.The special concern of tonal analysisis the change or set of changesintroduced in the harmony so as to have the effect of making referenceto a new tonic. Alterations through which the primary system persists unimpaired in its hegemony i.e., those of no tonal significance! will be cancelled shortly after appearanceexcept whenthe changeis oneof more or lesslasting modal shift.
The identification of specificplaces wheretonal referenceshifts isbased upon objective criteria, while again the force of the shift is often a considerably subjectivequestion to some listenerseven themost elusivechange may upset anestablished tonalsense!. Toput the issue anotherway, the question of the existence of tonicization relevant at somelevel isone of substantially objective determination; the question of a hierarc/zic ordering fy .system components, likethe judgment that a tonic emergesonly parenthetically, is one in which determination is in some part subjective. Our definition of tonal_fluctuation willbe simple and inclusive: it is a change of tonal reference, secondary or primary in its implications, and however immediate in its significance. We will consider that a secondary tonic is as a rule supported by the appearance of its leading-tone i.e., is tonicized by a dominant form! or, occasionally, byother means", normally including but conceivably without the expected tonic resolution or its substitute, since the dominant alone may have strongtonal implications. Tonal fluctuation involves tonal events-harmonic successions irylated by tonicization;every tonicization is of tonal significance in some degree. Tonal fluctuation is thus significant with respect toa level of function: for example, a secondaryleading-tone may have purely foreground impact. Moreover, thesignificance ofaparticular event in the tonal structure depends in part on the environment in which it occurs. Thus,if a tonicization occurs Wl denotes the lowered seventh scale degree. b 2°The theoretical problem ofmodulation is, again, entirelyone ofthe level of analysis perception, observation!. Any changeof tonal significance-any tonicizationother than that of prior incipienceor prevalence-ismodulation in some degree ranging frompurely parenthetical tovery substantialexpansions of the primarysystem. Thesignificance oftonal shift isthus measurable in accordwith the level ofstructure overwhich itseffect isrelevant; it is true ofcourse thatthe retentiveinfluence ofa prior system, likethe extentof its supersession byan enteringone, isa partly subjective issue. °'I`onicization canof course,although lesscommonly, bebrought about by plagal action andeven, intheory, bydurational orother insistence on aparticular tonicform without supportof attendantharmonic functions.
tonality
71
within an atmosphereof fluctuation e. g., during the courseof what we shall refer to as accelerated tonalrhythm!,it has more persuasiveeffect as tonalchange than one that occurs within a stable closed! context in the tonal structure e. g., one that occurs over a pedal point, or one surrounded by persuasive referencesto an underlying system having predominance over a broader structural level!. For example, the tonicization of D/T at the end of an antecedent phrase will have significance in its cadential affirmation at the level of that phrase,while at the level of the entire period of which it is part the function of D/T will be seento be tonal embellishment of the prevailing T which the consequentreaffirms. Finally, this study continues to make the point that the analysisof harmony, including tonicized harmony, is ideally comprehensiveand multileveled, with the multiple interpretation and identification of harmonic function a necessary consequenceof the fact of structural levels in music.
Secondary or renewed primary! tonal references shifts, fluctuations, modulations! involve particular techniques of derivationWhile.such techniques are of less interest to many theorists than are the broad outlines of tonal
structure, we would argue for their importance, like the importance of many sharply felt eventsthat are of primarily local impact, in the expressiveeffect of the musical surface.How a tonal inflection takesplace the processcan be decisive in the quality of its expressiveimpact. Processes,or techniques, of tonal change can be classifiedin a way that provides a basis for useful inferencesand conjecturesrespecting such effect. A diatonicsuccession occurs between two pitch factors pitches, PCs or PCCs! which coexist in a single diatonic tonal system" not
necessarily the
primary system in whose context the successionoccurs, possibly even a parenthetical system. A diatonic tonal fluctuation occurs through a diatonic succession Ex. 1-19!.
Ex. 1-19.
Diatonic succession
~ IW ~C-F 30Forthesedefinitions,"diatonic A tonal system"in the minor modeis consideredto be the harmonicA scale,with the raised7, sinceit is this scaleform which expresses tonal function. The lowered7 is a melodicfactor tendingto subvertthe tonal senseby a leaningtoward the relativemajor.
72 tona/ity
A chromaticsuccession occurs between two pitch factors which do not coexist in any single diatonic tonal system Ex. 1-20!. It often involves a chromatic inflection G-Glt, B-Bb, Db-DU, etc.!, but this is not necessarily true consider an augmented-6th chord and its resolution!. A~chromatic tonal _fluctuation occurs through a chromatic succession. Ex. 1-20. Chromatic succession
;VJJ-gJ1inflection Chroma J
V` 92
VV
V
The hypothetical generic tonal system discussed earlier implies a possible, interesting theory of chromatic successiongermane to its apparent experiential effect. That is the theory that chromatic succession, whenof tonal significance,is in effect tonalelision Ex. l-21!. Thus, for example, the Ex. 1-21. r_*-f0l1iCS'ii| tonic ,c
Q 1 Q tg
*I TD
D/D D/D/D
tonic
z an
*I T D/D/D 92 tonal elision
,
modulatory processby which SM/T = D/st/T, etc.! is derived, when one of direct chromatic succession, maybe regarded as an elision of the process T-D-D/D-D/D/D = SM!. This theory of chromatic fluctuation proposes that the fact of omission of stages terms! in a potential diatonic series of underlying relationsin somedegree accountsfor the effect ofapparently wide tonal thrust in relatively little time. Whether relatively distant regionsemerge directly chromatically! or gradually from T is an important facet of style." Since very nearby regions can bederived chromatically e. g.,approach tothe SD of C by theinflection B-Bb!,the presence of chromaticsuccession does not of course initself imply tonal distance.
tonality
73
An enharmonic succession is one in which enharmonic equivalence pertains and enharmonic change occurs, explicitly or implicitly. It the succession! may be diatonic or chromatic, so that it is proper to distinguish between an enharmonic-diatonicsuccessionand an enharmonic-chromatic succession.Where
tonal significance is involved, these are further techniques of tonal shift." In Ex. 1-22a, diatonic relation pertains between the two harmonies, assumingthe necessaryenharmonic interpretation; in Ex. 1-22b,a chromatic nondiatonic! relation pertains. Ex. 1-22a, b. Enharmonicdiatonic successions
e7 Note: x p
el»
g!'.
y an enharmonicrelation; z a diatonic relation.
Enharmonicchromatic succession
atic inflection
+
E7 Note: x p
z E/y ~ p an enharmonicrelation; z a chromatic relation.
ssAnalysts sometimes speakof an "abrupt modulation"asa classification of technique, but it mustbe seenthat if the term is construedto denotean "unprepared"entry into a new tonal region, the conceptis contradictory and illusory. While a modulationcan when associatedwith the entry o+» >[> ~~
>> y>~
>[>~ ~>y>~ » y» >y/y»7
chromatic successionto Ff: "I". It is interesting that the harmony of m. 18 is a shadowyreflection of thoseof the preceding bars; it is thus of somewhat dominant character, although its minor 7th, E, is delayed to give the harmony relatively greater stability. After a seriesof auxiliary harmonies on E, D, B in a descendingstream! the Ff harmony is returned as the ultimate tonal resolution.
The techniqueof tonal shift which had earlier introduced the secondary tonic E mm. 9 12! is related and should be compared. Measure 9 begins with a quasi-dominant of F$. This harmony is "horizontalized" note the bassnotesCf, Gf, B not shownin the sketch! until at the secondhalf of the samebar the basssettleson the primary tonic note; but the chromatic successionof parallel auxiliary chords above the bassnow settlesinto an harmonic complex making of that Ff a dominant 9th-chord. The dominant 9th on Fll becomesa minor 9th-chord in m. 10, the Fx so it is notated in the voice part! resolving into E:I after tantalizing delay, and the C serving as a chromatic link to the B of E:I. Despite these passing, nonharmonic factors, the progressionat its basis is diatonic, the two essentialharmonies having diatonic relation in B, representedas a parenthetical system in the structure. One further element in the succession described above must be noted:
it is an harmonic elision of a sort typical of the style. The dominant on F$ "expects" B, which would in turn be a potential dominant of E. But the movement here omits the B harmony which would resolve the one and prepare the other. Viewing the successionin this light helps to clarify its nature as a diatonic rather than chromatic succession, and further accounts for the
"parenthetical" function of B. Finally, attention is called to some summary concluding notes which
tona/ity 147 have direct reference to the sketch in Ex. l-47b. The reader should note: the
extent ofchromatic relation in contiguousharmonies, functionaland nonfunctional, especiallyin embellishing successions; stylistically characteristic resistance to semitonal successions in the voice-leading reflected in the overall tonal successions F# -E-Fd!; the significance of an underlying motive,
,
as bassline determinant; the types of embellishing
chords e.g., major dominant 9th-chords and derivative half-diminished 7th-chords!; means of tonal stabilization opposing the activating power of dissonance-the broad F il-E-F# successionof prolongation and neighbor auxiliary; comparison of the I of m. l and its recurrence atthe end; m. 15 as avery visible prolongation and embellishment ofsurrounding, functional, structural harmonies e. g., retention of gil!; mm. 18-22 asprolongation of F it:I; the recurring e of mm. 18-20 asa backward look toward the secondary tonic E; and most fundamentallythe underlying closed and explicit manifestation ofthe primary system, whose expansions areunorthodox in the quasi-modal prominence of the system on E, but nevertheless, intheir secondary tonics, entirely diatonic more strictly, pentatonic! in their relation to F it .5° Bartok. String Quartet No. 2. Op. 17. third movement
The Bartok movement posessome relativelycomplex problemsof tonal analysis, butits approachesto tonal structure are stringent and definite: it is a structure whose chief foci are diatonic to A. Consider, for example, the tonal implications of the succession inEx. 1-48a,which constitutesa recurring Ex. 1-48a. Bartok, StringQuartet No.2, Op.17, thirdmovement.
Lento JS63-60! -m.20
Y # jf ry P 12-Llf
Copyright920 I byUniversal Edition; Renewed 1948. Copyright and Renewal assigned to Boosq CQ Hawkes, Inc. for the U. S.A.!.
The collection of secondarytonics, togetherwith the primary, havea very characteristic formulationwhen renderedin a chordal configurationas PCssuperimposed over E: . Compare the formtaken byFil :I pearances.!
in openingand concludingap-
148 tona/ity
harmonic motive in the movement. While some fragmentsof Bart6ks music are quoted in connection with the following discussion, the reader should, ideally, have the scoreat hand.! The succession is stronglyaflirmative of A,a,°° the tritone F,B acting as a quasi-dominant of the sort discussedearlier. With the tonic A in mind it is established early in the movement!, consider thetonal implications of the lines in Ex. l-48b, each ofthem directed toward or otherwise giving prominence to leading-tones of A, the tonic resolution explicit in the bass following in each instance. Ex. 1-48b. m. I5 _ :;r1i _
P My
difn. _. QYZ 3. -1 I
o
#0 or
rn. 23 ff r P'
Like other freely tonal music and this work is part of a vast body of music of comparable aestheticin the twentieth century!, that of Bartok relies upon someof the techniques oftonal expressionnoted earlier, especially the following: emphasis upon the tonal center and its supportive functions by iteration, prolongation, stress, etc.;affirmation by quasi-functional harmonic and melodic elements resemblingconventions ofthe tonal period the quasi-
dominant actioncontinues torely heavilyupon theconcept ofleading-tone tendency and accustomed rootmovement!; and by linear direction toward and indirect or direct encirclement of the tonic PC, especially at points of formal punctuation. °°I nthe Bartokmovement, the tonal system is typicallyone inwhich modaldifferences are freelyexploited. Symbological distinctions between A and a despitethe movements conclusion on the minor3rd sonority!,or betweenl and i, etc., are notobserved inthe discussion of this example.
tona/ity 149 The initial
two motives in the cello form, with the violas E,
a kind of
evasive tonic -|- dominant! sonority in which the root, A, is tentatively suggested byencircling motions above and below: B-Bb and G# are pivotal in these movements cfl A: VH!. Thereafter, the cello phrases consistently conclude onA, which is alwaysa longer note mm. l 1-12, 15-16, 21-22! and often directly supported by leading-tones aboveand below. Its descending triad of mm. l8-l9- may be said, moreover,to act as V to the I minor 6th, C# and A! which follows. At the same time, the long major 6th
in the
viola mm.
9-12, then
15-17! resolvesas a V to the later octave C, eventually absorbed into A: I. Note that this major 6th is also part of the traditional A:V-,, just as the tritone of m. 20 is both suggestiveof C and an element in the traditional dominant 9th of A, which of course prevailsat the cadence. One of the most compelling of the melodic formulations of-quasi-dominant character is the first violin motive of mm. ll-12, beginning on one leading-tone of A, ending on the other, strongly expectant of the tonic PC, which is held in reserve. The same is true of the second violin motive,
mm.
l-2, which accompanies thetonic note. The opening measure thusboth establishesA and sets againstit a Vlike motive primary dominant augmented 6th?! in the second violin. Thereafter, within the first section, the melodic units often are projective of and/or directed to G# and Bb leading-tones toA, sometimes,as a reflection of ambivalent tendencies, notatedenharmonically! and D#/Eb E is established earlyby emphasisin the viola, andquasi-leading auxiliariesare strongly felt in the recurring Eb of viola, mm. .13-14, andsecond violinthereafter; in the first violins active tritonal successionof mm. 1-2; in the major 2nd of m. 5, etc.; and especially at the releaseof the first violin, m. l9'!. While these interrogative leading-tone gestures and motives occur, the cello part continues tobe directedconsistently andrepeatedly towardthe A of the cadence. The longest notes inthese opening 22 measures are E, D, C, B, and A, their functions in the tonality obvious. It is interestingthat despitesuggestive leaning toward E, and early emphasison E in the viola, that PC fails to emerge cadentially, omitted from the quasi-tonic sonority at m. 22, perhaps because of its subsequent prominencein the tonally contrasting section to follow. Immediately before the cadence,there are two tentative suggestions of quasi-tonic harmony: the end of m. 20, where the harmony is activated by a suspended Bin the bass, delaying the root; and the second half of m. 19, where thelower instrumentsexpress thetonic at the sametime that the upper instruments expressthe inconclusivedominant.°' Preceding this is a quasi"Quite possibly,the Cil, Dil, G8 sonority ispredictive ofthe C8 secondary tonicto come.
150 tonality
dominant which includes the upper leading-tone, Bb, notated as All! of exactly the kind referred to in theory earlier in this chapter. In Ex. 1-48c are some of the melodic and harmonic materials to which
we havereferred, aswell as others that are supportiveof the tonality of these opening bars. Measures 23-27 repeat the cadential formula, reestablishing the A tonic, coincident with the now familiar G# -Bbleading-tones, thento leadinto and prepare the contrasting lento whichfollows. It is impossible to examine every detail, but let us at least point out further manifestations ofthe cited techniques oftonal expression. Again there are clear examples oftonal direction and orientation. The first violin of mm. 23-27 articulates a phrasebeginning and ending with the two A leading-tones, stronglyemphasizing Bb.Later it comes torest on G# mm. 33-36!, which resolvesto A, then conjunctly climbs to and insistently reiterates E, as part of the cadence at m. 46. The final harmonies of this section are ambivalent in tonal meaning. The violins, one sustainingE, the other reiterating A and its upper leadingtone, are supportive of A, and the F of the inner voices seems notto have departed far from the primary tonal system. But the ultimate thrust of the cello toward c and
clk' foreshadows, in the environment of
an inconclusive
cadence, theC# tonality to follow. The more intense outer voices piano,as opposed tothe pianissimo of secondviolin and viola! project the C#,E seeonday quasi-tonic sonority.
The Ct#has beensuggested harmonically,of course,much earlier. The sustained, motivic harmonies at the beginning of this section first reiterate and reconfirm the quasi-dominant and quasi-tonic functions in A mm. 25-27!. But at mm. 29-31 they effect a tonal shift pointing to Cl, the latter tonic sonority having the same form as that of A which preceded. From here to m. 46 the melodic lines arein essenceexpressive ofA and Cif, sometimes ambivalent
between the two tonics,
like the first violin
from m.
31,
which has quasi-dominant suggestiveness of Ci# at the same time that the resolution of the long G8 to A is reminiscent of earlier tonal reference. The second violin from m. 27 also tends to expect C8 ; it prepares perfectly the eventual C8,E resolution at m. 31. In the climactic, essentially rising passageleading up to the cadence, thesubtle hints of C# combined with its careful evasionare of enormous importancein the effectiveness ofits ultimate arrival in the cello. Consider,for example, the substitution Q/'Bit _/or C itin theotherwise diatonic rise ofthe jirstviolin fromgit to es. The transitional passage towhich we refer above is sketched in Ex. 1-48d in a manner intended to show in larger notes, by enclosures,by arrows, by connecting lines,and by other symbols! some of the expressionsof tonality we have notedas wellas othersof importance and comparablefunction and technique.
152 tonality Exe 1°48d|
Tonal fluctudderivation ion Cl:ofl"! mm.23-36
I
=" ""*_1'.. :Qi-.iw ~ ` 9292|' .AW
_ rr
IQY'_ na U1 ri1
--" _/` "ir II
--'- "-------
if / 1 '
.| il/ ul
/ ll
~ 92
as -. 92_/ `_/"
"`L""iF1?'|S&If-§5".§f15C ' i, 4|"F='l|ll I ff* [L_
A "7
|| _II -1111 / l . I -iv ./ I
' --~»
Increasing of cl factorspredominance
Structural emergence of cl , Gll, E.
-11; ""' J "',~~ y
mm.37-47 '
=,_ "`*°§f"ii ii
92 l
t ' #;_ _;ff_; __ __,"£
Largely chromatic linear successions toward¢- A, CI i+ , andoi l-1-----»
It is necessary fromhere on to limit discussion to the most essential points, leaving for the readers independent investigation a number of elements of tonal expressioncomparable to those alreadyidentified in earlier portions of the movement. The closerelationship of C# to A, the fact that G# is both the dominant root of one and the leading-tone to the other, and the coincidence of the notes C#and E in both tonic triads, make it possible forBartok to express a certain ambivalencein the contrasting material following m. 47, to establish tonal variety by functions suggestive ofCH! without relinquishing implications of the original tonality. Note too the interrelations explicit in the forms the quasi-tonic harmonies take: the occurrence of A:I,,, with Cll as its lowest pitch, in the motivic harmonic succession, as well as the implicit relation between the two tonics in the M,m triad on A, consisting of superimposed minor 3rds which are the basic ingredientsof the two chief harmonies of resolution:
.
Melodic analysisof the first violin part from m. 47 to m. 67 would show very clearly that it is built around E at the beginning a member of the Cll:I and at the same time a dominant preparation for the ultimate destination of the line, A! : its first nine measures, in fact, are a prolongation
tona/ity 153
of e, after which it prolongs e3. Followingthis high point in a broader sense, a resumption of the e of mm. 41-46!, the line descends emphasizingBll, the C# leading-tone! in a quasi-dominant formulation from m. 60 to m. 63~. The descent of the line after this point, sequential, is a condensed,abrupt, reaffirmation of A, the note of its cadence,through an intriguing diversity of modal implications. The secondviolin part also requiresexamination in similar connections. It is, of course, conceived to a considerableextent as a parallel doubling of the first violin; much of it is a prolongation of the note B lending a modal quality to the tonic Gil!. Shortly before itsfinal resolution, on an emphasized A, there are reiterations of the upper leading-tone Bb, notated as All, in the repeated successionB-All!, and the lower leading-tone, Gil, has a single tentative appearance in m. 64, important nonetheless inits propinquity to the arrival point. The strongest aiiirmation of G# is in the cello, fundament of the harmonic structure, whose melodicanalysis wouldshow a prolongation of c# in the first several measures,then describing an octave descent to Gil, through Gil and Fil. With the cancellations of sharps except for the bass note of A:I° in its familiar form and context at m. 65! at mm. 64-67, the cello comes into agreement with the other voices in the shift which restores the primary tonic. We do not take space for the melodic analyses suggested, although the reader may wish to do so in confirmation of the tonal expressions ofthe individual lines in this passage;perhaps theconsequences of such analysis are,in general, preevident. The harmony is, of course, conditioned to a considerable extent, like the individual lines, by Bart6ks technique of mirroring the outer voices and filling the texture with the parallel 4ths which accompany them in the opening bars of the Lento assai.Nevertheless, harmonicfunctions, especially quasi-dominants andquasi-tonics, are of decisiveimportance in the expression of tonal feeling and coherence.The first harmony of m. 47 is a recurring Gil :I. Some ofthe auxiliary chords by which it is embellished,incorporating one or more leading-tones, actas quasi-dominants e. g., very potently at m. 50, third beat!; theseare markedin the sketch in Ex. 1-48e. The cadence of mm. 54-55 prolongs the I approached through a quasi-dominant in mm. 52-53! by alternating the root position with the first inversion. Elided
with this,the violinquintole figures describe triadic functions suggestive of A, alluding to the later restoration of that primary tonic. The sketch indicates essentialquasi-tonal functions in the modulatory section of mm. 56-67 Ex. l-48e!. The C|l:I is seen to persist in upper voices and there is a G# :V on G8 following. The Gil, as suggested before, has an ambivalence in the tonal dichotomy of this passageleading-tone ofA, dominant root of CB.! The cello reiteration of F it denotes a prolongation of harmony on Dil, coincident with the prolonged leading-
154 tona/ity Ex. 1-486. Cl; "v"
A="1"
mm.47-64 --.- - /E --. ..
:I "
' ,fl I-r
`"
v Y -_
`92 92~
_ inn.
-./' /_ " M- ""-""`il---~. lr____ "
_ if
92 -
l *__-:flags
92J l
|92!l. .f1V§.l lil! FSIC921.¢ ,' -_- I unix'--uqfuvn lT"'l7l11"lL_ 1».¢|u 1;-uf: 11 r qw _-¢/1.4-~_|u/-vi|. »r»_|_I1 nltniir1.-1 m|z.|.;i|1-r p aw?
-' cz
-_ _--_/'
Cf:l" prolongation, elaboration ---. .... _
`;92 »;_" l
|ig!=-Sllniiiér/ 1|~-C ll"' ¥ nn 1
V n
/-
ll' .
clwv
DL V » I"
mm.64-67 ____ iii"---~;92` *Ii
"* "-~---i1f;;~.
*_ ll-
.*'-`-`L=9292" `_--
2 "*--11. __
"3
__| "`---_i " """
A: "V"
Nl" V
V gf ||I||
tone of C# in the top voice; the All of this C#:II, as we have seen, later turns, asBb, toward the cadential A, so that the Dil triad also hassomething of an ambivalent function. The final cadential harmonies in the primary tonality are by now familiar, although in this instance the viola F of m. 66 makes of the A:I, a I-I-VI, the F anticipating the recurring quasidominant of
m. 66.
The developmental section which follows mm. 68-87! is of great interest tonally,and of considerable fluctuationand someambiguity, ultimately directed toward the SD level, D see mm. 84-85!. It should be regarded from two viewpoints. In the beginning of this section the two violins reiterate a motive derived from m. 47, except in m. 85 consistently a3rd apart. At first, the 3rds move twice to a D root, forecasting thePC of culmination at m. 85. The pattern is as shownin Ex. l-48f; the points designatedby accentare thosewhich are repeated or, in the case of the conclusion, stresseddynamically and otherwise prominent as highpoints in the riseof pitch. Note that the sketch notates theupper note of the penultimate interval, the only tritone, as Eb; it does indeedact asan upper leading-tone ofD in a harmonic complex which is a strong quasi-dominant consisting of the normal dominant root, A, and the upper leading-tone, Eb.! Also striking is the relation of adjacent interval pairs ascompared with that cy'the movement s two chiqf tonicsonorities. The secondstratum of tonal progression the two are initially in a kind of antiphonal relation, but come increasinglyinto confluenceas thedevelop-
tona/ity 155 Ex. 1-48f.
' ts / 1"'
Y
Y
Fi Q
[Y 47'
92,_..-"'f
J_
92__,vj Progression ofupper tessituratoward D tonicization culminating atm. 85
[.f.
1
ment progresses!is that of the viola and cello, in which the second violin also participates. This series ofharmonic successions begins mm. 69-70! with the familiar cadential formula in A, with its familiar resolution. The progression is then repeated fourtimes, with extension andvariation, deceptively moving to harmonic sonorities of changing tonal implications. The A: V by this time in the movement clearbf expects the A: I , realized only in the first succession of the series.The deceptiveresolutions createa senseof fluctuating tonal direction: each is a dissonance, each has one or more specific tonal implications seeEx. 1-48g; possibly theyare quasi-functional in C# and A!, but the sum total is one of considerable,uncertain fluctuation until the final clarification at
m. 85.
If the flats like those ofmm. 74 and 77! are heard enharmonically, the range of tonal fluctuation is lesswide than it seems.Against the precedents established earlierthe secondharmony of m. 74 is interpretable as Cl#:IV and that of m. 77 asC# :V. The sketch in Ex. 1-48g showsthat the second Ex. 1-489.
WT W» P 716 »...~»»» -. etc. f
FJ #Q
PQ
156 tona/ity
of these,the Ab/Gil harmony of m. 77, now recurs asconsistent penultimate
harmony inthe motivicrecurrences which followf. Whileit has the effectof recalling earlier quasi-dominants of A and Ci, iit resolves indecisively to deceptive dissonances until finally, abruptly on D. In the course of this the expansion ofthe motivic interval is of coursedeliberate and intensifying, and the tonal implications of its placementsare visible:with the extraction in Ex. 1-4-3g. Example 1-48his a sketch, like those seeneazrlier, intendedto represent schematically some of the observations of the albove discussion. The piano appearance ofthe motive in mm. 86-87, somewhat whimsical, tends to subvert any tonal decision achieved by the strong sauccession preceding it. It serves both to establish inconclusiveness of cadence and to recall in a subtle
inflection the primary tonic, at the same timeeasi_ly interpretablewithin the secondary tonalsystem onD, just aflirmed, neither of which is supportedin the measures which follow.
Ex. 1-48h. mm.68-88 un
_ ,, _--_ /` _-----~--_` ---__ ' `~ "_-----_ 4 .r |aw/ rf? 7 '1" v-3 /h.a's-|'92s»:Lf1qf 'I ,rg: ."» _ ."lui'_in,_/`°'°"8 . , -_____--'3`~._____-.A I I- =='ii _ ;» v V v Ir ' lI.~ ":" 'Y lI 9 ||:f.! ' .'T||..§|.§"..|'_ll. T ll ' f C7 V ||ll 1 ,ru-1.: gli' llff -dill' _IU if Ilmil. 1' fi r,' 4 I |w 'I 924 I *_* | ,, . 4, . fx ,` U r v ¢-92 M 'Q _ __ ____ '_ 'I ' H l'. h I " Is ' 'A ._ `:_ U Lil ' _ll H/ I 1 Z :fa =IY-1§{'!
ll: V
__ '
»= -I
~-_.______ _?_---
¢"
After A: V-l" in normal" succession, increasingly ` deceptive, fluctuant consequences A:V:of ambiguous. transitional tonal references linking A, D.
The Bartok movement approaches now asecond stagein developmental procedures whichwill lead back toclear reaflirmation of the primary tonality. The sourceof the materials developedafter m. 88 will be found in the movements first bars first violin takes the cello motive fof mm. 3-4 as its point of departure, asof coursedoes theviola; the opposite voices-second violin and cello-take for extension andvariation the top voiczeof mm. I5-I6 , which in turn relates to that of mm. ll-12, or the secondviolin of mm. 7-8! . The tonal Huctuation of the section followilng m. 88 begins with a strong allusion to F, F and C# symmetrically relate:d toA; it is a tonal reference in which both voices join.
tonality 157
Following the first few bars of this section Ex. l-48i! tonal meanings are again relatively ambiguous. The Bl»,E tritone so suggestive ofF in the above-mentioned extract gives way now to rapidly changing tritonal relations in which the lines do not concur!-each of them having certain tonal implications but none of these realized, as tonal allusion shifts from dissonance to dissonance. In the brief sketch in Ex. 1-48j mm. 92-97! the EX. 1-48i.
9 #jf-5 F. Ex.1-48j.
Q . l*
r1 T
git; rf-3-
indeterminate tritonal
fluctuations are
Fw !
1
ref-1
bracketed; the sketch also makes very
apparent the essential chromaticdescent ofthe bassfrom the BI; of the initial F reference down to F il, a point of brief prolongation. It must be acknowledgedin passing that the above-noted passagecan be broken down into more explicit tonal allusions if the individual lines are examined. For example, the top voice of mm. 93-94 expressing Eb! is sequentially related to that of mm. 89-90 expressingF!. But the difference is that in the caseof mm. 89-90 the lower voicesupported the reference toF; in mm. 93-94 the lower voice is more independent, although the Eb :VII is briefly formulated in the bass notesD, Cb, Ab at mm. 94-95. Another significant difference is in the fact that in the earlier passage the prolonged E leading-tone of F! moved down to C F: V!, while in the latter the El»,D succession moves to A in one of the tritonal movements!, tending to disrupt any senseof Eb which may have beenfleetingly conveyed. Again, the passages treated above havetonal implications which can be felt; that they constitutean interval of relativetonal flux and obscurity at the
158 tona/ity
same time is anecessary conclusion of analysis, revealing an important aspect of theidiom andthe form.What isevident here is theshaping opposition of relative ambiguitywith relativeclarity oftonal reference, a principleprobably applicable to all idioms of tonal music despite differencesin the means and extent of ambiguity.
In mm. 97-991 the lower voicetraces theleading-tone harmony of Cl# and theupper voice underscores that reference in an elaboration of thequasitonic harmony; here, C8 is a clear passingtonic in which the voices concur. It is broken up with the bass Gof m. l0l_ andthe tritonal leap from G# to D in the upper voice of the same bar. Either note could of course function
ornamentally inthe C8tonal system, but theysignal atonal departure at this point.
Measures lOl-ll.,in which the materialsare developed climactically, are thesort ofpassage sometimes described as pantonal, although tonal specificity andunanimity become relatively morepronounced inthe approaches to the cadence. Asthe climax develops thereare brief tonal allusions within
the linesbut thegeneral tonalef'l`ect largely is indeterminate. At onepoint a suggestion bitonality of arises, when thebass expresses a quasi-dominant, then tonic, formulationin imitation of a similar configuration in the upper voice expressing Gb. The tenselydissonant clash of tonalreferences Ex. 1-48k! is calculated to serve therising intensity. EX.1-QUIK.
. a- J
,
ii/J 3'
K,¢"'|
tg
The approach to the cadence atm. _lll, is also bitonal, the first violin persuasively aflirmative of AI; primary leading-tone ?!, the lower voices joining in quasi-dominants andquasi-tonics suggestive of B. At the cadence, interestingly, the upper voiceends with a quasi-dominantformulation in Al» , the lower voices statingthe first inversion ofthe B tonic. All of this isshown in the sketch l-48m! of mm. 104-lll . Finally, it should benoted that while the cadential successions into the B: I are underway the bassvoice is itself of extraordinary fidelity in the
tona/ity 159 Ex. 1-48:11. Concurrent, transitional references to A* and B
mm.l04-1 ll
L
/&l is
__ ll*
....
..___
__
expression ofthat harmony in a linear succession which includes a complexof leading-tones mm. 107-ll; see Ex. 1-48m!. Bartok has manifested inthe abovepassages the traditional tendency to establish in developmental areasa pattern of tonal fluctuation. Following m. ll4 the primary tonic is restored in harmonic and melodic functions of greater stability, recollective of those of the first part. The fluctuation into the primary tonal system retonicization of A! is brought about in a number of ways. The iirst violin part of mm. 112-13, the only voice occupiedwith significant melodic motive, is itself revealing, as indicated in
Ex. l-48n.
[;"".f 1 A: KW"
NI!!
At the same time, the harmony is a quasi-dominant m. ll3! which is an expectant preparation of the A of the cello. That harmony is built on B, which has throughout the movement beena vital factor in quasi-dominant functions within the primary tonal region. The V contains the upper and lower leading-tones the latter notated as Ab!, and it is anticipated in the first violin of the measure preceding.The D of the viola might be saidto have its premature resolution in the C-C# of the first violin motive. This quasi-
160 tona/ity
dominant see Ex. 1-480! is itself introduced by a harmony of semitonally related auxiliaries except for the bass,which is tied!-suggesting again the kind of linear determinant
of harmonic auxiliaries which
is so crucial in this
and other late tonal styles. TheV and its prior auxiliary harmonic complex are shown at the outset of the concluding sketch Ex. 1-480! of tonal functions and aH'iliations in the Bartok movement.
Exo 1'48o|
mm. 1 2}I I /:___`92
he IUX
rv -. 51;
on '
,- f
Ia `
~i
/
~+ F-
IN 1
l:P1I' il; Y '
fnIrt, : h
T
92» l -° A; ~v»-»» *i
|28 d 9¢ml¢°*| ¢8hb°"
::...~@-=.;.: =: l-92 92 Af. , l .921||_I"i1
/K
` i fl /I
__ -
__
_
us"
C|92°|l¢t¢fi9'-i¢_3¢i|i°fY °f
mm -en L --1 '_.."§¢:¢f '. f
wg 7/
'
_ I-
r
Underlying chromatk bass
successionl and linking 5
-_ , #I-_"~_
A
n~;';|~1.av192./ __:92.r :gil ",now Q _J r ,r92 aE |¢ | ~--' , 92_"Y'L|l`Ll'| ri.; l.| -¢ +" a na I* W 92` 1 II ;` -,_ ____ ____---" ' &/4 O 92 ,ir 92é4' 92.»r nv/vu? nv" "ln
V, -_ 92 » 92` 9292 ;,
The phrase which begins at m. 114, with the first violin imitating the cello abortively, provides in the unaccompaniedA the resolution of the preceding quasi-dominant. The diminished triad motive thereafter occurs at various levels: on A and All, on G and Gil a momentary sounding of A: VII!, on F and Fll _The tonal references arethus free and fluctuant but two factors are of particular importance: the ultimate quasi-tonic six-four over the bass dominant E, toward which the entire successionis directed, and the chromatic descentof the bass melody with two register shifts! from the ton-ic
A to
the dominant
E-the latter
sustained for
three bars.
Both factors are represented in Ex.l-480. The quartal Ighas considerable prolongation, embellished by quartal auxiliary chords again consisting of semitonally related neighbors i.e., leading-tones! in a rising sequence of pitches. The prolongation of the Ia traditionally a dominant auxiliary! 4
is a striking functional preparation forthe _final resolution. One of the embellishing auxiliary chords, an enharmonic notation of those preceding,is itself prolonged asa quasi-dominant-the harmony built
tona/ity 161
on the bass All at mm. 127-29; this harmony is embellished by auxiliary neighbors ofits upper members, but the bassAll remains constant. That All can again be understood as the upper leading-tone of the tonic A and, in accord with its notation, as the ambivalent lowerleading-tone tothe B prolonged withit, the B also resolving to A mm. l28-29 and l3l in Hrst violin, and mm. 130-32 in cello! in a functional recession whichhas beenrecurrent
Ex. 1-48p!. The harmony is, likethe preceding I%, fullyquartal in construction. Ex.1-48p. /~, 'T'
Ut * EK. 9292 9292'
r
. #»'9292. 9292
In a quartal harmony the identification of root is problematic; the analysis suggested here givestonal, functional precedence tothe bassnote of the harmony on A# as doesany reasonabletonal assessmentof the tradi-
tional Ig!. The harmony occurring over All canbe construed as quasi-dominant twice removed V/V/V!, introducing the V/V on B, the All acting in its ultimate course as an upper leading-tone to A see Exx. l-480, p, q!. The cadence is consummated by the motivic quasi-dominant tritonal sonority F, B! and the quasi-tonic harmony A, C!, preceded bya dissonantanticipation of I combining C with C# in characteristic modal ambivalence. The entire succession might be conceived asanalogous tothe following traditional sequence oftonal harmonies, a hypothesis best examined in actual, attentive listening toBartoks cadence in comparison with the conjectured prototype, which is reprinted in Ex. 1-48q in explicit demonstration of the significance of the concept of quasi-functional tonal harmony. Ex. 1-48q. _
=, __.n
=*" BW
gg
92 !|V 92_2
Cf. -Q-!
Q it _ v/V/V
~"~ Q
lfgvg Eg
ew
,,.
;
'Q
V/V v
B
= _
162 tonality
While the cadential formulation is underway there is a most important manifestation of tonal expression in the first and second violin parts. The earlier prolonged B in the former, with its A# auxiliary embellishment, now descends toA az!, after which the line falls two octaves intoa cadential resolution on a. The second violin,moving in parallel 3rdsdown to G, a modal auxiliary, underscores the arrival on the tonic in a concurring resolution. The primary tonic is now well reestablished the ; restis its prolongation for conclusive finality. The sketch in Ex. 1.480 shows the motive of rising 3rds, only mildly evasive in tonal allusion, subtly animating that concluding tonic elaboration. These motivic iterations supply A: V once m. 132! and otherwise recall the secondary tonicsC# and D, as well as the B of the cadential function just realized. The motive occurs onceagain, on Eb, in a slight stir of tonal evasiveness immediatelypreceding the final two tonic statements, this time lacking the quasi-dominant tritone. Further enhancing the sense of tonal resolution is a melodic tonal reference-that of the first violin in its phraseat m. 135 ,again beginning on Gil and ending on Bl, compare mm. ll-12 or 23-27!. We have sought in the above discussions toexplain the overall and specific directions of Bartoks tonal structure in this movement, identifying tonal functions in melodic and harmonic extracts, and pointing out areas of relative tonal stability and relative activity as delineating structural factors and giving analysis ofthe meansby which tonal referenceand tonal fluctuation are carried out. The discussion is not of course exhaustive, and it is confined to factors
of tonal structure and technique. A tonal .gzstem of the sort described and at times charted with reference to earlier works could of course be derived and
graphically represented: it would reveal the remarkable extent to which tonicized PCS by which the system is expanded are diatonicalbf related loA. Webern, Four Pieces, Op. 7, for violin and piano, No. 1
An important example from the works of Webern is given next as Ex. 1-4-9a.It is the first of the pieces for violin and piano, Op. 7. The tonal structure isof truly compelling effect.There can be no insistent suggestionof consciouscreative intent, although Webern is said to have afiirmed the importance of tonal relations in his works; the tendencies which seem unequivocalin this piece and it is representative ofmany! may well be intuitive. The important concern of analysis isexperiential efzct as plausibly described and underscored in objective evidence. Such evidence of course requires interpretation, and verification in experience i. e., listening!, with such experienceconditioned by the understandingwhich follows from analysis.
tona/ity 1
63
Ex. 1-49a. Webern, Four Pieces forviolin andpiano, Op.7; No. 1.
Sehr langsamIkea 50! mit "1 55:52 fr
espress.
Diimpfer /'92 1
uvlzgqr ,o PP e
84 PP
e er
:E _ ll..-. 92u'|/
:1:=, B..
9292lV '
I, 1-
-I l-
i l-
HPh_ H
IQYHD ll X1 l' --
:~
I 9| .Ln .r * `E Q _ r 1l-2|l 9Z
n' Ei
[1
IM
Y ll
HP-°""'1' T
m°4 f92 1I
:uZ°¢§hg;e!zogen -__-_-_-_-_-_
,° I ==5i¢=I¥ii'-|¬l§3iE;|-1L~--§=»=HJfv I1
-_ -_
'; ;"=""
-_ 1-
Imp-¢mPf`¢ dusserst
gart
| 92 IL! gf: 9292|7§
q
'`
h -_' _Z
:sir f
-57
4-4;
4f*" `
i
I __
:_..l= `
`
7
#
. HP
_7 rit. m "J-'I_.'1 Halt-'Y -_
N*
.. If 1| 0 110;
°6
_D 1'W-_ __ -_ lj
---- -
Ii._* _ V
'_ l'l -=-I' lfl
a -H!
--
75
92
1/.;1_.i1»1 1' Q 92 ml 1-
~ ° » H-92 ee mv
fifl! 7
_U
li,
--
pg.. m
--
*
7 3 f"§ PP;_.:=--
Copyright 1922, Universal Edition. Used hy permission the of publisher. Theodore Presser Com pany sole representative United States, Canada, and Mexico.
164 tona/ity Three sketches will illuminate
tonal functions
of different
kinds, all of
them oriented toward a primary tonic Eb. The tonal system of the piece is simple suggestive secondarytonicizations of the leading-tone D could be inferred, but the primary Eb is virtually without enrichment of expanding secondary systems in this very brief, concentrated work!. Indeed, there is no significant fluctuation: no real deviation from insistent referencesto Eb at a number of levels.
The first sketch Ex. l-49b! shows the descentfrom structural manifestations of eb to eb and finally to el», root of the final chord. The first of these pitchesis simply held asa violin harmonic, establishing the tonic PC. The second is reiterated, and preceded by a succession derivedfrom a chromatic set illustrated! which signyicantbf avoids Eb. The repeated figure containing the eb is released, significantly,on eil and d2,creating an intense expectancy ofreturn of the establishedEb. Ex. 1-49b. Tonal tendencies in the Webernupper stratum. chromatic set
lacking El'
J
F_ Qi/
Q 9292 9292 _,
92 .
At what might be described asan internal stratum in the pieces spatial field, a quasi-functional harmonic recession takesplace, as shown in Ex. l-49c. A further, implied step in this succession,expressed asa melodic configuration, might well be included; it is representedin the third sketch, Ex. l-49d. When onehears thissuccession its functional implications are very persuasive. Thetwo neighbors of the V are shown by the symbol N ; one of these is stated in two
registers, and
is embellished
by its
own lower
tona/ity 165 Ex. 1-49c. Quasi-functional harmonic successions in theWebern. NN --+` 9292 i
Zi
N rr
,--f i; 92 92 /
re;
§ ""
''''
n - dissonant! re In /
neighbor asindicated in Ex. l-49c. A dissonantform of I precedes the final resolution. Throughout the succession,the importance of voice-leading, its smoothness aresult of linear adjacencies,is apparent. Finally, an ascent inthe lowertextural stratum Ex. l-49d! complements and is a counterpoint to the descent notedearlier Ex. 1-49b!. Again, Eb isits center; the succession is Eb, eb, e|». The harmonic succession noted above is
included parenthetically,and tendenciesof leading-tonesand otherauxiliaries are indicated by arrows. Particularly effective in function is the penultimate event, Fb Ell!, which functions like the d2 of the violin, shown in the Hrst sketch! as a leading-tone expecting, and directly antecedent to, the final tonic root.
Ex. 1-49d. Further especially lower stratum! tonalexpression in the Webern. J
7F
_=
c!
92 lm
` ||| -._ m ,
, ,J '92 7'
. lg Z$ mu it
l ll nn i- ,'
IZY' -.r'_!j-' _ A I' _'__|'l| / 'I i1 »' ' `/
_______ ___ Q" W,
'
'
___I !
I| *If
E ¢`___, |l _
A summarysynoptic sketch,superimposing andincorporating principal factors representedin these three, would project a comprehensive imageof tonal tendencies and relations in the piece as a whole.
166 tona/ity Berg. Four Pieces. Op. 5. for clarinet and piano. No. 4
The works of Berg, strong and resourceful in their expressionsof tonality, within both serialand preserialprocedures, aregreatly rewarding in study of this kind. The following discussionrefers tothe Four Pieces forclarinet and piano, Op. 5. The reader would do well to study others of these piecesas to manifestations of tonal order; we refer here to No. 4, in C. In this piece, expressions ofC are of numerous kinds. The opening C basspedal is of courseitself highly suggestive andpreconditioning, especiallyas theroot of a major triad to which is added two pitches that might be construed aschromatic auxiliaries of the G and E of the triad!. During the statement of this reiterated harmony consistently of the
duration of 3,f!! theclarinet sounds two motives one the extensionthe of other! which tend to encircle the C tonic, one of the motives beginning on B, the other on Cll. This principle of encirclement is made extremely compelling and explicit in the return of the chords atmm. 1 1-12 Ex. l-5Oa!. Ex. 1-50a. Berg, Four Pieces for clarinetand piano,Op. 5;No. 4 clarinet notated at sounding pitch!. Langsam
l'|l.ll -/92- if; _,Lf
__
h92|~ , e HIP ;"'="* Echoton. _ _ _ _ '_ _ -Ig; _
4I
3 Q | lla
PP '
-'H-.-, .f|.¢.»..5_..1 »- -1-_ niiallfi |'= -T? = _:vm I r rl|51 -1 -i W; 3V
= v= E
I4
ll 1'
g1!
..; il
HP 7 -'~ PPM ,.-92I"j_ ,___,3
4; il
__ __
` =qu|192l ' 92./V 2 ,lg ,__ marc.
3 .f
Illfl =_" lm! = nt 92llZ»..l;P1l| Er' -"TES _ 2921|| 17 PH _ Z! I _ - _ pil r-5% fisll l'3i92 r-3-92 UNL! ' §"|;.r ' si- `
=i .`£
__-` i-
ll
Copyright 1924, Universal Edition. Used by permission the of publisher. Theodore Presser Company, sole representative United States, Canada and Mexico.
In the approach to this reafiirmation of C, there isa strongly suggestive and functional succession whichin some ways expects C and at the same time powerfully manifests a quasi-dominant sonority in the piano in mm. 9-10. The piano harmony consists oftheconventional Gand F with the upper
tona/ity 167
and lower chromatic auxiliariesof C-all of this symmetrically disposed around c2, which appears only fleetingly in the clarinet and is otherwise deliberately withheld, intensifying the expectation of the C resolution of
m. ll and its anticipation inthe clarinet,as ca,in m. IO. SeeEx. 1-50b.! Of major interest in this passage is themotion of the clarinet line toward
ci. In m. 8, the clarinetbegins thisprocess withtremolo repetitionof two pitches: b|»and g. Once theline moves,it doesso with insistent implications ofbileveled compound! structure functionally directed towardca, as summarized in Ex. l-50b..The lower level of the compound succession moves
toward cz,as does the contraction in thepiano, whose chromatic neighbors of C are transferred unresolvedto the clarinet in m. ll, at a lower octave. Of Ex. 1-50b. Berg, Four Pieces for clarinetand piano,No. 4. Mm. 8-10and sketchof recessive actions towardC. /
1 S.--My user of .» !! |1..=e!._ _L_-;-__ 'u n_H §7'l| 'I 7 ,,
voice|Eparat1on: I I wg,=,f-c/ *Prominent asV preparation m.B ,/-""'"i » 92
?TF==§- i 92 »
7; J
and auxiliaryin concludingmeasures. '. __ EEEQEEEEEEEEEEEBEEEEE-EEE=E==3-|
M
3 "sr;r;r::.1r;s:::;=r: -""" ""
/'j""§-_ . __3.7¬"f Z,:== "ii wi 7e /""
3_
3 3_e
.
iI
if "E_ . 'I' u' ' u'_ ;"%3 _ ll m_9 3
..-...:-..=
N: P "'_?°'% 92
kurzleicht und gestossenW ..
ii
°°
° gl=°° iE;|;;;| si-'
Y.
-5
gf/ "" /`92
°
°
i'
i 92
1;-3 iz*
_r Jf-
_L
3- n' `
_ /T"
. A ='- g ;; F_ g¥ Ja i I. P-s-Y wi :f _
92_i.¢_-4 ij-I
,r
»' ::: :::
3 -4
-'*
r-'Ffr!.f=:fzH_L!'_l!'_ ge ::, , -'.___._;~ 1i
I 192:r=`
'iiE§i§§§`§§§`i§E§ii_ii -L ', _, eg/
168 tona/ity Ex. 1-50b continued. mio f*--
,
--
--
° ./"
--
--
H' /°
-
"
EEE! QW/El! V Timmy? lf!
=e = . .= § ?»-s ° f .
:.s»;I; :;l ~: ; -- _ g ___ if' 595*-lL-r:2r1'¢rJ»;E::::.L;E::;' -»/'I*gi ::!::-: Ll.: .
5' 2
- in
Copyright 1924, Universal Edition. Used by permission the of publisher. Theodore Presser Company, sole representative United States, Canada and Mexico. Summary ofpiano contractionin mm. 9-10:
course, thereis tentative resolution in the arrivals on c insistently repeated in slowing tempo in the clarinets upper stratum!, then C in the bass ofthe piano; the absent c' does appearas root of the pieces linal sonority. Other factorsof quasi-functionalsupport of the primary C can be noted briefly. For example, thefinal recessionof tonal structure toward C involves an underlying chromatic ascent of the clarinet toward ga e-f 1-f li-ga! in a progression bywhich that dominant is projected with great emphasis.The progression isopposed toa piano descent toCC in mm. 13-17, after which the C triad with B! is first outlined in the piano bass, thensustained in a chord of piano harmonics and, subsequently, repeatedin unstopped notes over c. The function of the pianos gradually accrued penultimate harmony echoed in the clarinets final phrase! as a complex of leading-tone auxiliaries resolving to the final tonic is apparent in the sketch of PC content in the two harmonies Ex. l-50c!. Ex.1-50c.
J?
is o
Cl3.l'.*i ee
92J%
----; -/
5
gé-";£' I//'Pdf BQEZ/,
, ?/ Q! In
tona/ity 169 Summary notes on the un/'versa/ity and s/gn/Hoance of the pr/nc/p/e of h/erarchic Iona/ order
Any analysis of liturgical chant demonstrates,like that of Ex. l-41, that a critical factor in its structure is the quasi-tonal hierarchy which yields, at points of cadence, to preestablished componentsof the modal scale as elements of relative finality, the ultimate being the final itself, which is in this sense unmistakablyanalogous to the tonic of later, formalized tonal functions. Moreover, we have seen thatin modal polyphony groupsof auxiliaries tend to revolve around notes most basic in the hierarchic, modal or quasimodal structure, and this too mustbe recognizedas analogousto the systematized tonal functions of later styles. We therefore take the position that modality whether pure or in later evolutionary stagesis a manifestation of the fundamental principles essentialto tonal organization in music. Indeed, in much modal music the later specificconventions oftonality are in vivid evidence. And while the transition from purely modal to emergent tonaldevices isgradual-and overlapping among different coexistent styles, it is clear that the conventions of the tonal period are incipient and
sometimes very significantly manifest in applications which long predate their formalization in Baroque and Classicalliteratures, and that, in modality, the concept ofhierarchic tonalorder is trubf relevant.
Along with the wider applications of leading-tone functionsby the uses of musicajictain contexts of modal derivatives, onemust regard as indicative of later tonal conventionsthe increasinglysupportive role of the bass inearly multivoiced compositions--a role characterizedmore and more by the useof 4ths and 5ths of quasi-dominant-tonic succession,less andless bythe intervallic and rhythmic equality between bass and other voices which is a Renaissance ideal.
An element in this trend is, of course, the bass line ac-
companying sixteenth-centuryvocal declamation in, for example, the practices of lutenist composers;its function is substantially that of defining root progressions ofclear tonal purpose, asultimately is that of the figured bass of the Baroque, an important embodiment of harmonic conventions in the tonal period. It is primarily the practice of musicajicta, ofcourse, with the increasing emergence ofleading-tone relations,which contributed in the Renaissance to "Of _]osquin,Donald Groutwrites thatwe becomeaware ofa consistent organization of theharmonies along the linesof ourown commonpractice. Thoughthey arestill rootedin the modalsystem, the chord progressions and thegeneral harmonic plan in most of_]osquins works are to a large extent governed by dominant-tonic relationships.One sign of this organization isthe conductof the bass line:more andmore it has begunto bedistinguished from theprimarily melodicnature ofthe othervoices andhas begunto assumethe function of a harmonic support;consequently itoften movesby fourthsand fifths.This kind of harmonic organizationand this type of bass movement become increasingly prevalent in the course ofthe sixteenthcentury. [A History ofWestern Music New York: W. W. Norton and Company, Inc.,1960!, pp.243-44-.]
170 tonality
the gradual dissolution ofpurely modal conventions, especiallyin polyphonic music, and to the increasing expansionof tonal systems and consolidation of the conventions ofthe tonal period. At times densechromaticism is practiced," constituting one of the most intriguing sixteenth-century stylistic currents, to a degreethat eventuatesin a great flux of tonal reference Lowinskys floating tonality!, presumably as a conscious expressiveresource. It is noteworthy that most highly chromatic passagesare homorhythmic in texture, to
facilitate and
focus attention
on chromatic
successions without
competing textural complications. In other than chromatic styles, too, the increasing use of simpler textures, often in modified polyphonic contexts, is another manifestation of the growing emergenceof harmonic formulae of the major-minor system. It is easy tounderstand that more homorhythmic textures, linkedwith a bass line less andless independent,increasingly given to defining codified successions in relation to a central tonic function by movement of4ths and 5ths, would be associated with advanced trends in the rise of major-minor tonality. Moreover, one of the fundamental differencesbetween thepolyphony of the Renaissance andthat of the Baroque an issueof style,ofcourse, not of value! is in the latters firm conditioning of individual lines toward conventional tonal ends and of their confiuence in the Baroque to serve prescribedtonalharmonic functions.
In many transitional works the blends of modality and major-minor tonality are extraordinarily provocative and often of great charm. Where this occurs, modal vestigesshould not be regarded as inhibiting tonal order although many do in the specificsof major-minor terms!, but as conditioning the mean.;by which tonal order is expressed. In the Baroque, the use ofold melodies-chant, Lied, chorale,etc.-as cantusjirmus in polyphonic compositionsor assubjects forvariation in compositions of highly provocative ambiguities markedly in phrygian derivatives! exacerbated the modality-tonality ambivalences and conflicts, the melody imposing upon evolving harmonic conventions of the rising major-minor system vestigial inflections of the past, the harmony increasingly pointed toward the later conventionsof the tonal period. Music theory awaits a deeper understanding of this evolutionary processin, for example, seventeenthcentury music; at the same time analysis attempts to determine structural consequences of tonal referencein theindividual work,and seeksto understand how tonality, broadly conceived,functions counteractivelyand complementarily in its relations with other elements in the musical work. Historical factors of ambivalence and evolution concern the devicesof tonal aflirmation 3 not the fact of tonality itself See Lowinsky, Tonaligy and Atonaliy, for analytical discussionof music of Lasso, Gesualdo, andother sixteenth-century chromaticists.
tonality 171
The technique of juxtaposing tonally fluctuant areas ina musical form with areas of relative stability or ambiguity with clarity!, while dating from very early practices, isfundamental to larger forms of the tonal period. It is of particular importance in the late nineteenth centuryand in the tonal music of the twentieth century, where fluctuation may be over an extremely wide range. We have noted that in those twentieth-century styles in which the identity of tonic is less explicit, the expressionof relativerepose and relative mobility of tonal allusion is a particularly vital, even necessary,and often clearly felt, aspect ofmusical structure and structural function. When one or more of the essentials of dominant function are retained in a succession, the sense of that function, as we have seen, with the implica-
tion of affirmation of tonic, is likely to be preservedin some degree-whether significantly must, again, be in part a matter of subjective response and judgment. Of importance in such proceduresis the expansion ofthe concept of leading-tone: with the important technique of tonicizing encirclement, especially potentwhen the encirclement is by chromatic auxiliaries, the upper leading-tonetakes onan importancecomparable tothat of the traditional
lower asobserved, for example, inthe Bartokand Berganalyses!. Such leading-tones act in encirclement of tonic factors at disparate levels, and in encirclement of the individual factors ofquasi-tonic harmony, in practices of voice-leading involving frequently quasi-dominant! harmonic derivatives comprised of semitonal neighborsto harmonic structures of resolution. The extent to which such functionsdenote tonal reference depends,of course, on their relative strength as experienced in a particular context, as established by rhythmic, metric, dynamic, and other means, as well as on the supportive or contradictory terms ofthe surrounding particular context.It is frequently possiblein textures ofconsiderable complexityand freechromaticism to establishtonal feelingby suchapplications ofsubtle inferencesof traditional functions, as we have seen in quotations from relatively recent works.' The question of when tonality is expressed andfunctional! and when not-i.e., that of atonality or nontonality!, is among many that are probably incapable of insistent verdict. Some theoristsinsist that any configuration or concurrence ofPCs conveyssome inevitabletonal sense,especially in cadential actions, while others consider tonal coherence obliterated
in contexts of
relatively mild complications. Still others regard any consistent application of a twelve-tone setas akind of tonality. Perhaps the truth is that examples of altered, diminished, possibly extinguished reflections of tonality in recent musicare subjectto classificationinto a number of levels ofsignificance of tonal effect and function Fig. l-7!. The tonality-atonality question arises particularly with respect to serial music. Tonic function can of course be obscured by contradictory events, but it is clear that serialized associations of PC materials cannot,
on
the basis of the prescriptive technique of composition in and of itself, be
1 72
tonality
Fig. 1-7. Conjectural set ofclassifications of levels ofsignificance oftonal function. Absolute atonality? Atonality as a relative tendency? Irrelevant tonality? Multitonality? Pantonality ? Tonal flux extinguishing, or severely attenuating, tonal function? Tonal flux within broad, prevailing tonal unity Extended expanded! tonality Tonality of quasi-functional manifestations Conventional major-minor! tonality Tonality of ambivalent conventions Tonality of modal conventions Purely melodic tonality Primitive pedal! tonality
pronounced necessarilyexternal or
irrelevant to
the experienceof tonal-
ity. In connection
with his discussion of cadential functions
and other fac-
tors which can be interpreted to point to E as a tonal factor in Schoenbergs Klavierstzick, Op.33a, Edward Cone makes the following observations, of significance forour considerationof the relevance oftonal allusion and tonal analysis! in certain twentieth-century styles: More controversial is the attempt to find tracesof tonal form in avowedly atonal compositions; yet I do not see how music like Schoenbergs, with its usually clear cadential structure, can fail to arousecertain traditional associations and responses. To the charge of irrelevancy, I answer that one who cannot indeed hear such cadentialphenomena in this music must judge the analysisto be prescriptive and inapplicable. But one who does hearthem must admit to that extent the validity of the approach. He may counter that one ought not to hear the music in this way; but he is then criticizing the music, not the analytical method. Unwanted cadential effectswould be as great a Haw in atonal music as thechance appearanceofa human Figure ina nonrepresentationalpainting."
We would embrace Conesconcept ofrelevance as vital and necessary in analytical discussions of tonality. In serial works,tonal manifestationsmay well appear as often in Schoenberg, for example! in the light of techniques already much discussedin examples of preceding pages. In others, atonality or irrelevant tonality may indeed be found to pertain. But hierarchic tonal order may be implicit too in a twelve-tone set itself see the following discussion ofthe Schoenbergsong, Tot! ; somespecific twelveEdward T. Cone, AnalysisToday, in The Musical Quarterly, XLVI,2 960!, p. 185.
tonality 173
tone setsof obvious,potentially functionaltonal possibilities are quotedin Ex. 1-51.
Ex. 1-51. Twelve-tone sets:Berg, ViolinConcerto; LuigiNono, CantoSospeso forsoprano, alto, andtenor soloists,mixed choir,and orchestra.
_:_ #,_ #4 li!
Berg: b. #Il/':/IT Lf.! _ - g_ f/;»__/
Nono: JKT-C g. CI lg h #I . T glrBerg ViolinConcerto copyright I 936,Universal Edition. Used bypermission of the publisher. Theodore Presser Company, sole representative United States, Canada and Mexico. Nono Canto Sospeso set reprinted by permission qf ArsViva Verlag.
The tonalimplications of the Nonoset areespecially evident when asin No. 6b,Corn éduro dire addio! thenote Ais stronglyestablished at beginning and end as a referential PC. The opening of Part 6b has the tenor voice sustaining boecachiusa! a' for two bars, after which the upper and lower leading-tones notes 2 and 3 of the set! enter. The final cadence of the section takesall twelve notes mm. 407-1 l! in order from 1 to 12, then returns the first note, now a, in the first violin. There is, it seems, afirm sense of tonal resolution.
In the song, Tot,by Schoenberg,a tonic element _ is suPPorted b Y
two leadin
formin E-tones 8
°a
u
.the C1 uasi-dominant com P
lex
_ 5 , . _ Schoenberg thus expresses a tonal structure means by that are free ofstrict tonal conventions, yet insistent. Examples 1-52a and 1-52b show the composers applications ofthe abovequasi-tonal functions. In the excerpts, some suggested primary and secondary tonal implications are indicated. While there are comparable tonal functions at other levels, expressed both melodically and harmonically, those notedare the most vital and pervasive.
In Schoenbergs song, thetwelve-tone set is sodisposed that when the voice partis basedupon theF ,Cquasi-tonic PCC and itsleading-tones, the piano is not, and vice versa.The entire song isan excellentexample ofquasi-
tona/ity 175 m.I I_
EI i3 der fand W ,___
E"
sein Gliick __....___ , __
:_*_ * °' _im 7 ,V , .: q° 7L. L 0 " 13- vlrlf1 1 lr 1 lg- gm
|.111vi g unti 92 '$I 7_
I' _*f _ I_;| i 1 _----zlzj-i_l_|l:¢;».
und ich _;_____i.
7 7 ; ,` I ll L in| l» | IHTILJQIIY I § _ ° I nn ' °I lu.
1- Q ~ B ll-Z1lT_Q-l_Il'|I"'1"1' -- -lr' -l_r11», 1.l|.4.;.4.4 |
?. . .. nr in IFY' _? . _--__I'1-Il I l..J1_1_-'i_l__l1l Y.Q'1 -2-Il-fi1]l`__l"Y_-Q-TQ 'nv lA.4 l1l!1_11 |r..¢ -1 | | in IliI-rrL;I |_LF |,9'u'|.,, s 5 ll1 .l1l'Ht_;l ¢I ; I unit ___ fi V f" h
P
7I
I
7h
;iif I . ...v-; . _
r .,
m.l3 _fl-i Ili
-
-v
nxt ..-_ .1
II
, fand
-
II
l
_ lteins.
In _,lit ll
II . adIll' lil l r.1w|;'-Hpwdwiwuls nxt §LJ.|.JA-I-ll-Q qi ,_ V1 1-|1_!1l 5 r|;:». --- '
_~
Z li! C11 nitllmi nuullilitm 31 P ' 1ii.1| _.1 I '0 A l-` 1/ 'R 'IA -'llldvir iltlli if II l1 'r¬1.ll IPI it Il *lil-|_illl §l||_ll_'I¬_l'I'.l1'_Ql'l..l'1 lr.; li|,u"11l9292.l1I;l$,[1il-1/ | uv;If'_.Y-» ltlliiulli"1l_ lb l~ni'1-1-titlil i ..¢»..|.11 l1l~_l92921l Lili-r_1.1l|192 Hill-:ln
IL
»'
|I
W' I1-Z r11!i' ~ '
,F |
I' ffl;
is I*
._ V.
|1
n»
II I|
|I
Reprinted permission by Associated qf Music Publishers, Inc.
tonal functions in a twelve-tone work, and the final cadence_significantly realizes tonalexpectations, yetis imperfect. Tonal orientations, which of course would not require realization in actual applications, are potential in the set itseltf Schoenberg exploitsthese particular possibilities it will be noted that, for example, thefinal three PCs of the set could have been applied to tonicize FI# as well! in deliberate expression ofthe primary tonic F and secondary tonic D, in much use of Ex. 1-52b. Twelve-toneset forthe Schoenberg song, "Tot,"with potentialquasi-tonal affiliations suggested.
Note that the right hand of the pianopart in the final two bars,taking thesecond tetrachord oftherow in retrograde orderG, Ab,E, Bb-8, 7, 6,5!, comesout onthe lowBb as finalnote, lendinga functionalambivalence tothe F which has been sopersistent and nearly constanta tonic center of reference. Atthe sametime, theleading-tones FII,B and the tritoneE,Bl» have in relation to the F ,Csonority stronglytonicizing effect.
176 tonality
relevant tetrachordal segments, andin avoidance, although the set isused in retrograde, qf any transpositions. The second tetrachord has the potential of leaning, dominant-like, toward F, a potential exploited as shown in the quoted excerpts Ex. l-52a!. Tonal referencesmay thus, more or less significantly,follow from certain specific properties of the twelve-toneset andits applications, when compositional procedures submit to the implications of such properties: the set fully or partially disposed around a particular axis or axes which, symmetrically applied, may become spatially central to the pitch content as at times in Webern; see Chapter 2, pp. 249-53!; intervallic relations implicit or explicit in the set such that potential is established for quasifunctional relations dominants, leading-tones, etc.! supportive of a recurrent PC as tonic ; such thematic usesof the set asformal recapitulations or other recurrencesin which a particular variant and transposition of the set takes onquasi-tonal significancein terms of recurrent thematic direction and orientation e.g., Webern, Op. 22!; and the application of the twelve-tone set
such that
invariant functional
relations recur-relations
grouping together in disparate set formsinvariant adjaccncies of two, three, or more PCs having structural predominance; and, of course, rhythmic and other applications in any way lending PC primacy and, conceivably, hierarchic arrangementcentered in that primacy! to a particular element of the set, or elements of the set.
One of the problems in tonal analysis of music in which tonal centers are supported only ambiguously is of course the extent to which analysis should goin the pursuit of and explanationof tonal allusions thatare suspected seep. l7lf.!. Any music, reduced toits most microcosmic units,obviously has tonal references-i.e., if one considersa work interval by interval nearly any analyst will find in each the experienceof one tonic root! or another. Any work, then, can theoretically be regarded asconsisting of the constant fluctuation of such tonics so that fluctuant tonality is presentin every work, even the most atonal.
The termparztonal can refer toa musicalsituation ofthe kindsuggested above, in which a kind of all-tonality prevails, a constant shifting of tonal reference within the smallest units, and a free combination among textural strata of diverse tonal implications, creating what is analogous to the whiteness ofthecombination of all colors. In the extreme, sucha pantonality must be a neutralization Qf tonaligg theextreme spreadof tonal reference causinga blurring to the point of extinguishing the effect of any single tonal implication. Pantonality is a concept suggestinga particular perspective forregarding what are also described asatonal situations. This conceptmay be pertinent to Ex. 1-53, a much-quotedexample, in each microcosm of which a tonal center could be said to be implied, but
the
tona//ty 177 Ex. 1-53. Webern, SixBagatelles for string quartet,Op. 9,first movement. D? G?
_,m l -/T Hal" H2
Dlw? F?
l?
k gl!
Elv?
r 92q i / i c /I _ §__./
, sord. 11! I 5:1 ' lui I
3-1 g
,'
v7
am Steg ___. 1__.. PP sord. §_*1'
I LJ_ |2111 -Ill » I ¢l'.L~.';"=:.
L
.
I/r 2
._/
1,
,
3. e
i S am tes """ """
»
..
RP ~-92
We '
'
..
.l
.
L: w rd.9292 = / i 3 $92 iq i 9292 1 .9292 _
UQZ 92 92 _S
g
.i
-,
Eli?
- ge - ,_ A?
0
`/e
D? F ?
Set: Y
* N*
L Q-'Lf
I
V'
Copyright 1924, Universal Edition. Used by permission the Q" publisher. Theodore Presser Company, sole representative United States, Canada and Mexico.
sum of which is so fluctuant as to be a neutralization of tonal feeling. The individual tonal allusions suggestedare inherent in Weberns row, which is included in the example. Whatever results are realized in particular analyses, and these are, again, often in some degree subjectivelydetermined, there is certainly music of the twentieth century in which the aim of the composer isto resist tonal leanings andto achievewhat Pousseurcalls a distributive equilibrium-an equilibrium which is directly opposed tothat of tonality becauseits specific harmonic weight is equally distributed at all points with a resultant harmonic homogeneity and mobility which, rather than the thematic rigidity of the seriesof Schoenberg, are the function of the twelve-tone series.66 These words seem to state very well the intent and eH`ect ofatonality. Henri Pousseur, The Question of Order in New Music, in PerspectivesNew of Music, V, 1 966!, pp. 107-8; translation byDavid Behrman.
178 tonality
Almost since music began there have been, in given style contexts, complexes ofpitches in linear and vertical arrangements by which listeners are conditioned to expect certain responsive succeeding events.These are sometimes conduciveto a range of expectations, sometimesvery specific in
their implications:
It is thus virtually impossible to exaggerate thesignificance of tonality in the structure of Western music.
Tonal reference
and fluctuation
are the
chief pillars upon which the standardforms of the eighteenthand nineteenth centuries rest, and we have seen that music of the Western tradition
before
and after what is described asthe tonal period is very often dependent on orientation of musical events in relation to a tonic toward which melodic and
harmonic elements perhaps quasi-functionalharmonies! aredirected. In the vast majority of works of this monumental tradition the languageof music is substantially that of dissonancein which cadential andother formal articulative functions are characterizedby fulfillment, nonfulfillment, or limited fulfillment of expectations arousedand conditioned by prior assertion ofa tonic and its structural, hierarchic system. Foregoing exampleshave demonstrated that many twentieth-century composers havesought to achieve new ways of establishing tonal feeling, without altogether dismissing the idea of order around a tonal center, or complex of centers, or tonal axis, with tonal homogeneity a fundamental source ofunity and tonal contrast of variety, or with relative stability of tonic opposed in formal delineations to its controlled fluctuation and, at times, relative obscurity.
While the many examplescited in this chapter, diverse asthey are in style and historical orientation, give testimony to the vast importance of tonality in the determination of musical structure, our chief purpose has been to show how in various idioms tonality is establishedand made to Huctuate, and to demonstrate techniques the in anabfsis qf tonaliyfwithin awide gamut qf genres and styles. Theanalytical techniques incorporated in this study are of an intentionally heterodox scope;their aim is the discovery oftonal structure, range, andsystem pertinentto the individual work or at times morebroadly to classes of works!, and the specialmeans bywhich tonal structure isexpanded and modulated in accord with expressive needs. When tonality is abandoned,such factorsas therecurrence and variation of melodic-rhythmic theme become basic,as do the shaping effects of rhythmic elements, dynamic contrasts, and functional contrasts of texture. The extent to which tonality is resisted or dismissed in music is thus normally the extent to which other elements-color, texture, rhythm, and
tona/ity 179
others-have ascendant significance, with the controlled distribution of contrasts and aflinities among these elements, and among their individual manifestations inprogressive andrecessive operations, determining structure.
Conc/ud/'ng notes
The foregoing analyses dealwith the question of tonal andlinear functionsin melodic and harmonic contexts in which tonality is of essentialimportance, with a few exceptionsin which particular issuesare exploredapart from such primary concerns. We have seen that in most music linear and tonal functions coincide, and a central point has been that harmonic and melodic analysis isproperly viewed from the perspectives ofdiffering structural levels in which events havediffering implications. One of the main points of discussion hasthus been that analysis must discover the
various levels of structural
function. The
rnost immediate
is
that level which is the object of analysisseeking toidentify tonal and linear implications of all the notes thecomposer haswritten. But an event that is of essential functionat a given level, e.g., a cadential note,is often seen to have at other levels auxiliary function in relation to structural factors of higher hierarchic order.
It is this kind of consideration that leads to the identifica-
tion of structural profiles which are more and more fundamental, and which underly that of the harmonic-melodic surface. Still, in the surface reside many, probably most, of the characteristic features bywhich the uniqueness of expressivepower is felt. It might well be that all the melodies ofMozart, transposed to C and reduced to fundamental structural bases, would look very much alike.! In some of the foregoing analyseswe have been concernedwith supplementary, important approaches. Harmonicand tonal rhythms are such a concern, as are considerations
of dissonance-consonance fluctuation and
of
density fluctuation, especially in styles in which tonal implications are obscured. Melodic
curve, motivic
unities, and
rates of eventfulness are other
such important supplementary concerns.And in music in which harmonic colors are an important factor in themselves, sometimesindependently of tonal function, we have noted that auxiliary chords are often introduced for relatively pure linear function and for their sonorousqualities, evenin parallel, nonfunctional streams or complexes of like sonorities in elaborative formulations.
Analysis cannot hope to point to a single, correct conception structural functions.
of
To make this clear and to remind the reader from time
to time that in most music a number of analysescan be shown to be admissible, wehave repeatedlyused theword interpretation to describethe resultsof
tonality
180
analysis.The skillful analyst learns,however, to make the most valid possible choicesamongconceivablesuggested interpretationsof a givenexample,on the basisof proceduresand criteriaofjudgment of the mostpersuasivepossible substance.
Chapter 1 will have to be seen,ultimately, in the total context of which it is an aspect.The concernsof the remaining chaptersare with texture and rhythm in music; fundamentally, this book regardsmusical effect and experience as deriving from the complementary and counteractive functional associationsof all element-actions. The comprehensiveanalysis of music must take all into account, and must see,ultimately, their progressiveand recessiveactions in interrelations by which expressiveeffect is realized.
NOTES In the samecollectionoccur manyother examplesof potentialvaluein theseconnections, and while spacedoesnot permit their investigationhere the readeris advised that he will find this basic,rich anthologya fertile ground for study of early tonal structures.For instance,a later Renaissance composition,the delightfully spirited chanson of Lasso,Bonjourmoncoeur,p. 159,might be analyzedfrom the standpointof a tonal structureon G which can soundto modernearsambivalentin its modal Ff and consequentleaningtoward C, in tonicizationsof D and evenA in tentativetonal expansions a stepbeyondthat of Ex. 1-7 foreseeing the later tonal principle of expansionto the level of the dominantbeyondthe dominant!,and relativehomorhythmin which the lower voicemovesa great deal in 5ths and 4ths. The tonal systemof the Lassoexamplemight well beseento havethefollowing,symmetricalimage: FC
GD
a.
bMeasures19-37of the samepiecereveala comparableexpansionof the tonal system in the prolongationof T:I. Secondarytonicsb/ and D! both diatonic factorsin t! occur. Moreover,a synopticsketchof the bassshowsa linear expressionof t:iv
The D/ systemcomponentis stronger than
any of the fluctuant tonal allusionsto th.'s point; it is continued, with its own auxiliary subdominantand dominant functions,for sevenmeasures. The two Liszt examplesshowtonal expansionin elaborationof harmonicprogressionT:I-V! and prolongation T: I-I!. ~A comparableexampleof the principle morebroadly applied is Brahms'Intermezzo in A minor, Op. 118,No. l. In that piecethe primary tonic a! is stronglyaffirmed at the end, supportedby a strongdominantpedal.But the first primary dominantis in m. 11,the first tonic very brief! in m. 19.Thus,the primary tonic is embellishedby fluctuation at the outset,and in a sense the fluctuationis moreradical than in Op. 53 sincethe first measureis itself a secondarytonal reference.There are passingrefer-
tona/ity 181 ences toF, G, and C before thefirst primary leading-tone appears. Even thereafter the musicobscures the primary tonic more thanit supportsit. The structural importance ofthe primary tonic is that it draws intofocus followingthe tonal ambiguity which is the chieftonal featureof mostof the form. Thereare, in the Huctuantareas of the Intermezzo, certainhints ofthe ultimatetonal course:the substitutionof a sixfour onA in m. 2for theexpected resolution of theF:V-1; theaugmented 6thin m. 5, which expects A:V but docs notproduce it; and otherabortive references to functions which, in their normal contexts,would be supportive of the primary tonic. Similarly, thereare afterthe final,long dominantpedal, reminiscences of earliertonal deviations-e.g., the BL of m. 35 part of a dominantof the subdominant!, andthe F-C appoggiaturaover the primary dominantof mm. 37-38. A synopsis ofthe bass succession might be asfollows; it shows againthe principle of linear expression of t : i, and the tonal system components are a,F, C-factors in the F :I F = SD/R/t!, F beingthe systemof referencewith which the tonal structure setsout. Thus, of the two most fundamental triadsof reference,the overall bass lineexpresses one and a resumé oftonal systemcomponents expresses the other:
' E: -an" ='
|p
c D/SM R/t! -, .1
and e-
SM
SD/R/t!
A remarkably comparable example for melodicanalysis can be foundin mm. 12-19 of the first violin part, second movement, of Bart6ks Divertimento forstring orchestra: its essential course runs from all established and prolonged differentlybut with comparably strongeH`ect! to da, againin a tritonal essentialmovement. Thereader will find in the Bartok melody thetechnique ofsequence repeated motivic unitstransposed ina conjunctsuccession of subsidiary highpoints oflargely diatonicrelation! : U _i,»"~-___;_--- `92_¢ __ gf '
Q1 1 3
| » I
In Accenti, second pieceof the piano set Quaderno Musicale di Annalibera,by Luigi Dallapiccola, thesingle notewhich emerges out ofthe harmonicallydense motivethe final noteof theform ofthe serialhexachord used in the given instance!often takeson a tonal meaning simplybecause, inits severeisolation anddynamic stress, it hasthe effect ofresolving the preceding, intense dissonance and density. Whenthere are harmonic ormelodic elements that supportthe feelingof tonalresolution atthe same time, that feeling is of courseenormously strengthened. Thus, at the pieces final cadence a strong sense of tonalfulfillment arisesfrom thequasi-dominant character of the outervoices ofthe harmonyofthe final measure B and Dil! as wellas thechromatic succession inmoving voices: C18-D-D#-E. Whether this is the composersintent cannot besaid, butthere canbe little question thatthese factors impart tonalimpressions and,thereby, strongerimpact to the powerfulcadential thrustwith which the piece ends. Another example useful forstudy inthe sameconnection isthe firstof Weberns Five Piecesfor orchestra,Op. 10, in which a concluding,reiterated F,in an atmosphere ofseverely reduced texture, hasthe effectof quasi-tonalresolution. Thepiece should bestudied for evidences, ofwhich many could be cited, of quasi-functional projections of or expectantsuggestions of!F, and A and C, in the pitch materials of the piece. [For example, lastnotes of the trumpet-F, G, A, F; trumpet and trombone ofmm. 6-7, centering onF; the leading-tone E in the flute motive of mm. 8-9; clarinet phraseof mm. 4-7, centering on F and A; or the succession E
82 tonality flute!-F-F if-G violin!-A-G1f cello! preceding andsupportive oftonal feeling in the final, isolatedF.] Certainly it must beacknowledged thatthe analystwill find many instancesof apparently arbitrarily and rapidlyfluctuant change,or staticdissonance effect, in recent styles. Thethird of Schoenbergs set of piano pieces, Op.19, isa casein point: dissonance seems relatively fixedin intensity,as isharmonic densitypitch factorsof four and five in chords throughout thepiece!. At the sametime, the pieces structureis compellingly shapedby other factors-quasi-tonal function and harmonic rhythm are ofparticular importancewithin the harmonic element, not to mention theshaping effects of other element-fluctuationsspatial field, movements ofline, rhythms of attack, directionsof dynamicintensity, etc.!.It must not beassumed, then, that dissonance fluctuation is necessarily a significantfactor in the shapingof structure.The discussion ofthe Op. 23 extractinvolves onlytwo-note simultaneities; where chords are concernedthe evaluationof dissonance quality and process involves appraisal of such qualityas thesum ofinterval propertiesin chords.This can be calculatedin a number ofways, andagain ajudgment mustbe madeas tosignificance ofdissonance quality in intervallic relations between thelowest in relation to upper voices,the upper in relation to lower voices,or all intervallic relationsamong all voices inthe texture. Uln the third of Eight Etudes and a Fantasy for woodwind quartet, Elliott Carter develops hisform entirely out of coloristic contrasts,since theEtude is entirely an expression of the D major triadwithout changein its position ordistribution andwithout auxiliary notes orharmonies. Onlythe assignment of notesofthe triad among the four instrumentschanges. Itis perhapsthe mostelemental expression of tonality that is possibleto imagineand it is, in this piece,of strikingeffectiveness, although it is of course adevice thatdoes notbear repetition. When early modal scalesare usedas resources in melody and diatonic harmony in twentieth-century styles, as theyare not infrequently, especiallyin earlier music of this century,they createthe samemodal/ tonalambivalences one often associates with
works of] say, thesixteenth century, in which,for example, the mixolydian lowered 7 can to modern ears effecta leaning toward the region of the subdominant.For example, thefirst thirteen measures ofBart6ks Divertimento for string orchestra, third movement,sustain, inintense tonalreference, aquasi-augmented-6th harmony of primary dominant function.It is followed bya V-I root succession setting offthe principal thematicelement, amixolydian melodywhich, while on an F tonic, leans toward Bb. In this piece, thereader maywish to evaluate dissonance fluctuation in the light of the composers own specifications of dissonance in thesource cited on p. 1 10.A number of cadentialsuccessions show what appearsto bea verypurposeful dissonance control: mm. I4-15, in the direction of milder dissonancenote, too,the voice-leading determinants: outer voice contrarymotion, and more conjunctinner voice!; m. ll, an appoggiatura figure whose pattern of dissonance release is very evidentwhen thetenor and sopranovoices areregarded independently; and m. 6, both of whoseharmonies contain sharpdissonances, but in which there isnevertheless sense a ofrelease occasioned bythe factthat in the firstharmony thedissonance occurs between outer voices while in the secondit is relegated toan innerposition withthe consonantmajor 10th between outer voices-a good example of the mitigating injiuence qfspacing and distribution inharmonic dissonance effect. In mm. 18-22, the general increase in dissonance intensity seems tobe a compensation forrelative rhythmic inertia; and toward m. 30 there is a distinct mounting of dissonanceand steady increase in density! towardclimax. JRave1s Surgi de lacroupe du et bondNo. 3of TroisPoémesMallarmé! de mightbe citedas an examplein which tonal structureis notconfirmed, i.e.,is left open, at the conclusion. Theearly, relativelyconcise tonalexpressions of D major! deteriorate atits
tona/ity 183 conclusion, where there isonly theslightest hintof the original tonic. The final harmony isa combinationof dominanton C, to which the basshas progressed in chromatic descentfrom E, and an A major triad with F 8added andnot resolved. This latter, if construed asa primary dominant, isthe only,faint allusionto the initiating D system.!Tonal clarity thus substantially dissolves in the finalcadence. Inthis sense, the tonal structure is illustrative of opposition ofstability and instability relative clarity and ambiguity! asshaping features. The earlierexample fromBrahms l-14! and theBrahms Intermezzo, Op. ll8, No. I, cited infootnote description on pp. 18081, reveala patternin whichinstability oftonal reference gives way to ultimate,strong clarification; the Ravel doesjust the opposite:
*It wassuggested earlier that the two mostprominent tonic sonorities, harmonically conjoined, providethe intervallic basis for the recurrent motive of superimposed 3rds:
1
The collectionof thesemotives atthe endof the movement canbe seenas a wholetone seriesin which, as noted,basic tonalfactors arerecalled: .-I. lla f.92 & .. 92___..,
ILJT _92 92 J92.||92
L, »-lnL, &:?
_
The lastof themotive statements, that ofmm. |38-39,is tonallythe mostdeviant, but it is strongly functionalin reemphasizing, at a penultimate point,the Bbupper leading-tone with which the violin phrasehad concluded,at the same timeproviding a tonally distantperspective against which the ultimate cadentialrelease isstrikingly enhanced ineffect:
---_____""~-__ -_--____" _-__` `§_1```` _"V/ 92 /*92 92{1-l/§H"§/_'" I" I If Relatively anomaloustechniques oftonal usagelike bitonality and polytonality are not treatedas suchin this study, but in a bitonal situation e.g., Stravinsky, Symphonies qf Wind Instruments, 1947 revision,following rehearsalNo. ll, where different tonics areactively anddeliberately opposed! the expressionof tonal center at each stratum is likely to be in accord with techniques andfunctions likethose notedin this chapter.Indeed, it is characteristicof mostmultitonal usagethat each stratum asserts itsindependent tonal center ina verydirect, evenprimitive, manner,as in the Stravinskyexample cited.
CHAPTER TWO
texture
/n troduotory notes
Certain of the qualities and classifications of musical texture have been treated abundantly in the work of music theorists; references to relative densities andsparsities oftexture, to categories ofdescription such as polyphonic or monophonic, and to many other features and types of musical fabric are common. But adequate formulationhas notbeen givento analytical treatment of processesinvolving textural events and changes, or to the significances ofthese in the structure of music. What is musical texture? The texture of music consists ofits sounding components; it is conditioned in part by the number of those components sounding in simultaneity or concurrence, its qualities determined by the interactions, interrelations, and relative projections and substances ofcomponent lines or other component soundingfactors. Density may be seenas thequantitative aspectof texture--the number of concurrent events the thickness of the fabric! as well as the degree of compression of events within a given intervallic space. There is a vital relation between density and dissonance; the relative intensity of a highly compressed textural complex say, three components within the range of a minor 3rd! is a product of the severity of dissonance as well as of density. Furthermore, density clearly has a relation to coloration; thus, two simultaneous pitchessounding in tight compression say, amajor 2nd apart! will project varying degreesof intensity depending on relative homogeneity of coloration e.g., two clarinets, forte! as opposed to dissimilar coloration in which the proximity in musical space seems attenuated by the separation implied in the disparity of color. Similarly, intense dynamic levelsexaggerate The term concurrent or concurrence is used inthe sense of occurring together -coexisting, intersecting,overlapping inreal time. Two lines are in some degree, and at some specified level, concurrent if they overlap inany part. Concurrence ofindividual sounds is onlyof themoment ofsounding together, unless such sounds are parts ofconcurrent lines. 184
texture 185
the effectof spatialcompression. Relations between coloration and density could be traced far beyond thesesimple observations. The nature of interactions and interrelations
within the musical fabric,
apart from calculable density, might be said to constitute the qualitative, as distinct from the purely quantitative, aspect of texture. Thus, in conventional broad linesof classification,monophony isa type of texture, and a condition in which certain qualitative features oftexture apply. Unlike textures of two or more concurrent componentschordal, polyphonic! monophony isexplicit in its condition of minimal density. Examples which follow will be much
involved with the demonstrationof musicalprogression and recession as shaped by changes bothin densigfand in the qualitative interactions qf the components ¢y musical texture. Reference will be made as well to the applications of such changesin the delineation offorms-as, for example, inthe emergenceof polyphony in developmental contexts,the stabilization of textures in cadential formulations, 'or the common association of relatively uncomplicated texture with
thematic statement.
Of necessityin the analysis oftextural qualitiesis the evaluation of various kinds of interrelations and interactions among textural componentsthe degreeand nature of interlinear concordance agreement,lack of conflict! or coincident factors of relative intensity and variance counterpoint!. Changes in relative independence and interdependence among concurrent components in a given musical texturewill be seen toconstitute someof the most decisive and subtle! factors in the expressive shapingof structure. Much of the attention of this chapter will be devoted to considerations of textural progression and recession shaped as bysuch changesin interlinear relations and in qualitative and quantitative textural conditions, and to the convergence or contrast between a textural structure
and those of other elements.
The complementary and compensatoryrelations of texture with other elements of structure will be evident in examples analyzed in this chapter. A simple instance maybe cited: that in which, for example, heavier,greater densities seemfrequently to require relatively reduced rhythmic activity. Other relations of complementation and counteraction compensation! between textural and other factors are suggested, forexample, in foregoing references to coloration and
texture.
If a single pitch is sounded,a texture here, oneof maximal simplicity! is established. If a second pitch is sounded in simultaneity, the texture is altered-its density is increased. If the two pitches are a 2nd apart and they are succeeded bytwo pitches a 6th apart the upper moving up a 4th, the lower down a 2nd!, a textural event takes place-a succession involving not pure quantity density-number! but involving densitycompression and a number of important qualitative factors in the texture viewed independentb' of otherelements. An incipient quality of interlinear indepen-
186 texture
dence is asserted in the opposition of direction of movement-a quality subject to enhancement if rhythmic differentiation in the relations is introduced.! Two lines moving in parallel 3rds may in an important sense besaid to constitute a single real textural factor consistingof two components. At any point at which differentiation is established-in rhythm, in direction of motion, in the distanceof motion, or in any other sense-a texture initially consisting ofa singlereal factorof two sounding components!becomes atexture of two realfactors or at least progresses inthe direction of such differentiation!. Progressions and recessionsinvolving changes of these kinds or of analogous effectare decisivein the shaping of musical structure. The foregoing extremely simple illustrations are suggestiveof proposed distinctions among implicationsof factorsof quantity and quality in musical texture, both subject to a vastrange ofsubtle and constantly changingfeatures. The sectionsimmediately following are concernedwith the theory and terminology of musical texture, and with further fundamental considerations of the means bywhich textures undergo quantitative and qualitative modifications in functional events
Textura/ progression, factors
and in the delineation
of forms and structures.
recession, and var/lat/on as structura/
Example 2-la serves asa very simple, but exceedingly vivid, instance of progression andrecession withinthe element of texture. In the example, thereis progressivedevelopment oftextural complexity toward m. 4 and recessive decline in that complexity toward textural accord and simplicity! in approach to the cadence atm. 7. The changesin degrees of density and textural diversity-the succession toward maximal interlinear independence andthence toward textural homogeneity-can be traced by evaluation of the numbersof sounding and real components ateach point of significant change. The exposition of the brief subject, presentedin overlapping imitative entries at harmonic intervals of the 4th and 5th, is conventional. Lessstandard in procedure, but important in the expressive quality as an element of asymmetry, is the variation in time intervalof imita-
tion: 2al , 4J ,2J The . accrual of voices, their relative independence in the directly vertical sense!asserted byrhythmic and/or directional differen2The termcomponent may refer genericallyto any textural ingredientor factor as indicated in the immediatecontext of consideration, andas qualified by such adjectival modifiers asreal component, inactive component, doubling component, etc.
texture 187 Ex. 2-1e. Milhaud, Six Sonnets for mixedchorus No. ; 3,A peinesi lecoeur vous a considérées images et figures lfthe hearthas scarcely considered you, images and impressions! on textof J. Cassou.
J = 92/ "ff
-1 pei-ne si
le
016 F A pei-ne
si le
coeur vous
a con~si - dé
V .I
H
A pei- ne si
le coeur
vous a con - si - dé-rées, vous
i "2
,5
'V A
m.4
'Q .L _LQ Q3J is coeur vous
IV rees vous_a
Fi a con
a con - si-dé-rées,
i ma-ges et 6 - gu - res
i0 II con - sl-de-rees, 1
92 --Z . si -
0
ma-ges F1 - gu
- res
ma-ges fi
- res
§.f - dé
- rées,i
pei-ne si le coeur____ vousa con-si-dé - rées,i-ma-ges Reprinted permission by Heugel qf é? Cie., Paris, France.
- gu
6-
gu -
res
188 texture
tiations, canbe represented as follows:l _l_ if l_ _L!}_
the lattercondition of
ll 1
maximal diversity achieved at m. 4, with the entry of the fourth voice. At the end of m. 4 and the beginning of m. 5 the upper two voices become associated in parallel rhythms and directions we are overlooking minor differentiations in interval of motion at the most foreground level!; this 2
condition mightbe represented by thesymbol _L-showingthree realcom1
ponents and four soundingcomponents-the first significant decline in the textural diversity. Immediately subsequently the lower voices are disposed in comparable association: 2 , representing a further decline in textural 2
diversity and complexity but none in density-number!. Finally, at the approach to the cadence, all four voices becomestrongly interdependent in both rhythm and direction of movement with octave duplications!-a vital factor in the expressionof cadence. Two extremely foreground factors might be noted in extension of the analysis in Ex. 2-lb. First, in the recessiveprocess distinctionsof intervallic Ex. 2-1b. Qualitative and quantitativetextural progression and qualitativerecession inthe Milhaud excerpt.
1lllg2g24
1 21,
lg gl
1 _1_
2 1
1
92 Quality curveconditioned as changes by in independence-interdependence!
T Quantity density-number! curve
3In thissymbolization, theactual verticalalignment of voices isnot necessarily represented. Parentheses may denotea componenthaving independence or substancein some way restricted. The term density-number will be usedto distinguishthis parameterof density from that of density-compression,briefly notedearlier. Thesetwo aspectsof densityconstitute texturesquantitative dimension;both havefurther treatmentlater in this chapter.
texture 189
content amongthe variouscomponents continueto occasionmodest diversity so that, for example, in a situation of almost consistenthomogeneity m. 6! as manyas threedifferent ascendingleaps areprojected? A second very1oc_al factor can
be seen in the cadence itself, where in one sense there are four
components four different pitches and concluding successions!, inanother sense onethe agreementof all four voices in rhythm and direction!, and in another sense two octave duplications of soprano-tenor and alto-bass!. The analysissuggested inEx. 2-lb traces thetextural processmore broadly, omitting any accounting of such local, minor differences,but it is important that they be mentioned as indication of the complexity of even modesttextural development and analysis beyond relatively categorical observations. The foregoing example illustrates very simply the concept of textural progression and recession, thecontrolled shaping of textural events here,both quantitative and qualitative! in speci/ic structuralfunctions here, cy' development and
cadence!. Changes in texture-surely quantitative changes,but thoseinvolving textural qualities as well-are :Wen among the mostreadily perceptible and appreciable inthe experience of music. An example from _Iosquinis drawn from a setting of the De profundis Ex. 2-2!.
The example is a beautiful illustration of the controlled sloping of textural structure. The phrase is characterized,in its texture, by significant interdependences ofthetwo linesat the beginning andend, andby significant independences in its central portion. The complementary relation of the textural processesto the melodic structure of the superius is stunningly effective, as is the complementary application of rhythmic technique-syncopation anddotted rhythm at the climactic point. The associationof the two lines at the phrase extremitiesis not without diversification: at the outset,there isconsistent rhythmicidentity but oblique, then contrary, motion; and there is some directional opposition within the homorhythmic approach to the cadence.The internal portion is characterized by rhythmic and directional differentiation. The phrase is thus an illustration of progressive diversificationtoward a point of relative intensity, and of the cadential function of` increased interlinear interdependence. In the kind of symbology which has begun to emerge in these discussionsthe
example mightbe saidbroadly toprogress, then recede, from2 to L to 2. l
These twoexamples expose, of course,only someof many parameters of textural shaping; other exampleswill pose other kinds of issuesand reveal other functional, expressive, processive operations. It seems clear that what we have described as textural diversity complexity, activity-like density anaspect ofintensity in the texture! seeks 5This texturalcondition is identified asheterophony.
190 texture Ex. 2-2. Josquin, Deprofundis c/amaviMotot!. m.36
'f
V if
:°~*°JHiU
1, f
_
Errflffrg Reprinted _/rom the Smgers edition, published by G.Alsbach and Co., Amsterdam, by permission qf Creyghton Musicology-Musica Antiqua, Bilthoven, Netherlands.
release in what wehave described as reduced interlinear independence i.e., textural interdependence, accord, homogeneity, simplicity, inactivity!.Thus, progression toward increased levelsof diversity and interaction creates the sense ofneed for reconformity, an expectation that the trend toward com-
plexity will be reversed in cadentialexpression. Like dissonance, rhythmic acceleration, ascent in pitch,etc., theprogressive complication and diversification ofmusical textureare assumed to be evocative ofthe impression of rising intensity,an intensitywhich increases the more the progression is prolonged.
texture 191 Types of musical texture; terminology
prob/ems of
c/assi#cation and
Texture isconceived asthat elementof musicalstructure shaped determined, conditioned! by the voice or number of voices and other components projecting the musical materials in the sounding medium, and when there are two or more components!by the interrelations andinteractions amongthem. Except, of course, in monophonic textures,the evaluation of interrelations and comparative substanceof motive, of activity or stasis,etc.! in and among thecomponents ofthe texture is the all-important problem of analysis both toward the characterizationofthe textural class andtoward the analysis and understanding of functional successions oftextural events. Rhythm is surely the most critical factor in interlinear relations; thus, within the independence-interdependence scale of textural values it is the most decisive factor in the assertionof interlinear opposition diversity, resistance, counterpoint!. Attention is essentialtoo to interrelations of motivic material including imitation!, intervallic content, directional opposition, and dissonance,as well as number and degrees of proximity in the vertical alignment of events, i.e.,density. One is reluctant, as always, to make the problem of terminology a central issue; nevertheless, apart of the businessof music theory is lexical, and words along with some concordance of understanding as to what they signify! are essential todiscourse. Complexhyphenated terminologicalforms which follow from the system outlinedhere arerarely needed but are nonetheless logically plausible and fitting! ; and it is always possible simply to describe events,even though terms denotative of textural conditions and classes are a useful convenience.
If allowance can be understood forthe difficulties of iirm and arbitrary distinction within any spectrum of textural classifications, andif it can be well understood that any musical instance,seen beyondmost local events, is Density is defined asthat textural parameter, quantitativeand measurable,conditioned by the number of simultaneousor concurrent components andby the extent of vertical space encompassing them: densigr-number and densib'-compression. is interesting It that the two aspects of texturetermed density-compression and texture-space developed later as theoverall fieldor ambitusin which events takeplace! arenecessarily contradictory in progressive or recessive inclinations, assuming afixed density-number. Thus, theinflation of texture-space is regarded asan intensifyingfactor expecting recontraction! andincrease in density-compression similarly intensifyingcf. dissonance, and theexacerbated interlinear conflict of highly crowded events!. Thefact isthat cadentialprocesses in music aremore often thannot operationstoward spatialcontraction and,consequently, normally increased density-compression relation in to which suchrecessive factors as consonance and textural simplicity, aswell aslinear declineand anynumber offurther recessive element-successions, function compensatorily.
192 texture
likely to represent a mixture of such classifications,there will be value in the statement of proposedterminology. Certain widely used terms of relatively_firm conventional signyieation canbe accepted asgenerally understood. 1. Pob/phonic,while literally meaning many-voiced, can serve to denote, as conventionally, multivoiced texture of considerable interlinear independence, oftenimitative; it is thusgenerally understoodto havequalitative implications beyond its literal, limited meaning. 2. Homophonie would literally denote a condition of interdependent voices, but its traditional connotation is that of texture in which a primary voice is accompanied bya subordinatefabric sometimesinteractive in tentative ways, the bass normally in a contradirectional or other contrapuntal relation to the primary voice or voices!. 3. Chordalis aperfectly acceptable,and very useful, conventionalterm referring simply to texture consisting essentiallyof chords, its voices often relatively homorhythmically related. 4. Doubling can denote lines homorhythmically-homodirectionallyhomointervallically associated see thefollowing definitions of these terms!. 5. Mirror association, usuallyunderstood asstrict, involves a relation that is homorhythmic-homointervallic-contradirectional; again, it is a term in common usage.
6. Heterophonie is understood to denote a relation that is homodirectional parallel in contour! but heterointervallic see below!-having minor diversification of
interval content.
7. Heterorhythmic is a conventional term adopted in accord with its conventional signification see below!. 8. Sonorigfmay be defined as the overall sonorous character determined by texture including doublings! andcoloration including articulation and intensity of dynamics!. 9. Counterpointcontrapuntal! denotes a condition of interlinear interaction involving intervallic content, direction, rhythm, and other qualities or parameters of diversification. The traditional usage of contrapuntal is comparable to that of polyphonic.! ` 10. Monophonicis taken, as conventionally, to mean single-voiced monolinear! . Beyond the above, it seems appropriate to formulate certain terms which can be used to apply with some precision to specific conditions of interlinear relation in multivoiced or two-voiced! and multilinear textures." 7In thisstudy theterm linerefers toany texturalcomponent inwhich horizontalrelation and configuration canplausibly be traced as a logical continuity-an identifiable stratum inthe textureat somegiven level.The termvoice willnormally denotea linehaving distinct relativeindependence; itmay thusbe a complex ofdoubled lines,but is not itself
texture 193
A system-scale spectrum! of textural conditions or values from simple e.g.',monophonic! to complex e.g., ultimately multivoiced, contrarhythmic-contraintervallic-contradirectional,3 the highest manifestation of polyphony! can be establishedas a lexical basis for description of relations among textural components which are concurrent or simultaneous at some given level. In the following system the prefixes homo- uni-,or eo-!,° hetero-, and contra-'° areadopted to refer to conditions of identity, mild and veiy local diversification asin the conventional heterophonic!, and morepronounced contrast, respectively.Moreover, three specific parameters aspects, dimensions, spheresof reference! are adopted as relevant to the evaluation of textural conditions: these arerhythm specificallyrhythmic pattern!, direction of melodic succession!, andlinear intervalliccontent." l. Within the parameter of rhythm, the terms homorhythmie, heterorhythmic both of these in conventional usage!, and eontrarhythmie all emerge as potentially applicable and useful. 2. Within the parameter of direction, the terms homodirectional, heterodireetional, andeontradireetional motion in a straight line exists asa possibility along with motion up and down! all have potential applicability to relations among components oftexture.
capable ofdoubling. Theterm multilinearalso bilinear,trilinear, etc.! can thusbe usedto denote textureof morethan onesimultaneous or concurrent component. As arule multivoieed two-voiced, three-voiced, many-voiced! has qualitative implications.Polyphonic, asnoted earlier, strictly means the same thing and has conventional implicationsof qualitative diversification.! Withinthese distinctions, which seemnecessary and unavoidable indiscussion ofmusical texture,monophonic single-voiced! texture can of coursebe multilinear; line isthe moregeneric concept. While such hyphenated terms are rarelyneeded, theymight beused inprecise reference toa complexof specificconditions ofinterlinear relation.It is more usualto refer to single parameters of consideratione.g., homorhythmic,or heterointervallic!,or perhapsto two conjoinedparameters of reference inhyphenated terms like contraintervallic-homodirectional. Applications of the prefixhomo- are simply anextension ofconventional usage, as in homorhythmic. 1°While the term polyrhythmic cf. the possible birhythmic, triintervallic, bidirectional, etc.! is areasonable and perhaps admissible synonym forcontrarhythmic, it is often usedin a sense more properly expressed as polymetric a distinctiondeveloped in Chapter3!. Beyond the terminological system outlined here,phrases likerhythmic contrast, intervallic conformity, directional opposition, etc., arefreely usedto describeparticular relations. An intervalclass IC!includes anygiven interval within the octave togetherwith its inversion complement!and all compound extensions expansions by one or more octaves! of the given interval or its inversion. If enharmonically equivalent forms areconsidered of the sameclass, thereare sixinterval classesthe unisonexcluded!; theissue ofenharmonic identity is,of course,one oftheappropriateness of functional distinctionsamong enharmonic equivalents withinany givenidiom.
194 texture
3. Within the sphere of intervallic content, the terms homointervallic [applicable to IC interval class!, intervalcomplement, specificinterval, etc., as areall terms within this group-a distinction to be clarihed when appropriate], heterointervallioregarded as synomous with the conventional heterophonic!, and oontraintervallie can all be used to describe particular textural situations and relations, usually applying to specific intervals rather than classes.
Of vital, general importanceis the need in all analysisor description of musical texture and textural conditions to maintain awarenessof the level of structure towhich referenceis made. Thus, two lines may have contradirectional relation in a local sensebut be seen, significantly,to have homodirectional associationat a broader level.! Example 2-3 is a list of very brief, contrived examplesin illustration of the foregoingsystem ofterminological classification. Ex. 2-3.
Relations the at levelwithin the temporal context! illustrated
§: i# RR homorhythmic F heterorhythmic contrarhythmic
I'
QP##Fl, Fl, ll-1' -|ll---ull'
_-__I--1:2 I i
J. contradirectional *Applicable atlevels ofone, two,or threebars.
homodi ional r¢¢¢ heterodirectional#
texture 1.95 Relations atthe levelwithin thetemporal context! illustrated
homointervallic'
heterointervallic'
contraintervallic'
'As to specific intervalof motion.
Some further considerations of term/no/ogy and aspects of texture Doubling does not result in interlinear interdependence of any certain,
absolute degree: the intervalof doublinghas muchto dowith the extent of association betweentwo doubled components, evenwhen the doubling is absolutely strict. This fact has to be taken into account in evaluation of a textural situation in which doubling occurs, and it can be appreciated if the differences in degree of interdependence or f`usion! are considered when doubling is at the octave[duplication of PC pitch-class!], with texturespace andsonority affectedbut with the qualitative association ofthedoubled lines virtually complete, as opposed to a situation in which the interval of doubling is dissonant e.g.,2nd, or 7th!, where individual components assert a greater degree of independence. The factor of interval of doubling and its impact with respect to interlinear independence isone ofdissonance and of size or distance between the doubled components!. Of course, any doubling at whatever interval greatly compromisesthe relative independence of lines." 12It isnot clearwhether considerable space between two linesis conduciveto greater independence, or whether, onthe otherhand, a small distancewith dissonance,as in doubling in 2nds! occasions a heightenedinterlinear resistanceconflict = independence! than does alarger interval.The complexityof thisproblem isfurther evidentwhen oneconsiders the impactof specificinterval sizeas opposed to IC in, for example, arelation whichis contraintervallic but homodirectional, andwhen the disparity of interval of motion hasone component movingby, say,the compoundform of that of the other: a l0th with a 3rd. Presumably, therelative independence manifest in the larger interval of motion is considerable, despite the affinityof IC. No doubtone mightgo onto hypothesize situations posing
1.96 texture
It is noted above that there is one aspect of texture which is affected significantly by doubling at octave or compound-octave distances. Attention is givenlater to the rangeof the sounding complex its space,and the profiles delineated inspatial changes!as anaspect oftexture particularly important in certain styles.In the analysis oftexture-space, andin the evaluation of progressions andrecessions formed by its increases anddecreases, octave duplications are of course significant. We shall adopt the term texture-space or simply space! in referring to this aspect range, field, compass,ambitus! of texture.
Example 2-4 consists ofthree brief extracts from the Stravinsky Variations for orchestra, a work of importantly texture-conditioning structure. Our concern here is chiefly with illustrating some of the conditions and distinctions of terminology with which we are currently involved; the examples also indicate some complexities of the problem of evaluation of textural relations, and some of the features of textural process to be developed in later analyses. The initial utterance ishomodirectional resultingin considerableinterdependence ofcomponent lines!but contraintervallic, a factor to which any degree ofinterlinear independenceis attributable. The subsequent,responsive motiveof flutes and stringspits oneconcurrent choir against theother in a contradirectional relation denoting a qualitative increase in textural diversity!. The line between heterointervallic heterophonic! and contraintervallic texture is tenuous but significant: the strings asa group are in heterointervallic association, the flutes contraintervallically related. Within the strings, while celli and violins both have ascending tritonal leaps a homointervallie relation as to class!, they are differentiated in PC-the doubling is in fact at a highly dissonant interval, as well as in typical for Stravinsky! heterorhythm. The texture of the flutes is homorhythmic and homodirectional, that of the strings heterorhythmic-heterodirectional. That of the entire sounding complex is heterorhythmic-contradirectionalcontraintervallic." comparable questions; but the underlying pointis that each texturalsituation will, beyond certain importantgeneralities, pose issues particularto itself. It is assumed that doubling byperfect intervals,especially theoctave, andto a lesser degree consonant 3rds and6ths, producesa higher degree ofinterlinear interdependence than otherdoublings. Doubling at theunison ofcourse does notaffect textureat all-not even its spatialaspect, butonly sonority. Within each such relationis likenessflutes 1-2,and betweenviolas lower line and cello! involving duplication ofIC but disparity of distance-i.e., occurrenceof simplewith compound formsof the same intervals. The overall texture of flutes andstrings togethermight be described asof eight sounding components lines!, whileof the following numbersof realcomponents withineach ofthe threeparameters of differentiation: inthe rhythmicrelation, l ; in directional relation, 2; in intervallic relation,5 or 4 asto IC!.
Ex. 2-4. Stravinsky,Variationsfor orchestraAldousHuxleyinmemoriam!.
2 Fls.
Alto Fl.*
2 Ten.Trnbs
BassTmb.
Harp
Piano
Vln.
Vla.
Vc.
~All instrumentssoundaswritten. 797
texture 199
Both motivesof m. l are activated by devices ofcoloration 11° the articulative doublings of chords in piano, then harp. It is clear that, in summary, the relation between thetwo utterancesof m. 1 isone ofprogressive tendency toward textural diversification and expansion of texture-space! in what is perceived as a textural event ofimportant, although local, structuralsignificance. The discussionof m. 1 isindicative of the potential complexities ofanalytical treatment of apparently simple textural conditions, all of them significant factors in textural eject. The second quotation m. 6! is given in illustration of a single voice heavily underscoredin coloration and sonority. It is, of course,an extremely potent monophonic statement, with a variety of colors and articulations. In this instance,all doublings but one are unisonduplications-of PC and pitch and intervalof motion, as well as ofdirection ; the texture-spaceis thus affected no more than its monophonic class. One lower octave doubling does momentarily extend the space, as the double-bass launchesthe ascending motion with its initiating attack. And there is some heterorhythmic diversification, e.g., in the activating role of the harp.! The beautiful phrase quotedas thethird of the extracts, mm. 20-22, is a further useful illustration. It is strikingly interesting, here, to compare the successions of distinct
element-events: that
'of ascent and descent
of
melodic line, and that of rhythmic deceleration. But the comparison of textural and color successions affords vivid illustration of necessary distinctions betweentexture and color or sonority. The texture changesneither in density nor in class. But the coloration is curved in a manner beautifully complementary to the downward-upward curve and rhythmic recession of the line. The distinction-conceptual, terminological, practical-between coloration and texture isvery clear in this brief excerpt, as in numerous other passages inthe Variations. Concepts and techniques oftextural activation,also with reference toStravinsky, have extensive discussion later in this chapter. The Variations for orchestra are highly interesting in the study of texture. One avenue ofapproach wouldexplore thefunction oftexture asthematic, asa primeelement by which the form is ordered inlieu of the traditional concept ofmelodic-rhythmic theme and its transformations. Claudio Spies, inan analysisof the Variations [Notes on Stravinskys Variations, in Perspectives of NewMusic, IV, l l965!] gives onp. 63! an outlineofthe Variations form with divisions variations! marked by changesof tempo, texture, and coloration. Spieswrites pp. 62-63!: Of Variations thereare twelve,separated fromone another, inmost instances, by a measured pause, a fermataor a change oftempo .... There is, however,no Theme on whosemelodic, rhythmicor phraseological characteristics these variations areconstructed. Instead, a sectionaldesign ispostulated onvarieties ofchange and contrast, ratherthan onactual transformation-diversityin phrase-structure, for exampleon subtleor abrupt textural shifts,on rhythmic variables, tempo-relations,as wellas ona beautifully calibratedsystem ofrefrains anda perfectrecapitulation. Sections II, V, and XI constitute the structuralrefrain . . . , and the musicin these polyphonic measures provides thesingle mostarresting features of the Variations. Some sentences omitted.!
200 texture Texture and style
Characterization and evaluation of the qualities of musical texture are important means to the understanding of styles and style periods. A few parenthetical examplesof the kinds of questions havingto do with textural phenomena and their relevance to style classification and description will underscore this relation.
l. It is well understood that,for example, the role of the lowest voiceis a major consideration of style classification. Thus, a primary distinction between polyphonictextures oftheRenaissance and the Baroqueis the higher degree ofbass equivalence in the former, ascompared with its later increasing restriction to intervals of succession whichbest underlie and support harmonic content within a
tonal order.
2. The unique texture of organum, of two or more highly interdependent lines is the feature by which that style is primarily defined. And the evolution of stylistic tendencies within organum has to do essentially with the gradual progresstoward modestdegrees ofinterlinear diversification. 3. The radical style change which characterizes musichistory around the beginning of the seventeenth century, the development of monody, is significantly a textural phenomenon characterized by the advent of sung declamation with a subordinate _fabric onlyoccasionally vitalized by emergent, briefiy competing strands-a development perceivedas revolutionary largely because of the prevalence of polyphony of supreme contrapuntal values during the Renaissance. 4. The conceptual attitude termed pointillism, a dominant feature of some twentieth-century music, is a matter of texture. Such music is stylistically characterizedby distinctive, uniquely transparent textures. 5. The evolution of style and craft in the art of many a composer is most comprehensivelyand revealinglytraced in the evaluation of his textures at various points. Beethovensmusic is an obvious example. 6. Often the distinction between musical genres is most persuasively drawn on the basisof textural differences. Thus,it is possible togeneralize to a remarkable degree concerning textural distinctions between sacredand secular musicsof many ages cf mass movementsand chansonsof _losquin or Lasso!, or in the latter of thesecategories betweendance and other kinds of music.
7. Texture is often not, of course, invariably! the key to dramatic developments inthe resourcefulshaping ofother elements:thus, for example, it is often in relatively simple textures that the most expansive and adventuresome treatment of harmony is seen, as we have noted. Or consider the homophonic contexts in which highly embellished melody is to be found, as in keyboard music of the Rococo, or the fiorid operatic styles of the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries.!
texture 207
8. Any description of the style which we know by the term impressionism will necessarily givefundamental attention to texture. An obvious example of this is the technique, in impressionism, of ornamentation of a fundamental harmonic scheme by the idiosyncratic, uniquely
characteristic,
parallel movement of auxiliary chords within dense texturesof interdependent lines.
Textura/ rh ythm
This book suggests thatprogression andrecession withinelement-structures, events pacedand shapedin a way that is interesting and consistentin functional relation to an apparent expressiveend, are basic to musical effect. These studiesfurther suggest that a primary facet of rhythm is manifest in the pacing and qualities of the changes constitutingsuccessions of events at various hierarchic levels and involving the various elementsof structure." just as tonal, melodic, and harmonic events changes!express inqualities of extent of change! and pacing what we can describe as tonal rhythm, melodic rhythm, and harmonic rhythm, so the changes in texture are expressive, intiming andin thenature ofchange, of what is properly termed texturalrhythm. In a logical extension of this idea, reference canbe made to color rhythm in music-the consequence ofthe distribution and nature of
changes in structure as
delineated by
coloration, color
events.!
Naturally, and significantly, the idea of textural rhythm is extensibleto that of rhythms of its various aspectsdensity rhythm, spatial rhythm, etc.!. The importance of textural rhythm, and of other element-rhythms, can hardly be exaggerated. It may be realized, for example, that the rate of accrual of the voicesin the imitative exposition of a fugal subjectis a rhythmic phenomenon i.e., the pacing and assertiveness of such events in the quantitative-qualitative textural unfolding!, this specific aspectof progressiona factor of textural change asof no other elementand, hence,a fastor slow, strong or weak, succession in textural rhythm. Within structural segmentsboth large and small, the rate at which texture changesin the course ofprogression and recession isa vital aspect of expressive effect.Textural rhythm is of most obvious and immediate effect where changesin density are involved, and when thechanges aredecisive; butsubtle qualitative changes aresignificantly "The conceptof concurrentelement-rhythms, mutuallycounteractive orcoincident in articulation, is pointed out recurrently in this book. The comprehensive treatment of tonal, harmonic,melodic, textural,and metricrhythms notto mentionrhythms ofelementparameters-or subelemcnts-like density, dissonance, aspects of coloration likedynamics or timbre, etc.! would require a broad contextdevoted essentially to theseproblems; but Chapter 3gives summary emphasis tothe conceptof element-rhythmsand their functional relations, andto therange ofissues withwhich significantdevelopment ofthe conceptwould have tobe involved.
202 texture
expressive subtle of rhythmiceffect. Withthe concept of texturalrhythm we move toward a comprehensive concept of rhythm asthe combination and interaction
cy' allelement-rhythms-indeed, parametric of rhythms of individualtendencies within elementsas well e.g., oftexture, butalso ofits individual aspects!. Example 2-5ais a basis forcomment concerning textural rhythm as expressed very simply inthe recurrence of a signilicant characteristic, even motivic! textural event in a Webern song."
The pianopart isthroughout an alternation between two events sharply distinguished intexture asin other elements!: these appear inEx. 2-5athe triplet sixteenth-notes witheighth-note leap this motive is shortenedand
varied in its many appearances! and the punctuatingchord sometimes reduced toa singlepitch ofanalogous articulative eH`ect, as in m. 4 or m. 9!. Reference inthe followingcomments isto the recurring chord,clearly a motive of primarily textural definition. Although the evaluationof intervallic
content andits changes, as wellas functions of suchchanges, is an intriguing and importantproblem ofstudy with reference tothe recurring chordal motive, and one ofrelevance toconcern with texture, it cannot beundertaken comprehensively here.!
Ex. 2-5a. Webern, "Wiebin ichfrohI" How joyous Iaml!, No. 1 of Drei Lieder,Op. 25, on textof H. Jone.
12-tone set: i|
* ll*
Langsam J = ca 60 rit.
-.
P 9-L07
tempo rit.
_ll' _
Wie bin
I3
1 ll
ll.!'_IF- l uv; in IF
92 g
ich frohl
i 6 I _ =i§-v 1grl!1 ,VIZ /1 U
» pq f ll _k
u
9; q
_-
,-
_I
'.
F1
The songis givenin full in Burkhart,Anthology Musical _/br Analysis, 2nd ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart andWinston, Inc., 1972!, pp.497-99.
texture
205
Ex. 2-6a. Carter,EightEtudesand a Fantasyfor woodwindquartet,No.2.
Ob.
Bn.
«Soundingaswritten. Reprinted bypermission of Associated MusicPublishers, Inc.
ture with that of coloration!is largely thestructure.Textural progression and recessionfunction in a broadly axed space;" texture is accumulative in intensity to m. 12 and subsequentlyundergoesrecessiverelaxation to the s Despitebroadly fixed space,densityof coursevaries,in compression aswell asnumber, at local rnotivic levelsasstatementsarrive, proceed,exit, reenter,etc.
206 texture
cadence at mm. 17-18. Following this cadence, thereis quick reaccrual of the textural components precedingthe final, more abrupt cadence. These are quantitative considerations. Since the motive is fixed in length, the varied spacingsof its entriesthat is, the varied distances or time intervals! of imitation, are crucial in the structure; they constitute the principal qualitative element of textural change. The control of changesin the distance ofimitation taking into account contiguous entriesonly, although any entry is in a sensean imitation of all preceding entries! can be represented asshown in Ex. 2-6b. The figures represent distancescalculated in eighth-note units. Ex. 2-6b. Analysis ofthe qualitativetextural progressions and recessionsas theyconcern changing distances of imitation! in the Carter Etude. Measure number of imitative entry:
34
l5$6 5
56
89
a I3
-_ if
10 ll
4§4§3§3§3 1
--fi 1
12 I4
15
4§6§ f Maximal textural intensity m.l2!
What is striking here is, of course, the progressive complexityof the texture, the progressive horizontal compression achieved by the increased stretto. Thistechnique is of basic importance in qualitative textural progression in a greatdeal ofmusic, assubsequent examples will further demonstrate. The progression toward the smallest time interval m. 12! is, as one would expect in a subtly contrived art form, less than consistentand predictable. Measure 12 is followed by a succession ofrelease in which two subsequent entries follow at increasing distances in preparation of the strongly punctuative cadence of mm. 17-18.
The concluding bars 9-22; see Ex.2-6a!, which are a rapid, intense resumption of the motive, contain imitative entries at extremely close dis-
tances-2IJ!, 5, 2¢b-in a culminative, sharply progressive operation, followed by relatively abrupt cadential recession.
The Fantasies for stringsby HenryPurcell are, as awhole, atruly splendid sourcefor the analysis oftexture in all its implications. The quoted excerpt Ex. 2-7a! is the third of five sections typically
texture 207 Ex. 2-7a. Pu ell Fa t yln C mlnor Z 738, forst g fn.34 slow! J1'll'§ A lf % I° Ylfi -m l929292':111l2 ..92i1Slli7
EEF _ _ 3
I II |Jl" Vfjlli I l|.h"1J I l'1§'ll-IW11
fff f°f
V V3? 'V'
II NJIT' llI' Vfjll gil! I 'H
0
YCYI1 1 lIl'l; Up°:J_
92 f 92-9* m.36 7 I ¢§
J EE
F Wfff e
f 6!
==
J /,-15
5J
/' if'
__ F`.IV
1192
lull' In-'Y -!1 ll __-||
|!'||771 -_
fe e-=»» f 92J__,1. J
--
E e: !,.
%VF IIJII. _ lfl' . 11- l11 -l-_-_-22-_UI IVZLIH ll.4"ll| _.l1|_.l1_1i.11 IITIQQZZ
EQJ .I
% qg
H -1]
o 2
208 texture Ex. 2-7a continued. m.40
A "E' §'r
I
f'
F' V
I
_ .J
J
delineated by changes in tempo and motivic content. The qualitativequantitative progressionis striking. Ex. 2-7b. m. 35!
3* 3
mm. 38-39!
21
T [Tl
3
4
L 1! *As to rhythmic interdependence.
Following the process symbolizedin Ex. 2-7b, involving qualitative progression and recession, arediversification takes place in resumption of progressive qualitativechange asrepresented inEx. 2-7c. Ex. 2-7c. mm. 39-40!
42
L 2 l_
L 1
texture 209
Gne has only to give attention to thesemeasures asa listening experience to appreciate the persuasive effect,vitally functional, of the textural events represented. Time interval fluctuation is functional too: there is
vacillation first at between the intervals of 6,l, 2J anextreme, early contraction!, again 6,l ,again 2J , andthen imitation between voices 2 and4 at theintermediate distance of 4,|. Time intervals2ofJare associated with tentative stages in thefirst qualitative progression; of that 4J is associated with and somewhat compensatory in relation to! the area of maximal diversification and complexity of interlinear independence. The reader is urged to pursue questionsof textural structure and process inother sections of this fantasy, and in other fantasies ofPurcell, proceedingto consideration of overall textural shape to which those of individual sections contribute.
Density and d/ssonance
Density asthe number of sounding components isthe density-number; density as the ratio of the number of sounding components toa given total space is the densigf-compression.
Despite thesedirect and apparently simplepropositions, thequestion of density is, like those of other aspects of texture, very complex indeed. For example, while there can be no doubt that the proximities by which components areseparated invertical alignment the degreeof compression!constitute an aspectof density superimposed 2nds make upa denser textural complex than superimposed 5ths!, the issue of dissonance hasto be regarded as a related, conditioning factor. It is reasonable,for instance, to ask whether an harmonic tritone is in some sense moredense than a major 3rd. At the sametime, it is a convenience toregard the evaluation of dissonance as 2'Density, inthe secondsense inwhich wehave definedit that having todo with the extent ofcompression of the texturalcomponents-i. e.,their spatialboundaries in the vertical field andtheir distributionwithin suchspace! canalso beseen inits interdependentrelation to dissonance in the higher probabilityof dissonance, and itshigher intensity,in contextsof greater density-compression. I t is assumed that dissonance effect canbe evaluated and itsimpact asa texturalfactor subject therefore to analysis. On pp. 107-ll there is referenceto the problem ofclassification of dissonance severities.! Whatever scale of dissonance values onechooses to accept, or however itis consideredto be modified-as it must be-according to the historical stylein question, itis probablytrue that the minor 2nd and its compounds m9, etc.! or inversion M7! are in generalexpressive of relative dissonance and that of thesethe smallestinterval, the m2itself, isthe dissonance of highestintensity. Texture density-compression! clearly is a factorin this judgment, andin the subsequent statement that among the forms within anyIC dissonance properties are more severe the smaller the form. Whetherthe M2 is moreintense adissonance than the largerM7 is however problematic;these aredistinct classes.! The position of the m2 in the scaleof dissonance values, inany style,that is, in which the dissonanceconsonance profile is apertinent structuralfactor, seems doubtless; whatis moresignificant as astyle factoris the manner ofits resolution.
texture 2
11
Figure 2-l gives the key points in a quantitative progression in the Schoenberg song,Tot, in a statementof the density-number curveexpressed in numbersof simultaneouspitches. SeeEx. l-52a, pp. 174-75,which quotes most of the extracts to which reference is made.!
The graphic representation in Fig. 2-1 is explicit in description of the quantitative progression of that aspect oftexture which we havetermed densitynumber, and its subsequentrecession. Atthe microlevel, of course,there are further gradations; for example, the extreme opening of the songhas adensity-number 2,
and the final sound is of density-number 1.
Fig. 2-1. Functional curve ofdensity-number in Schoenberg, "Tot," from Three Songs, Op. 48. Measure numbers: Density-number :
l-6 6 4- 5-6
9-10 ll-12-13 78
l3-14 I5-18 63
The successionis spacedin a rather common linear ordering in which there is accrual toward a point slightly beyond the temporal middle of the song.The high melodic linesof m. l l are a complementary factor in this progression, asis the striking coloration in that bar, with the voicepart heard entirely below the piano. The dynamics, on the other hand, are at times complementary to the density-numbercurve m. 6! and at times a compensatory element m. ll!. The foregoing discussion doesnot but could be extended to!take up the problem of fluctuation in density-compression. In the texture of the cadential harmony from the third of Weberns Pieces, Op.7, forviolin and piano Ex. 2-9! ,dissonance anddensity, whichare clearly interrelated factors here-one an intensification of the other-are of primary signihcance
in effect.
The structure
of the
piece is, in part,
a
progression towardincreased densities: the openingis relatively dense incompression butnot in number-a sustained note in the violin with staccato,ppp, impulses inthe piano a semitoneabove never prolonged! ; but subsequently the textures are relatively open and uncrowded until the conclusion. The piano texture immediately preceding the one quoted is a cluster of three pitches two semitones, within a diminished 3rd!.
texture
212
Ex. 2-9. Webern,FourPieces,Op.7, for violin and piano,No.3. Example1-28, p. 93, quotesthe completeviolin part.!
Sehrlanasam/=ca.60!
m.6
col legno,tyeichgezogen
m. 12
Copyright 1922,Universal Edition.Usedbypermission of thepublisher.Theodore Presser Companysokrepresentative UnitedStates, Canada andMexico.
texture 213
The density of the final sonority, a striking and emphatic contrast to the prevalent textures of the piece,is of course relativelyambiguous in pitch content becauseof its low register-the register, however,enhancing therichness ofovertones andthereby the sense ofcompression. Thereare six pitches and PCs! within the compass of a minor 14th or augmented l3th!. The tritone is the greatest distance separatingany two of its components. The sonority is one of severe dissonance even though use of the pedal and the low register are calculated to counteract any explicitness ofpitch!, and the dissonanceis intensified by the density; the harmony contains tritonal relations G-c# and Gb-C! as well as semitonalrelations or their compound or inverted forms! to each ofthe threefactors inthe major triad at its center. Density-number in the piece as a whole, measured in simultaneities alone, progresses along the following consistent order: I - 2 - 3 - 4 - 6, punctuated of course byreversions tolower orders.It is impossible to escape the conclusion that these element-progressions aspects of density! are decisive in the structure.
1/nter//near independence
and interdependence
lMuch of the concernwith texture is directly involved with what is undoubtaedly themost fundamental and significant criterion of textural quality-the :relative independence and interdependence of its components.
Illustration has already been made of essential factorsby which interllinear relationsof independenceor its lack areexpressed: directional, intervallic, rand rhythmic corybrmity or disparity-all of these ofcourse concernedwith relattions among concurrent lines further subject to complementary or counter:active factors of dissonance, imitation motivic parallelism in temporal sseparation!, color,spatial distanceand compression,dynamic or articulative distinction, and any of other parametersof projection by which independence iis assertedor minimized between two or more components. Example 2-10 is presentedfor the readers consideration at this crucial jpoint. There should becareful scrutiny and evaluation of the various qualitaitive changeswhich occur in this beautiful movement,and of the contrapuntal values it embodies without prevalent imitation. Of course, directional conttrast is applied in degrees it cannot exist absolutelyl! so that it is comproimised as the counterpoint gets underway note relative intervallic and tdirectional-but not rhythmic-conformity of voices l, 2, and 4- atthe outset, 22A symbolization of density-compression in theWebcrn sonorityin questionwould be :22 pitches withinthe spaceof 22semitones!. Although compression within a portionof lthe total spatial fieldis, aswe havenoted, oftenvitally pertinent, distribution is relatively eeven here.
214 . Ili
certo Grosso in F minor, Op.6, No.3, thirdm
Q- E 1-ri-5' P5 Q ""==== V `V Sw Fai-fl VVE Qf JVV J* F *fr NV r'% r E; J
V- '
"T V
Vr
VV|'.| §.,V"
J
if o
V -VH Jr?
V J|- .J
all
-R ff
F
_ ,Fas
VV .IV
t9XtUf6 2'll'll!92 1.111111-i 21-1 = n1nv7i=.4ui 1i=i
-1 11-
1
fl
U lgr-1
-4
II MII ll '.Ll| J Y T1 2 Ill ""-l*- ' _92
215
jf »,
I
Q..Q
or between voices 1 and 2 in mm. 6-7!.23 Rhythmichomogeneity studiedbf is avoided except where functional necessities bring the voicesinto rhythmicaccord in recessive processes. The function of dissonanceshould be examined in the greatestpossible detail throughout the movement.The particular value of viewing dissonance as a textural quality is that this view induces one to seeall dissonancerelations. For example, the opening note of the second violin is seennot only as a suspended dissonance against the bass but in its relation -th! to the upper voice as well. This view of dissonance as a textural quality leads naturally to the conclusion that the intensigf ofdissonance #ict has todo inpart, and significantly,with the number ofsimultaneous dissonance relations withinthe texture as well aswith theirrelative severities. The mere computation of the number of such relations is, therefore, a significant analytical procedure in the evaluation of
dissonance effect and fluctuation.
Attention should be given to manipulations of texture in cadential approaches. At m. 9, for example, there is of course increased interlinear accord; but some vitality persists thisis characteristic of internal cadential expression inwell-made music!in the separation of voice l from the others, to all of which it is dissonant, and in the directional opposition of voices 2 and 4. In the final cadence, too, the complementarity of textural recession with those of tonality left open, one step short of the tonic!, harmony, and line is evident and balanced. Along with the tendency toward total interdependence oflines there are compensatoryevents, again expressing vitality within the cadential process:homorhythm is achieved gradually, preceded
by texturalgrouping of2; and considerable contrary motion andintervallic 2 23In numerous instances inthis chapterthe textural components willbe numberedfor convenience of reference; voice number l is alwaysthe uppermost.
216 texture
opposition are maintained. Again, dissonance exertsits influence of interlinear tension and resistance-especially the six-four and the diminished 7th are vital in this regard."
/mitat/on, a universal feature mu/t/Q0/e counterpo/nt
of many polyphonic sty/es;
The near universality of imitation in polyphonic styles in Western music and its frequency in homorhythmic, homophonic, and other textures! is evidenceenough of its paradoxical value in asserting the individuality of voices. It can be regarded evenas the supreme manifestationof interlinear independence, normallycomplementary tosimultaneous rhythmicand, often, directional opposition: the expression rj contrapuntalcompetition implicitin the enunciation fy" like motivicmaterial separated in time. Two textural components projecting like musical substance at a distance assert their relative independence byvirtue of the sqzaration-andconsequent diagonal interaction-of clearlyidentifiable materials in time. If interlinear interdependence accord! is the ideal of repose toward which textural process ultimatelystrives, imitation is the most persuasiveopposition to that tendency because of the explicit potential for parallelism denied by the imposition of a margin of temporal separation--the distance of imitation, the time interval. Thus, the shorter the time interval the more intense the con-
flict arising out of the contradiction of motivic affinity and temporal separation. Progressions in adjustment of the time interval are an important technique already demonstrated in the intensity-release curve of textural shaping. See Ex. 2-6b.! Actions of dissonance ofthe sort to which attention is called neednot be seen as purely, or even essentially, matters oftexture; dissonance is an issue ofrecurrent concern throughout thisbook. Butviewing dissonant relations asto their impact in textural shaping broadens, andcomplementarily sharpens, the understandingof this vital aspectof musical effect. The intensity associated withimitation requires clear distinction between that technique and dialogue in which twovoices exchange motivic material,the leaderdropping out while the follower responds, withno concurrentactivity. And the deviceof melodic sequence, inwhich motivic recurrence iswithin a single voice,differs fromboth imitation and dialoguein that it implies no texturalinteraction between voices. 2°The timeinterval of 0 is of coursethe ultimate accord, or resolution, ofimitative conflict pursuit!.Up to the pointof suchaccord, theassociation of progressive intensity with contraction ofthe timeinterval seems clearly appreciable;any valueother than0 expresses intensity greaterthan that of wider imitative distance. "Imitation, of course, isvery frequentlya strict duplication ofrhythms andintervals and directional successions; but every musician will recall instances inwhich imitativestatement duplicatesintervals onlyin oppositedirections, orrhythms in relative durationsbut longer or shorter values. As the time interval changes inimitation in augmentation or diminution, theangle of diagonal relation changes continuing in fluctuationof theintensity curve within this particular parameter.!
texture 217 The distance of imitation,
or time interval, is
thus seen as a factor in
textural intensity in the sense thatthe more distant the imitation the more leisurely the pursuit of one stratumby another; fluctuation in time interval is functional in many more contextsand stylesthan generally thought. The closer the imitation the smaller the time interval! the more intense is the competition; it is reasonableto assume,and experienceconfirms, that the awareness ofexplicit motivic affinity put out of joint by temporal discrepancy mustbe heightened by arelatively small margin ofdistance. This is the basis for the useby composers-demonstrated in countlessinstances-of stretto, often late in a given form, as part of the expression of intensity toward which textural structure, with other element-structures, progresses. A mental image of comparative relationscan be suggested inthis connection. Thus, the following changes areof strikingly different effect in the intensity-relaxation curve, the effect of eachreadily appreciable: two lines in simultaneity, one doubling the other; a separation of the two such that a diagonal relation, an imitative pursuit of one by the other, is established; a closerimitation, a narrower time interval. It seems impossible to doubt that the most intense of these situations,and the texture of highest urgency of expected resolution,is that in which the temporal disparity is close enough to give stark exposureto the fact of parity temporally denied. Fluctuations in intensity, occasionedlargely by changes inthe distances of imitation, are illustrated in graphic style in Exx. 2-1la,b,c, basedon three imitative sections from Bart6ks Quartet No. 5 for strings; these sections alternate in the Hrst movement with areas ofreduced textural complexity.
For an account of many important, prototypical musicalforms and procedures in which imitationis offundamental importance, and fordescriptive statements concerning the imitative proceduresinvolved, the reader is referred to the authors Form in Music: An Examination of Traditional Techniques of MusicalStructure and Their Application in Historicaland Contemporary Styles, 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ.: Prentice-Hall,Inc., 1986!,Chapter 12. Although the harmonic intervalof imitation, on the other hand, cannot easilybe seen asa factorin textural intensity it in no way determinesthe harmonicor otherinteractions at the pointsof interlinear vertical coincidence!,it is of courseprobably a significant aspect ofstyle; the preponderance, exceptions notwithstanding, ofimitative entriesat perfect intervals octave, 4th,Sth! in music upto recenttimes isevidence ofthis. While the memory and effect of] say, dissonantintervals of imitation are perhaps plausible aselements ofintensity, theharmonic effect of thisseems tobe superseded by that at the point of vertical coincidence-i. e.,the intervallicrelation betweenthe entering voice follower!and the leading voiceat thepoint ofentry iswhat is decisive, itwould seem, for intensityimpact ofinterlinear relation.Furthermore, adissonant harmonic interval of imitation doesnot find resolution ina consonant harmonic intervalof imitationin general practice. 20The Bartokquartets areextremely fruitfulsubjects ofstudy in this regard.See, for example, thefirst movementof the fourth quartet: mm. 8-13 or 14-26 as to progressive interlinear complexity,then release;or mm. 14-22 asto progressiveintensification inthe shrinking temporaldistance ofimitation; or mm. 27-37; etc.
2 78
texture
Ex. 2-11a. Barték, Quartet No. 5for strings,first movement. J: 138-132
ffl. '
J-f1,»mr 4 A119 A/'*92 H1 I1-11 1 |1 v1a|1111114111111111111115111
l/..1/11111111111111111111-_'jf' -_..1 ;92=1:111_I-I1l--l11**1- II'
3' ' I-l_-Arl-
I-1 111
-" 1
f!
1,1 2;=i;=-1 U11111111111111 1 ;= 11-11: |/..1/111| 1l 1:1 nw zgr 1- I
A
I_ IILI-I ru.: 1. ' |:|.41 1z.|:v11|:ru.| |31 11.111111 ||..1,1I1!111 |92f|1.1---_1 _1..---' 55 f!1-A_ - 11 r92t:,11»1 rw-r nu' .gvl 1 11.1 lI.¢||11 1191 1111111 ¢1.1:-:_-::1 1:_
A
----'I11 1 -»|-_111 1nnrnrrrru I' If 1' 1
A ,_
41%.r|.4z.1 v -.1u.|;|.glS¢_!1 _ l|1--_ .1
nv/ .u |||15111 __..-- s `
l§_li'/ rw-r|1111 lrr1l-r
f! 5
-_1
5
m.ll
J¢ 'v
mb v
$111.11
-
'
l;v'_=11
1:11111 --'--l1111t;§.";'f;|11 _1 1.11111_11l|11|1l11111:-|1|ll|!v'|1l1 1 _ |/..1r1 lI7"_f11|£'Y'1l.Y'l'l lI7'_1.Jll.P"ll! 11.01 l:v-1:11111 nv ' :gr-1 r' If , ' 1| £111 1711111 /I /92 ____ 4:!?Fll' 1 11 1 1 IILI- -I Ll Ily ._____ ln- !. |w:1l.1l1|1 |.A1|111 ;' .nu* i |IIIIQ--1-1 |..1-111 -ji..--' 111111
ff 5
41 A- u I rn11u ran 11.1 Z_-LI1 |1111 1111111 H
`5
I0 ll
111 rl r|.gu u I |.4.|1.|1111.1v1
- 17'
_ __ ' l.jlf
I 1? I1
12 13 »»1 1»
IJ 2J
Copyright 1936 by Universal Edition Renewed ; 963. Copyright and Renewal assigned to Boosey Co' Hawkes, Inc. for the U.S. A.!.
Ex. 2-11b. Bart6k,QuartetNo. 5 forstrings,first rnovernent.
15
17
18
........ Resolvent contractionto time interval0; in m. 16,prepares ostinato;in
rn. 18,prepares 1 simplificationwithin ostinato. 2 1
Copyright 1936byUniversal Edition;Renewed 1963. Copyright andRenewal assigned toBoosey
P Hnwkes, Inc. for theU. S.A.!.
Ex.2-11c. Bart6k,QuartetNo. 5 for strings,first movement. poco rit...
5$
a tempo
P1ZZ.
45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 56~ C & ~g ~C
Copyright 1936' Universal Edition;Renewed 1963. Copyright andRenewal assigned toBoosey
H Hawkes, Inc. fortheU. S.A.!.
texture 221
The illustrations show thefunction in a line of intensity of the degree andrate of changein the time interval in relation to measurenumbers acrossthe top of each graph, and, on the left, a scale oftemporal valuesexpressed innote durations!. The intensity line is of course drawn to connect points of change in the time interval. Most illustrated changesare progressivetoward a point of maximal shrinkage of the time interval; at other times there is fluctuation within relative stasis, broadly viewed. As often in comparable discussions, itis possibleto quote only minimal extracts from the work.! Multqble counterpointusually double, triple! is a particular species of imitation in which, if two or more different contrapuntal alignments are contiguous, linesof direct imitative relation intersect, asrepresented in Fig. 2-2.
Multiple counterpoint in which the treated motive is reduced in size in contrapuntal realignment can be somewhat analogousin effect to the procedures of contracted time
interval discussed above. Moreover,
vertical lineof coincidencee.g., from to
-
a shift in the
! alongwith
multiple contrapuntal realignment couldhave functionalconsequences analogous to those of time interval change. But variation among appearances of motivic factors in multiple inverted! counterpoint such that their vertical coincidences arealtered isrelatively rare. Nevertheless, multiplecounterpoint is in the simple fact of contrapuntal realignment in vertical distribution an important technique of textural variation. Fig. 2-2. Diagonal imitative interaction
" '- -' Triangular
imitative interaction
-li interaction
Intersecting imitative as in multiple counterpoint!
3°The intervalof inversionis probablya usefulcriterion of style distinction,but the extent to which this may generallybe true is uncertain. Certainly, the preponderance of examples before the twentiethcentury areat the 15th oroctave!, 12th,or 10th-intervals of inversionin which dissonance-consonance relations are relatively not entirely! stable.
222 texture The act/'vat/'on of s/mp/e textures
Musical textures are often activated by dynamic, articulative, rhythmic, coloristic, and other means.Techniques oftextural activation are applicable, of course,to any circumstances, butthey are of specialimportance in vitalizing relatively simple, fixed textures. A few examples aregiven here in illustration of some of the primary techniques oftextural activation; others will appear in subsequent analyses. The works of Stravinsky are particularly replete with examples of heterophonic, heterorhythmic, dynamic, articulative, and other types of activation within simpler, often inert, textures. We shall return to some of these questions in analysis of Stravinsky, referring for the moment to an excerpt from one of the symphonies Ex.2-12!. This extremely vital passageconsists oftwo major textural complexes in an antiphonal relation, both chordal and considerably homorhythmic. Neither undergoes significant qualitative change: one of them, that in the higher brass and woodwinds, is only moderately progressivein harmonic, melodic, chromatic content; the other, that of the strings, is static and its influence extendsinto the wind sonorities, where low brass andhorns persist in the pedal A!. Within this fundamental context of considerable tonaltextural inertia, an extremely important range of activating devices isinterposed. The heterophonic doubling heterointervallic, homodirectional! between, forexample, thetwo bassoonsor cello and viola! in the first quoted bar B in one, C in the other! gives a momentary, particular thrust to the ostinato pattern. A comparable heterophony occursin the next bar between double-bass andtimpani. The same techniqueis seenat other points-e.g.,
Provocative questions would beconcerned withthe extentto whichgeneral statements might bepossible incharacterizing practice within givenstylistic contexts. It can be suggested that in tonal counterpoint,that of the eighteenthand nineteenthcenturies, theintervals of inversion octave,15th, 12th,10th mostcommonly! arerelatively predictable within a given range. To what extent and how! is this true of earlier styles?To what extent are the resources extended in morerecent styles ? Theanswer tothe secondof thesequestions would seem perhaps more apparentthan it actually is: inversion atthe octaveand itsmultiples, and at the 12th and 10th, is surprisinglyfrequent in many twentieth-centuryliteratures, perhaps forreasons of the relativestability ofintervallic relations.The intervalof inversionis an important textural factor since it determines theimmediate intervallicrelations among concurrent voices. "A common device intraditional music,especially thatof the eighteenth andnineteenth centuries, is the local arpeggiation qf chordal factors eitherin purely chordal texturesor in texturesin which there ischordal, or partially chordal, accompaniment toa dominant thematic element. The Albertibass, ubiquitous particularly in the latereighteenth and earlier nineteenth centuries, is acharacteristic, exceedingly familiar exampleof this.One mightsay that rhythmic activation vitalization!of this and comparablekinds actstoward compensation for the absence of functionaltextural eventfulness.
Ex.2-12. Stravinsky,Symphonyin ThreeMovements, first movement.
2 Fls. 2 Oba 2 Cls. inBb Bass Ci. inIL
4 Hns.
2T ts.
in Tuba
Vln.
Vc.
Ex. 2-12 continued. 2 Fls. 2 Obs. 2 Cls. inBb Bass Cl. inB'~
2 Bns.
4 Hns.
5 Tpts. inC
Tuba
T imp.
Piano
Vln.
Via. Vc. Db Reprinted bypermission of Belurin-MillsPublishing Corporation.
texture. 225
two barslater betweendouble-bass andcello andin the following bar between the samelines. Heterorhythmic relations amongpiano, timpani, low woodwinds, and strings, in the ostinato texture of which they are components, mustalso be cited. A further
factor in
the activation
of both textural divisions
is the
enormously resourcefuland potent array of dynamic-articulative emphases devices of orchestration, of coloration!: the frequent, heavy doubling of A in violin and viola, the low octaves ofthe piano, or frequent marks of articulative stress.
'The entire fabric is, then, oneof relative textural simplicity, directness, and minimal textural or tonal, harmonic, or melodic! development-a context in which devicesof the sort describedfunction significantly in compematogw activation oflargely inert textures.° In Ex. 2-l3a, also from Stravinsky, a number of points will be seen as of crucial importance and significance in the analysis of texture. Concern isdirected hereto identifying and appraisingdevices bywhich textures are activated in atmospheres ofrelative inertia of essential pitch structures." Looking at the example very generally one can seethat the texture is layered in groups of components differently colored, each qualitatively distinct, so that the entire context can perhaps be said to consist not merely of linear components i.e., of lines! but of somewhat distinct subtextures in a polytextural complex. Tonal relations among thesestrata shouldbe consideredindependently -the various implications of the PC materials ofseparate texturalcomplexes, the essentialharmonic structure within each stratum see for instance Ex. 2-l3f!, the general C-emphasisof mm. 324-26, and the final harmony as a tonal synthesis of the preceding PC elements. Beyond tonal-harmonic interactions and relations, subtextural complexes aresharply contrastedin rhythmic and articulative character, coloration, and diatonic asopposed torelatively chromatic content; the interaction among thesecomplexes isthus oneof vital counterpoint. Activating elements within each .stratum-complex, especially vital in a context in which tonalharmonic succession is restricted, are heterorhythmic and heterophonic impulses within bass doublings cf Ex. 2-12!, points of imitation, and various techniquesof ornamentation within each stratum. Specific examples of techniquesof activation which are of vital effect butwhich do not materially affect qualitative textural content are given in extensions of Ex. 2-13. All occur in the opening measures ofCanticum, PartV 2-l3b,c,d,e!.
Attention is called toan interestingand insightfuldiscussion of Stravinskys textures: Edward T. Cone, Stravinsky: The Progressof a Method, in PerspectivesNew qf Music,I 962!, pp. 18-26.
texture Ex. 2-13a. Stravinsky,CanticumSacrum,PartV llli autemprofecti!; text from the Vulgate, St. Mark 16:20.
Chorus
2 Bns. Cbn. +
Tpts. inC
Sass
Tmbs. Bas ~Contrab
Vla. Db.i ~Soundingoneoctavelower.
texture 227
m.325
__ E
§§ §J§;;L?§;§
1/ 0?
wma'
" 3* 47' VP
_h
____
-==°=¢ u Poco
_
meno mosso, J =108
- __ ?
3_
92 'l
-Q? 3
nonf Stmpft , zfgm
II-rvli l--1 *Ul1 'I ini' 7' r t$ i1||1 1 92 1|
` ' -l 4-"_11
- *"'°°"° 1 ff
';,_"'i;:° ":_;:*""-' '""'° __ ______ ____ """`"''=l __________ "' ____i__l
Zll'|l.~ lui nn-v14. una; In - 'r§1~I Q 92_./
`.¢»__°"'°"
-_
'___ _
texture
228 Ex. 2-13a
tmued.
nm. °~=-~ . JVfi4F Q J; 1' '
llll.
l *
~ 5; -
:f % __ f_ E§
Bm _ {_ g 1% -; `f
mb.
F °=- $
V" T JJ
,
Q2 4
4
P1
- 1
_/'STI
IGXIUI6
Adagio,J 66 = 342
S. &¢r4"" _ {p -
Chor. A
I_
,1
--
|'H
-r
-n
men.
T°
»+ -
B. Q
{P_ l
,
I~ -|
7e ff°'**Tif ___ _____{P_;
A few
-n
°'f_ __ ~T
fL
M
_Q
Adag'io,J=66 -A_
Qii' __= * --{?-~
Tmbs.
H
if
_I _ ___W_____
Eg '
='-he § ' '°':;` l
V
=?-;- i__+ ~-
Y l| ,Ill i nn J un; li? 1 |I "go: .t 1.1|u.|| |1iIII_¢n 'li §"2 Vla. es cnivo div.2! inI{ qisillil PP' l"lUlUiQE1 i- _1| ' _ , 11: ||11 ..t tlulu _' iam/ T'_ll1 92./ r ¢Z _,xi ._. ;4-L U9292I92l H 1 T211Z-¢ Db. 3:3111 pespresswo
_ _;_in , QQ 192 1 ,1,?__ -O92 1i 1; __ -
_Q '
d bypermission. Renewed 1984. Copyright 1956 by Boosey Cd Hawkes, Inc. Reprinte
230 texture Ion as to essentiallinear movements!, Ex. 2-13b. Activation by articulative differentiat a species ofheterorhythm.
IIL307 Bn_12
9- -4 5f
_
"°"~f;` 1; m.3oa r92*'**"*Zl`l".-I Bass Tmb.:L-¥=--$$"'
mb. E3
E
*Sounding pitch. Ex. 2-13c. Activation by displacements transferences! of PCs and pitcheswithin a subtextural complex.
Jk
JJJJ
* Tpts.
II
*All at sounding pitch. Activated chord:
1 L_
924444 4444!
texture
231
The above techniques recurrent in a movement of rondo-like restatementsof thematic material! are motivating in contextsof counteractive forces of inertia, as are those illustrated in Exx. 2-13d and 2-13e.
Ex.2-13d. Heterorhythrnic techniquein the Stravinskyall notationat soundingpitch!.
Cb.Tmb.
Db.
Ex. 2-13e. Heterorhythmic and heterophonictechniquein the Stravinsky.
2 Bn.
Cbn.
The rhythmic activation of a textural massof considerabletonal-harmonic inertia is evident as well in the organ solo Ex. 2-13a, mm. 327 35! by which the Illi autemprofectiis punctuated. A sketchof its pitch content is given in Ex. 2-13f, one in which its relative tonal stasis is clear. The texture is
Ex. 2-13f. Relativetonal-harmonicinertia of rhythmicallyactivatedtextural massin the Stravinsky summaryof structuralpitchesand neighborauxiliaries!.
232 texture
moved within a limited tonal-harmonic spaceby devicesof rhythm, diH`erences ofornamentation of the fundamentally fixed structure, and the like. The techniques of controlled textural change and activation of relatively inert textural massesby such devices asthose noted are of recurrent importance in Stravinsky, whoseworks, as suggested inthe foregoing references, -are invariably rewarding objects ofstudy of shaping aspectsof texture. Analysis of the examplefrom CanticumSacrum wouldideally go much further than hasbeen possiblehere, reachinginto other areas ofinquiry: for instance, the transfer of harmonic elements over boundaries in the form where there is
severe and abrupt quantitative change e.g., between mm. 311 and 312!, where polytextural structure gives way to monotexture with corresponding pitch factors transferred from soprano and alto voices to the organ, right hand, thoseof tenor and bassto the organ, left hand, and that of the second trombone to the organ pedal in palpable links of pitch material bridging, in counteractive functions, severe textural and color changes. Throughout the Canticum andother works of Stravinsky complementarity of such elementsas dynamics and tempo not to mention others! at points oftextural changeare of great importance.
The comp/ementary and compensatory dispositions or texture in re/ation to other element-structures
Some examplestreated earlierhave takenup the questions ofcomplementary and compensatoryinteractions amongelement-events infunctional contexts. For example, Ex. 1-37 is a discussion ofcadential processin one of the piano pieces of Schoenbergs Op. 23 see pp. 107-9!. In that analysis a point is made of textural recessionas one aspect of that process: texture is reduced in density and qualitatively simplified in the closing bars. The substantial thickness accruing in the broad course of the piece out of the lean two-
voice texture of m.1! isfinally reduced to L, thenl. In the qualitativesense, l
the insistent imitative interlinear interactions of, for example, mm. 18-19 are slowly relinquished. These interactions among textural components continue into mm. 20-21 but in less explicitforms; and in the final two bars
they dissolve almost totally.There isthus agradual qualitativeas wellas quantitative! textural recession. The element-structures involving linear descent, rhythmic deceleration slowing tempo aswell as longer durations!, dynamics, articulation A then - , as well as longer legatounits!, and meter as to recessively longerdurations of event groupings! all functioncomplementaribi. In Chapter l it was noted that a compensatory factor in prevailing decline isthe maintenanceof considerabledissonance ininterlinear relations; but here too a recessive curvewas noted as a probability.
texture 233 A choral
work of Purcell, a double canon in which
there are concur-
rently two mirror canons at the gl !and two strict canons at the o!, is helpful in analysis ofcomplementary functionsin another instance ofcadential process.
Measures ll-13 of the Purcell piece Ex. 2-14! represent an area of maximal textural vitality" in the broad context of the entire work. ProgresEx. 2-14. Purcell, Gloria Patrl m.ll I ; in nt , ` |1l= _ _ i¥§--I1 1 __ -i :nt U't;1 lIi.`h -. 'M _ _' _ _ |-¢!_m92`_-I uni; 1 1 ~_ _ I 92 'igx 1' ~9 2'"_'_lv 9292 I/ /, 9292 - lo- rum, se - cu- ,, sc cu - -rum, A /H- men,A men, se cu - I 92 92 I/ ' 1 92» !f_ , I i-' _" ' ' "_ 92 92_, ' _ _ 92_ /'92 __ 9292 -1 l nF.£,i.1i=:=§=i|-1..i wil y; il; §=1»1=l1£n:r1'=1 = 9292l'- -I-'I~./_-_ Y'll!1!*lF'F$ 1il1`;Ql|1 -'_-|13 f'l" ln __ 92 92 _ 9292 / , 92, 92_ 92 92 se-cu-la se-cu-lo-rum, se-Cullo ,/' , se-Eu92l m,A men,A _92 / . `92 92 _ / _ ` c _ 9292 _ _ _ _ -'||I- ' 92 _ I' -1 2 '5I Q _ |$-1|1 itll# _" 1.1711 1in1»1 iii 31_ .' l. _ Q' __ s '. ` 92 / _ 92 et in se- _cu secu- _Iorum, , se-c`u~_ lo-rum, A__ -.la se-cu- lo- rum /. 92 _` `92_/_ I 92 92 _
._ W_ _L °"""'~__-;'i' __A-;"* _ :_ " V _VI__II-I !-.!ll-_-._*7'|-_ 1, sem-per et
in se
=| i| 1' ll -_Zin 1 T- _ /1, _ ! _ _ __ I ll - cu- la se-cu-lo-rum, se - cu- lo-rum, se cu - -
33The setof terms adopted forclassification ofinterlinear relations on the basis of directional, rhythmic,and intervallic aspects! canalso beused todenote variouskinds of imitative relation,with the term imitationunderstood asgeneric andsubject to modifiers of many kinds. The term homodirectional imitation opposed to contradirectional! thus refersto interaction in which leaderand followerconform inthe directionof motion,a situationfor which nogenerally accepted term isavailable. Homodirectionalimitation may or may not conform intervallicallyand rhythmically; hyphenated termshere too are usefulin comprehensive denotation.! Contrarhythmic imitation can ofcourse befurther qualifiedby common terminology in augmentation-usually understood as homointervallic,rhythmically free, etc.! and contraintervallic imitation issubject tofurther, more precise standardclassification tonal, by complementation, by compound expansion, etc.!. Free imitationor the term _/ieeb: imitative! may denote imitativeprocedure inwhich the aspect ofrelation Huctuatesat somestated level. The commonterm strictimitation can denote likereal imitation! a followerwhich conformsin all aspectsexcept, usually,that of PC e.g., imitation at the 5th!. Mirror imitation is homointervallic andhomorhythrnic, butcontradirectional, andthe term rhythmic imitation, or rhythmic canon, identifiedby specificreference toa singleparameter, isunderstood to exclude theothers i.e.,to pertain to imitation which conformsin rhythm only!; this is suggestive offurther plausibleterms likecontour imitation directional conformityonly! and pitch-class imitation or interval-class imitation as in manyserial procedures!. One couldcontinue in this process ofdeveloping andclassifying termsof potential usefulness, incorporating language incommon usage wherever ithas generallyunderstood meaning. It is unlikely that any setof simpleterms canor shouldbe intendedas applicableto all situations; descriptive adjectival modifiersare almostinvariably usefulexcept insimplest conditions. 34 The Gloriais includedin Wallace Berry and Edward Chudacoff,Eighteenth-Century imitative Counterpoint Englewood Cliffs,N._].: Prentice-Hall,Inc., 1969!, pp. 36-37. The reader isencouraged to trace progressive actions towardmm. ll-13.
234 texture Ex. 2-14 continued. m.l4
»- f V is ~lo92 - 92 rum,
nT A-
'QH men, Ji?A
men, A- -
+__._9292 !
as e men, et _,lvl i
_1 e Z . 9292 =_=.,,=;r_z;_ n .2?-=
Jl
--
- men.
tj' 92 V6
in se - cu la se - cu- lo- rum,A "" ' 92 /_92
H
- men.
e -»~ - °" f
I men, A -
men, se - cu - *lo
rum, A
I1 ~°:;:"___°____L:_i ,
kall 92 N -lo -rum, _E' , A - men,A -
- men. _
men, A
9292|§ H - men.
Reprinted from The Worksof Henry Purcell, A.Lewis andN. Fortune,eds., by permission qf thepublisher, Novello and Co., Ltd.
sion to that area is characterizedby complementarysuccessions withintonal, linear, and rhythmic structures, and by progressive developmentwithin qualitative textural parameters aswell, culminating in the complex web of interactions at highly contracted temporal distances! partly shown in Ex. 2-14. Following this climactic area a number of recessive tendencies, in com-
plementary function, can be noted immediately: the final dissolution of the canons and of imitative interaction constitutes textural simplification of important consequence, although considerable contrarhythmic activity persists, with voices settlingat differing times exceptfor the homorhythmic relation of inner voices; the important rhythmic deceleration can be seen at a glance, as can the recessive, gradual essentially conjunct! descent of upper voice against modestascent ofthe lowest-tendencies in which their canonic followers of course concur. A complementary factor which is less obvious is metric, and can be seen, for example, in the settings of Amenin the outer voices: thoseof the soprano having recessive valuesof 3, then 5,
then IOJ5, and those of the bassthen 3, then 5, 14.° Example 2-15is an illustration of quantitative recessionin which qualitative factors are only negligibly affected and in which the sense ofdecline is extreme!. With a situation of tonal stasis,the exampleshows howpersuasive a textural recession,with complementary dynamic and melodic decline, can be.
Of course, with the linear sloping of the upper voice, it is clear that the recessionis one of texture-spaceas well as density.The function of dissonance as a textural factor in interlinear independence is particularly
IEXIUIB
235
Ex. 2-15. Barték, Divertimento for stringorchestra, second movement.
,"" " » tr 5
.fi
Vln.1
-=
.. '
jf dim.
Q"1""=1.=-?=E
.... K
jf dim.
__
__ _
...----- "{
a I
dim. __.._
@
_ #4
_
§gn5%F:§_g§i% Ejf dim.-` ---- -"/% ,J Q Edim, J_...Lp jf m.46 vw
,J
.`/_ -
7 d____ ;== ii
i-
lGi=i2|»5" _ 5X,gl¥' 4..5pl ____ `4'= _ ___ 92/IJlFI7F _M,92~ ._
- I- 4 -r-
i
i
'I
17; on
con sord.
' EJ
J
M
__ .1 J ---==-== -» » »-fr";'1i= i
92 `_/
1 it
___t l
2l_|;r' |_-;r'_|Y I
3
Copyright 1940 by Hawkes éi Son London! Ltd.;Renewed 1967. Reprinted by permission of Boosey 6'Hawkes, Inc.
236 texture
apparent: the directional opposition of lines throughout the passage adetermined one, the two essential componentsin an oblique relation! is intensely complemented by a bitonal situation, the two tonal implications merging at the point of resolution. The texture is, strictly speaking, onlyvery incidentally of morethan two real components, and both remain active until the cadential measure, 49. In that sense thedecline is of sonority as well as densityand space.Textural qualiyf thus remains relatively constant and insistent while the range of doublings is gradually reduced. The disappearanceof the lower stratum of the first violin mm. 44-45! does, however, constitutea true modification in textural quality as distinct from the sonority or density by which the most severe shapingof the succession is carried out. There could scarcely bea more lucid example of the complementarity of elements thanthis in which density, space, sonority,dynamic coloration, melodic lines,and tonal relations achieve by their confluence apervasive anddetermined functional effect. We are thus cognizant here, as in parallel studies of other elementprogressions andelement-structures, ofthe necessitythat texture be seenin its relations to other elements. Wheresuch relations are not made explicit, the reader is urged to posefor himself the appropriate questions andto seek their answersto the extent that other elementsare functionalin the particular processive contexts observed.
Some textural
functions in
de//heat/on of form
Texture its class, itsqualities, itsdensities! isofcourse anessential elementby which thematic
statement is rendered distinctive
and expressive. For exam-
ple, the stark exposureof a line to be emphasizedin and for itself may be in monophonic orvery restricted,transparent texture.Or, the contrasts between opposing thematic elements orgroups within a sonata or rondo movement are often textural, with textural differences playing a role analogous and complementary to that of changes of tonal reference, dynamic level, and the like. Or controlled textural diversification may be an important variational feature within a single thematicunity, and among itsconstituent parts. Textural progressionand alteration are of course fundamentaltechniques in thematic developmentas inany manipulation of motivic materials in a generally unstable contextf Among earlier literatures, one of the most beautiful and often cited! examples which come to mind in illustration of the principle of textural
texture 237
change functioningin delineation of form is _]osquins Tu pauperumrqfugium, subject of extensive earlier comment see pp. 45-47!. The brief extracts quoted Ex. 2-16! show the strong textural differences bywhich, along with contrasts of tonal order and other kinds, sections in this recapitulative form are contrasted. The first part, only a portion of which is quoted, is in itself a very expressively disposed textural progression setting out from a condition of perfect homorhythm which, in the second phrase, is gradually diversified with tentative imitative interactions, then largely restored in the brief third and fourth phrases not quoted!. Complementary to change in cadential pitch upper voice: g, a, g; bass voice: e, A, e! is this sloped succession shapedby textural diversification and resolution.
The second part elided with m. 20! is contrapuntally imitative, although imitation is not predominant. Actually, imitation enters very subtly within the restricted diversification of the fifth phrase, not quoted.! At the same timethat imitative contrapuntal texture emerges, incontrast to the first part, density is reduced to a consistent two-voice pattern with contrasts of coloration!. The reduction in density serves the function of contrast and brings into vivid exposure the interactions of increasingly independent voices. The third section isrecapitulative, yet a variation note metric change, for example!, a technique in which Josquin showsadmirable inventiveness. Its beginning is quoted in Ex. 2-16; the homorhythmic condition and the thematic material! of the opening return here. A look into the complete example will reveal subsequent return and variation of the third and fourth phrases. Thesection precedingm. 60 is, in fact, one in which the composerdemonstrates aconsummate craftof remarkable economyin which the simple motivic substanceof the original phrase 3 mm. 12-13, with anacrusis! is widely extended anddeveloped. Throughout this process,as in the varied return of the opening two phrases,there prevails an attitude of essentialhomorhythm in which there is occasional,restrained diversification by slight rhythmic differentiations usually productive of animating dissonance. The premature cadence onthe modal final, E, at m. 60 setsoff a closing section of cadential elaboration again marked by significant qualitative contrast of texture. In opposition to the preceding, there are now once again points of controlled imitative interplay on earlier motives! by which the texture is rendered contrapuntal in modest complexity opposing the stabler, predominantly chordal textures of Parts I and III! 3Apel and Davison, cds.,Historical Anthology of Music,Vol. I Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1949!, p.92.
238
t6XfUl'6 -.
Josquin, Tupauperum refugium, from Motet,Magnus estu, Domine.
; ii -J
E E51? ';, ° 5 ? gf Qi 5 #12 |
5, fi f; U f m tu lan-
-wi;-mmj;
é QP?§ffE '
J
T4 la -bo -ra.n s-ti -
L; .ILA um, vi
--
-a
J
/¢, m,,° Tf f' 92
°I
II
5T 1f_ II
II
f8Xl'UI'&
JI-|:|
239
i
- C88
MQ# H '¢t
et
Ea _i'_'
#gr-f; 'LF' 8 Em# Jw-# J.
'O'
- tor
Do
- mi
- ne,
3.
ob -
dor -
Q
?
mi
Diff? 9? i ;1 i%' ob -
dor -
mi -
at
in mor-
-
i ? H5 %
0
- or-mi
in
240 texture Ex. 2-16 continued.
T 1?T T V T' _ te la; -
T T' J-
C4 Ji
-ma
_'
A, -L; 'é-__?l`;gll_i92_g _F T T -.f-'
92e=Q'
Fiiliuiv F `=i=»f_ `;=s=
Copyright 1946, 1949, by the President and Fellows of Harvard College. Reprinted by permission of HarvardUniversity Press.
Dominant textural projection is thus one of chordal homorhythm activated by diversification subtly and often gradually introduced and sub-
sequently relinquished, the overalltextural structuremarked bythe change to imitative counterpoint in mm. 20-33 and, to a lesserextent, in the last
nine measures. The mannerin whichJosquin anticipates the texturalquality of the Hrst of these contrasting passages notablyin the second and fifth phrases! isfurther testimony to the subtlety andcontrol by which his creative procedures are characterized.
Any discussionof texture in the delineation of form and forms! could
of coursego on indefinitely. Theprocedures by which, in so manyprototypical designs,formal delineation is one of relativebfuncomplicated texture in thematic statement setagainst subsequent relatively diversified, sometimes intense, textural activity in developmental andvariational processes,will be recalled by any experiencedlistener.
texture 241
And the literature is full of examples ofvariation forms in which progressions andchanges within the textural element are basic techniques. Examples of variation sets are an extremely fertile resource forstudy of textural progression and recessionwithin individual variations and along the broad lines of overall form encompassingthe variation series. Of course, textural events
at all levels of structure are
of inestimable
significance in the delineation and processive molding of all prototypical forms, and of all forms beforeand following the tonal period except of course where texture is limited to monophony without even the implications of contrapuntal diversity and interaction in compound melodic line. Textural stasis, progression,recession, andvariation are basic in the functional processes bywhich forms are shaped,and by which expressivefunctional events climactic, cadential, introductory, expository, etc.! are projected see Ex. 2-27!.
Textural contrasts, more broadly defined, do of course serve as well the function of broad delineation, in complementarity with other relevant elements, in intermovement relations
in multimovement
forms, and in inter-
sectional relations in highly diversified forms. This factor is perhaps too obvious to require illustration think, in one of countless manifestationsof the principle, of the Baroque suiteand the role of texture in the traditional distinctions among dance movements-between, say, the sarabande and the gigue!.
Textural processes /'n progress/'on toward intensity, in recession toward cadence, and in antic/Qoation of thematic statement
A factor of which a good deal of mention has already been made, that of the shapingof textural change inthe direction of increasedintensity in which other element-structuresoften work complementarily! is carried further in amplification of earlier referenceto ajosquin setting of the Deprtjundis. The De prqfundis settinghad prior reference pp. 87-90! in connection with linear
melodic and harmonic functions
at its outset, as well as limited
discussion oftextural processespp. 189-90! in a different extract. The ,portion of this magnificentwork quoted as Ex.2-17 isclimactic, and its climactic effect might well be said to be moresigng/icantlyproduct a oftextural progression See cantusfirmus, ostinato,and other textural proceduresdiscussed asvariation technique in Robert U. Nelson, The Technique of Variation LosAngeles: University of California Press, 1948!, pp.l0fl`., 51f.,58-60, etc.,and Berry,Form inMusic, Chapters 8 and 9.
texture Ex. 2-17. Josquin,Deprofundis.
Do - mi - ne,quis su-sti-ne - bit?
Do
mi - nc, quis su- sti- ne- bit?
Sland2 J
m.47
/ ti> J!
Oo u! Reprinted from theSmijersedition,published by G. Alsbach andCo.,Amsterdam, bypermission of Creyghton Musicology-Musica Antiqua,Bilthoven, Netherlands.
texture 243
than of any othersingle factor,although of course complementary rhythmic acceleration and tonal progression play important roles." In mm. 42-54 decisive factors in the texture are noted graphically in the quoted example. The time intervals of imitation are progressivelyshorter within the section as well as between this and earlier
sections: the imitative
entriesthe of words Domine,sustinebit? quis initially are oat,then at J and
againo;atand the rising quarter-note motive,§ J J ,l s , the motivic drop of a minor 3rd, ,
and the cadential drop
of a 2nd in half-notes, participate in a number of brief but functionally
important imitative interactions mm. in 45-46 at time intervalso,dof ,
and ,das indicated theinexample. Finally, in mm. 51-52, which are highly animated rhythmically and texturally, but not harmonicalbf;note the compensatory stasisof harmonic rhythm!, imitative interactions among appearances of the dotted-rhythm
motive appear in anumberinterlinear of relations at the distance _ The J textural complexity is enhanced here, moreover, by contradirectional imitation.
This section is the only one to this point in which four independent voices emerge incontrarhythmic activity. They do so earlyin m. 44, two bars after the passageis launched in the imitative entries described above. There is,
of course,interim release in, for example, the2 texture of sucha point as 1
m. 49, where the internal voices become interdependent homorhythmic, homodirectional, almost homointervallic!, or even simply in the withdrawal of the tenor at mm. 45-47 or the superius at mm. 48-49. Such factors as the relatively stable bassusat mm. 50-51 cf. the bassrole of later styles,and its restrictive submission tothe demandsof tonal-harmonic support, cadential definition, etc.!, the rhythmic identification but directional opposition! of tenor and bassus atm. 52, and the ultimate interdependence of voices to which the textural structure finally turns at not long in advance of ! the cadence arevital factorsof recessive process. The progressiondescribed above,and the larger higher-level! progression of which it is a culmination, are thus excellentlyillustrative of a number of the textural factors with which we are concerned. And this highly active, diverse, andcomplex passage, the section of greatest vitaligv inthe motet, is thrown into perfectly calculated perspective by the almost entirely homorhythmic phrases whichfollow-the only such phrasesin the entire piece, significant "The sectionof maximalintensity oftextural interactionand diversificationEx. 2-17! has anear parallelin the final sectionof thepiece, towhich theinterested reader might refer for comparativestudy.
244 texture
in their maintenance of the full density of four components presented in the forms simplest textures.
The reduction in textural quantity density! as a cadential processis surprisingly rare in music, especially where Final cadenceis concerned; it is morecommon in tentative, internal cadence, especiallywhen suchinternal cadence ispreparatory to important events to follow, when such events, as often with thematic exposition, call forth relatively full densities see the Brahms reference,Ex. 2-20!. The procedure of final cadence, wheredensity of texture is concerned, seems generallyto favor the emphatic substance ofquantity, as a rule, while com/Jensatovy action of recessive simpwication withinqualitative texturalparameters takes place. Musical examplesshow that greater densityof compressionas well as of number is
characteristic of
many cadential
formulations; relatively
ample density-number is, especiallyin tonal contexts, of value in the clarity of consonant tonal-harmonic function at its most crucial point, that of resolution, and increaseddensity-compression is a usualconsequence ofdescent ofupper lines. Nevertheless,examples canbe found in which dissolution of density is an important aspect ofrecessive, finalcadential process. A cadence from Sessionspiano work From My Diary Ex. 2-18! shows a functional, comparable density-numberrecession 4-3-2 as well as acontraction of texture-space from29 to 13 semitones. Ex. 2-18. Sessions, From My Diary,third movement.
Larghissimo emisterioso J= 32!
EH' 2
tempo _
Auf; fm
`*
fa g
morendo g
1*-U rfb P 'ii
g_
tr--D lr' _-T""=lf-'Z
Reprinted permission by Edward of B.Marks MusicCorp.
Even wheretextural qualitative complexity is maintained to a very late point, cadential expression maydepend altogetherin its preparatory stages,
and predominantly in itsultimate formulation, on processes of declinewithin other elements. In such circumstances cadentialfeeling is often relatively indccisive.
Example 2-19 is memorable in the twentieth-century concerto literature, and an excellentexample forstudy ofthe actions oftexture in cadence, in this instance thefinal cadenceof a movement. Themotive stated twice by
texture 245 Ex. 2-19. Schoenberg, Concerto, Op. 36,for violinand orchestra, first movement. Lento J 76! m.263
;=.:=.:_ -
1- 1,iE; y con sord. 'V
Hn.1 '
Tuba- 92
si _
rn
JE
__=;_ __
n
- +--f e__==____
"
_
l g`
/' -M/J
~ nv , ,_
Timp. -
'."
rP
rx
'g lf?-
_.
,_
5*
f ______"`
'
'° `
1.2. _
I _F: W //I .
In 'V__ sgmdo
" -e-e,I if
'
'_ 5:'__ " ___' --3
via E- Q iii? ii*
pp
_l/J
e
_M A
' ` ='
1 Ql g . J 5 P* 1
'l
ff i
' pizz.
' ,_
E ,QE
I
ma .ta *All parts sound aswritten. Copyright 1939 by G.Schirmer, Inc. Reprinted by permission.
the soloviolin is of primary importance in the entire movement. The radiation of its influence throughout the texture is seen in the indications drawn into the example: there are varied imitations of the characteristic semitonal succession inalmost every instrumental part. The significant and necessary
observation which must followis thattextural complexity, activity, incipient and abortive but impressive interlinear independence, and considerable density are compensated forby the lack or tight restrictionof linear, harmonic, and tonalactivity andespecially the complementary decline in tempoand dynamics. Cadence isoften, aswe havestated, markedby realization of substantial density, evenits full potential in the particular context, with other elements expressive ofcompensatory recessive stasis anddecline. In the Schoenberg
this principle goes beyondthe accrual of density useful for emphatic effect! in a texture active withinterlinear, diagonal relations Q'a considerable number, while the stasis of relatively leve1 lines, dynamic reduction, and gradually slower tempo to the point of fermata are of`essential recessive function, dominant in their overall eH`ecteven in the atmosphere of a cadence
ultimately slightlytentative infeeling. Tothe extentthat textural activity persists, cadentiallinality is qualified.!
246 texture
Of course the deliberate minimizing of the natural tendencies ofinterlinear interdependenceat the point of internal cadenceor preliminary stages in Hnal cadence! isa universal and vital technique of texture for suppressing too punctuative or conclusive an effect. And the device of preparation the process ofestablishing anticipatory atmosphere! can be an important factor in internal cadential process, usually involving dissonance ina universal technique by which expectation is heightened. The shaping of texture is requisite and critical to cadential and anticipatory effect in Ex. 2-20. The pattern is of course oneof decline in density-number, a very common technique by which anticipatory feeling is enhanced complemented by the unsettled tonal-harmonic situation! in preparation for resumption of thematic statement.
Measure 64 is a point of maximal density: the texture is essentially chordal, strongly activated by heterorhythm and intertransference ofchordal PC factorsamong very active voices.Dynamic level jbrtissimo! andextremes of pitch texture-space! are of complementary effect. Quantitative release begins in the following bar; there is somediversification here-a qualitative
Ex. 2-20. Brahms, Quintetin G, Op. 111, for two violins, two violas, and cello, second lT920V6lT928l1t.
Vln.l
/m.63 /-` Ad X ,X /_92 92
Vln.2
é on
N g Ag g/` ,`
CZ?! X F _ /,_ 1
" /._
/` `
Vla.l _ Vla.2
C1722 ` Vc;
.n
V'
.1 .11 .1
Common modification density reduction, simplification! of texture jbllowing cadential punctuation, one ofthedevices bywhich cadence is throwninto reliefand resumption announced, is anomnipresent factor in music.
texture 247
m.64 /A
nw /`
.J A ZVH 1.1.' ll.. Luv
'92
;/`; FJ _/` '9292
A
-Y| I
~~
A /$
=~;~ i
92 /1
_
,-5 _
5
wr'
_ :=. `ii;§
_
``
92
9. .22
EL F"-5
== , ?"T' 3 ° 3 3 3*
i W5
92 J'
m.65 _;, _
J
f _tiv
92, __,
in TJ f "/PR ii?= ==» 3 f'
fP
e f fJP
7
!
J kph 1.
. ""92
,b
»
f 1;
92 __
-f X
3_
,1
=§§a@§§_§g§a :Z ;:_-_:i_':
.54-~Jf -92 dim.
.I
.Jg
dim.
/
248 texture Ex. 2-20 continued. m.67 Jlf: , 1. ll.. _' nw
-' P espress
|_ 1. _1 lfl in-1'
'
ll.. t
P espressO
| I,|li i I_ m_ 4- 92 -fn Y I-m Y l 1-I . _ . Q ; 92 I/._ll11h.||1.3 l:r1Zr1|.4I_:|.1 9292-92vl:~ll111¢:|.A;| ll_| _Q * `i -;`_Q__& *i92 * i_ | tn i |i'r| 7 »` ,»-'Y _ ||.;1v ||'1 Q-f&` Ya I in » ,_ F `92
I
92 1,
I
/f
_
'Y °
e
'
r .O pizz lf
-i
compensation inthe quantitative decline-in a limited projection of motivic substance. 1
The subsequent stage inthe process might besymbolized as R3!, with all voices but one now reduced to stasis, anddeleted gradually. The surge and accelerated activityof the viola line is compensatoryto continued decline, holding the recessionin balance, as isthe maintenanceof dissonance intensity. Gther recessive factors are the continuing dynamic reduction and the total cessation inharmonic rhythm. The ultimate cadential measurecompletes theprocess, recedingfrom l
to l, with rhythmic values lengthenedgradually asthe viola line falls in a complementary gesture, andwith harmonic inactivity maintained within the dissonantframework. The entire passage is extremelyuseful asan example of textural succession calculated in expressionof cadential and anticipatory structural function, and of the complementarity and compensationof other, relevant element-events. The manner in which persistent instability 'of tonal-harmonic content on the brink of resolution! counteractsall tendencies ofrecession istypical of tonal contexts.
Texture as space
The following examples showtexture in its spatial dimension and indicate particular textural dispositions ofvery distinctive, even motivic, forms.
texture 249
There is a sensein which melodic successionin the extremely high or low components expresses animportant, functional aspect of texture-a modulating field in which events take place. The shape and compass of that field, which we are describing astexture-space,two-dimensioned a fieldsetting out horizontal and vertical boundaries enclosingthe element-successions which constitute the musical work, are functionally effective in circumscribing and governing much of structure, and it seems usefulto regard this space as a distinct aspect of musical texture. The spatial factor texturespace! might be defined as the field enclosed by lines tracing the pitch successions of outer componentsin addition to the two vertical, or diagonal, lines linking these componentsat left-right extremities at some given level ryf structure. All of these referencesare of course analogousto physical structures on a plane." One of the examplestreated at some lengthin the discussion oftonality is the first of Weberns Four Pieces, Op.7, for violin and piano, discussedas Ex. l-49, pp. 162-65. Let us attempt to see that piece in its spatial texture, as defined,by describing the field within which its pitch events arebroadly circumscribed and then by attempting to identify some of the important characteristics of changes in the spatial field as they might constitute an expressive succession within this particular parameter. In expression and prolongation of its EI; quasi-tonic the piece sets out with two functionally significant occurrences ofthis PC four octaves apart Ex. 2-21!. The violin descends thenceto a prolongation of activity on eb while the bass, firstrepeating Ebin m. 4, ascendsin the penultimate measure to eb, after which both recessionsconverge onel», the root of the final triad. Obviously, there are deflections in that spatial field, like the low F ll of the piano in mm. 6-7, a prominent local expansion of the space; but the foregoing comment describes theessential linesof the spatial contraction as to a marked extent influencedby the tonal centrality of the PC Eb, and the axial centrality of ebl.! Seen in this sensethe texture-spacesuccession isone of recessive contraction! The resultant focusingon the root of the final, cadential triadic resolution is of coursesupportive of tonal function, and the analogously spatial 3°It wouldseem reasonable to extendthe conceptof two-dimensioned musical space to include a further dimension,that expressed by the hierarchic levelingof element-structures from the foreground of the mostimmediate, detailedsuccessions to the remotestbackground-the structural level atwhich thebroadest, most generalized lines of succession are discerned. Hereagain, inthis referenceto a third dimension in musicalspace, terms like foreground and background are analogous toterms usedwith referenceto the qualities of physicalspace. Themost vital dimension, time,is symbolizedby the left-right factor in this image, whilethe up-down factor symbolizesthe field of pitch frequencies. The third dimensionto which the abovereference ismade is,then, one of actual depth of experience ofstructure. Otherparameters ofmusical experience-thefield of intensity, or that of timbre, forexample-can of course be conceived as further dimensions.!
250 texture Ex. 2-21. Contraction oftexture-space defined by recurrences of tonal PC inWebern, Op. 7, No. 1. mm.l-3 mm.6-8
l_ m.9
anticipated
_ in
mm.-4
m.6!
m.8
formulation described by these lines of succession fromthe extremesof the opening to the compressionof the final sonority is an important aspect of the pieces structure, constituting a factor significantly complementary in functional eH`ectto that of tonality, to which it is inseparably linked. Concurrently active element-structures for example, that of rhythmic motion, gently accelerative toward the middle and restorative of longer durations toward the end; or upper voice linear descent! are complementarily or counteractively shaped." The idea of space as anaspect ofmusical textureis especiallyrelevant to theworks ofWebern. In certain of that composersworks spatialsymmetries are formed by the distribution of inversionally-related set-forms in equidistance around a central axis. In Ex. 2-22a, an extracted major segment of the first movementof the Quartet, Op. 22, there can be seen aprogressive expansion of the texture-spaceto an extreme point m. 22, a critical point in the movements overall structure!, at which the synopsis givenin Ex. 2-22b is terminated. The point of extremity to which the spatial progression movesis underscoredin a number of ways: there are complementary progressions ofqualitative interactions of texture, and color and rhythmic structures are directed intensely to this same point, from which the movement, in a number of complementary element-successions, recedes. The As noted earlier, final increased density-compression is inevitably counteractive, while clearlysubordinate, torecessive spatial contraction and,in the Webern, ofrelatively and potently!consonant implications.
texture Ex. 2-22a. Webern,Quartet,Op.22,for piano,violin,clarinet,andsaxophone, first movement. 12 tone set:
Sehrmassig J.= ca.36! tD-
f
Vln.
Teno sax.*
Piano
m.4 Vln.
C1.
Ten.Sax
Piano
«All pitchessoundasnotated.
P1ZZ.
texture Ex. 2-22a continued. arco
Vln.
C1.
Ten.Sa
Piano
Copyright 1932,Universal Edition.Usedbypermission ofthepublisher.Theodore Presser Company,solerepresentative United States, Canada andMexico.
Ex.2-22b. Summary diagrammatic representation of thetexture-space in thefirstmovement of Webern's Quartet,Op.22, mm.1-22.
m.22
texture 253
functional actions of all relevant elements to and from m. 22 constitute
a fruitfuland criticalarea ofstudy inthe movement. See also Ex. 3-1,p. 307.!
Only theopening bars, and theimmediate approach to m.22, aregiven in Ex. 2-22a; Ex. 2-22b, however,is a synoptic renderingof the entire passage. Itis contrivedto showexpansions in the texture-Space, disposed equidistantly around the fll /gb' axis,and maximallyinflated atm. 22. A further example goes back to a far earlier historicalreference, the
Prelude to a Handel Suite for harpsichord. The shaping of the texture-space is vividindeed; and this particular kind ofconfiguration space of cflthe Webern examples!, achieved byalmost consistent, high-level directional opposition betweenouter components,projects a characteristic contradirectional counterpointwithin an essentially simpletextural context.
Example 2-23a shows brief a extract and Ex.2-23b is a condensed represenEx. 2-23a. Handel, SuiteNo. 3in D minor forharpsichord. Prelude.
m 6 Presto fl Y.-fi |l.. ' Ku'
*1' X
ll? #Ll =n'33 l rl
li-1 Zi
llf l-=
rl: =
1l
r'§.llPi111 »| _.atv
"_,
c
r35/
i §
_i_l_t _f
-8 f
=
»:-1_92L_ =r"' |92~l_ _ ,q HI
,,' ii ,,f
-I
il ' aui1;=' _U---I*-4'ii 11. .Ji _92"_/ ¬i.A7'/ jg 3 `
m.l0 / .I _e Y Hgi?.uf r921:;:.z:r;'1.a1 . ||n1uI;..J1.A_', _ f' 92 Q.
~' fi 'xli' _92 i
_-1; _
1 I1 I __ Q
H!__92 .f
11`_ ___ 11
~'
92i / hi J
__-92_ _».92/-nrnl Ir' .. ' 3 :.:1.4! 1 92 3 P;
3 92 lf /:ri l -
254 texture Ex. 2-23b. Synopsis ofinflation andcontraction inthe texture-space as expressed in contradirectional relation of outercomponents.
/+
I"
2 92
'.'/_E 9
|3
92 .»-fy,
3 929292
li'
92i ----
£55 lf
-*/-S
as
¢9292
~»
5'-.... ..-..----""»"__i...-'P
iv V
i
tation ; it is evident that there isa single,forcibly directed succession progressive, then recessive! in which spatial inflation and subsequent contraction must be regarded asmost critical elements in the structure.
Mot/'v/c texture;
the provocative
effect of unusua/ textures
There are many times in music perhaps especiallythat of the last hundred years! whenmaterials areof suchdistinctive textural cast, andwhen the particular qualities of texture are sovital a factor in the identity and interest of thematic-motivic material,that it seems plausible to think and speakof texture as motivic-or of a specific texture-motive. A thematic melody in voices interdcpendently doubled in 3rds, where this textural feature is a necessary and recurrent aspect of the material, would be properly conceived asthematic texture in an important sense. In Ex. 2724, the motive is incapable of adequate characterizationwithout reference to texture as wellas to rhythmic and linear formulation: one can scarcelyimagine representingthis materialsay, in a thematic index-with one line alone. A very famous exampleof motivic texture is the initial sound of the Stravinsky Symphony qf PsalmsEx. 2-25; seep. 256!; it is a recurrent motivic factor of arresting quality primarily becauseof itsarticulation and anomalous, distinctive spacing.It is not significantly defined merelyas an E minor triad as might e:i in a Classicalcontext!, nor with respectto its orchestral colora"Consider, for example, thefirst movementof StravinskysConcerto inD for violin and orchestraor the opening thematicstatement inBeethovens Sonata in C, Op. 2, No. 3, for piano.
Ex.2-25. Stravinsky,Symphonyof Psalms,first movement. J 92
5 Fls.
4 ObL Eng.Hn.
S BnL Cbn. 4 Hns. Tpt. in D 4 Tpts. inC 2 TmbL B.Tmb. Tuba Tlmp.
S. A.
Chorus T. B.
Harp
2 PianosllOh ItpCc gg div. piss. div. piss.g Db. Copyright 1991byEditionRusse deMusique;Renewed 1958.Copjeight andRenewal assigned to Booscy S Hamkes, Inc. Revised EditionCopyright 1948byBoosey O' Hamkes, Inc. Reprinted by Permission.
texture 257 Gesualdo. Or, cha In gioia credea viver contento Book 4!
from Madrigals.
An example from Gesualdois given below for relatively comprehensive analysis oftexture. Major attention is given to changing interlinear relations within the range from maximal diversity to total accord. Changing, func-
tional patterns of imitationare, however, also ofgreat importance and interest. Thus, while all four sectionsare imitativc at distancesranging from
lgl tol5¢l !,the shortest time interval, briefly suggested in m.4, becomes appropriately prevalent in the section Fuggesi . . _ flies away!, an area of relative rhythmic and textural animation conditioned by the text ; and
imitation persists, mostly at 4J , in theintensely chromatic final section Oimé, vienmeno; alas, it wastes away!. A representation of textural progression and recession with limited symbolization of` qualitative relations as well as density! is given as Ex. 2-26b. Example2-26a isa quotation of the entire primaparte ofthe madrigal. Investigation showsthat nowhere in this example isa diversity value of 1
I 1 achieved.toward Developing ofpoints maximal textural are comple T T
two progressionsof persuasivegrowth from one voice alone, or from two lines in interdependence!. The intensity profile of the entire structure thus rises quickly within the First severalmeasures, subsequentlyreceding and progressing againto a point of maximal diversity at the immediate outset of the intensefinal section. Example 2-26brepresents thestep-by-step processes by which the textural structure is shaped. It is important to note that progression in density-number is significant at more foreground levels that of the phrase especially, where there is progressive accrualexcept in the Hrst two units, with the cadence usually formulated in full density! while the progression of textural diversity significantly features lower degrees tj complexiy but notqfdensigy-number!the in internal phrases. Itis thus in this second parameter of textural structure that the overall shapeat the highest hierarchic level is bestseen: thefive voicesare engagedwith considerableconsistency throughout the piece while changes in their degrees of interdependence shape the broadly defined textural succession? Telemann. Fantasy No. 4 for violin alone, first movement
That the study of functional textural events canbe critically important
258 texture Ex. 2-26a. Gesualdo. Or, che ingioia credea viver contento from Madrigals, Book 4;first part ./
lI
l Or, chein gio -
II
lE
JV
,
Or, chein gio - -
lI
I _I
ia
-; Cre~dea vi- vercon-
'-"_ '~
Or, che in gio - g__&
ia cre-dea vi - ver con - ten - to,
l" 1
31 1
`t Or, che in gio - -
JM _WE_ , L_ ____ ia, or,
_e e
cre-dea vi /92
- if t V-VVI
E rE
ia cre-dea vi -ver con-ten- to,
'che ingio -
cre -denvi -
ia cre- dea
vi -
5_4
ver con-ten-
Lo
or, chein gio - - na cre-dea vi- vercon-ten - to, `°""_l%"_°1;__°"i"_ '_""_' ___ ' ' l __ VW _ Q1.l -I I1 _:lQ: 1 1 p. 92 gi/ IV ten - to, or,_____che ____ in gio -ia cre-deavi - ver con - ten - -1. r r ===. frm" 7'1"-I 25 - ver con-ten - to,
If
or,____* chein gio -
II
ver con-ten-
vi - ver conten 7,
mr*-r:.;=;=a= J
vi -
E*?__1rto,
ver con -ten - to, cre-dea
m.8 -/]'|4_ _ Y ,t,, /--*_§ , N' ' e ' to, M'a-pre la gio - -
Q* - ia.
m'a - pre la gio -
M'a ~ pre la
gio -
ia, |'n92
- pre |a
pre la
gio -
ia, |'l92'l
- pre |l
to. M'a
&_ to. M'a Np
ia cre-dea
-I 1
M'a - pre la gio
I !° pre la
L er
gio -
ie, m'a
r1
- pre ln jo
t
-
texture 259
m.ll
4 »'92 .I - iall se - no, Fug-ge-si
:1 1I J
lal-ma el cor, c'lcor,oi-mé, oi-
7O
l
I 92
gxo - - 131| se no, Fug
- SC-Sl lal - mael cor,fug -gc-sl lal-mae]cor, ol-me-
gio -
-ge-si la|- maelcor, oi
- iail se - no, Fug
- ia
il se - no,
-
Fug-ge-si -laI mael cor, fug -ge-si lal- mael cor,
oi -
92c c il se - no, m.l5 4 __
Fug-ge-si lalma el cor, fug- ge-silal - ma e|cor, oi-mé,__
,
@*1{_F3 gf? mé, vien me - no, # il:-»
oi - mé, vien
. *;Z;f;_
me -
oi - mé, vienme -
=» I - f=
- mé
e F ,_ , , - mé, A 1»=1' 7 _
it r
-1 -
mé _.__ vien me - no.
D vien me
- no
oi -
mé, vienme - no.
'_ JQ oi -
_
LE
no, oi
'8
mé, oi
, it
° ¥==92 JH - no. ..
oi mé,
£:=92 H
mé vien me -
I-
- no._____
oi -
mi, vien me -no.
Reprinted fiom the UgrinoEdition, W. Weismann, ed., by permission qf Associated Music Publishers, Inc., as agents Q' Deutscher Verlag fur Musik, Leqrzig.
texture
260
Ex.2-26b. Texturalprogression and recession, quantitative and qualitative,represented at phraseandbroader levelsin the Gesualdo example. m.4 m.7 m. II Qualitative changes:
Numbers nf real vniccs:
II I2
2I
m.9
m.I I
I I2 2,2
I2 2 !,2
3!,
2 5,2
ni.14I Oime... I I I2 2 II I2 2 22 I2 2I I2 I5 2I 3I
Qualitative textural profile:
Numbers oflines:
2-27a. We can only scratchthe surfacehere,again; but textureand textural structurein virtually all its aspectscould well be the basisof inquiry in comprehensive explorationof this movement." The movement'sform is basicallyone of recurrentstatementsof an openingthematicidea separatedby episodicdigressions of a more fluctuant character.One objectof inquiry might thereforeconcernthe applicationsof textural variation in thematic recurrence.The reader will perceivethat the openingmaterial Ex. 2-27b!, like other instancesin episodicas well as expositorypassages, posesa line of compoundimplicationsof two strata in oblique relation, with sequentialrepetition. The textureof the firstappearanceof the motivemight be symbolized as ,
where I! isan "implied" independentvoice.At the secondentry, in
mm. 34 35, thereis variationin the doublingof the lowercoinponentin the secondpart of the sequencebut not in the first. The statementat m. 57 is analogous to that of m. 15 exceptfor restorationof the primary tonic.! These variations,graphicallyrepresentedin Ex. 2-27c, show texture undergoing transformations complementaryto thoseof fluctuant tonal reference.The 42TheTelemannmovement is brieflydiscussed and quotedin full in Carl Parrish,A Treasury ofEarlyMusicNew York:W. W. NortonandCo., 1958!,pp. 297301.
texture 261 Ex. 2-27a. Telemann, Fantasy No. 4for violinalone, firstmovement.
/.l 1111--I--2.1 `l!,--Z1 `Z
Iii1 I-111 111111 bl--1 l__-Qu# _111 111 i1 ll-.1--H --1 ` Q
If -1-!-ul-H
m6 _ W _____ l |;r. 5i'11_-ld
92 __ -l 1: 'Till-U'1Y'1Y'1-I r~!1!!1_1$-$_1_1
F rr l!l!_El -
.Gln 1 /.th VI I
_ 1-111 r-11-1 _I
1l
V V TTT
1
1
ll _Z
'°'
.=. !
1 2| _2
I1
m.33 tr
a !""""'
1: E
11 Reprinted from Georg Philipp Telemann, Musikalische Werke, G. Hausswald, ed., by permission Bdrenreiterof Verlag.
Ex. 2-27b. Compound implications in theprimary motive, m. 1. hm-. ___ .un-mi -f /_.min lj __ ilix
1 Il'
.1 D
_ .u 11 nnnlIQ;1ll' » -
1|
'-'-F etc.
Ex. 2-27c. Texture inthematic recurrences. mm. 1-2
for J
mm. 15-16
]E
J J
mm. 34-35
[J
]
progression constitutedby this series ofappearances isone in which an initial 1_ _. texture T! of isenhanced subsequent in appearances by atrue, sounding doublin com 8P
onent°»where this au mentation of 8
densit Y is Y onl artiall PY
262 texture
carried out mm. 34-35! there is at the same time extension of the unit by repetition of the two-measurepattern. Most vital to the analysis oftexture in this as in any example is again the consideration
of the concept of
textural progression:
the quantitative
increase, orthe progressiveintroduction of qualitative changesin the direction of increased complexity and intensity in interlinear relations. It is remarkable, and typical of this movements unusual interest _within the restraints ofthe medium, that there are qualitative and quantitative textural shapes whichconstitute an important aspect of its structure in complementarity with other elements. The obvious but critically expressive profile of the opening phrase, considered in the light of textural change, isan example. The accrual from a condition of two implied voices, one relatively static, to two sounding voices in considerable differentiation, to the four-voice density of the dissonant chord at m. 5, to the releaseinto cadence,with a second soundingvoice inactive! momentarily introduced, is expressive ofa persuasivetextural shape beautifully complementary to that of the line of pitch events, and to that of the occurrenceof tonal and implicit dissonance aswell. The textural structure of the digression at mm. 21-34 Ex. 2-27d! requires special study: it is a crucial element in the climactic, chromatic EX.2-27d. '°'92 /°`
.
.w:=§=='§;:§§.:==L_E5== =;_53= ='
e=g5,=;.-=-' ~-=;_,._,....
m. 25 'J
1A '
=f
l§l@ e e k
A-92 ,.92
/`
13
m _ . /_T - _:Ji G5 "=--.!....! =!E=-.EE-%'{'°§;E=; -'-E I'-I lllll Ill' ~ s'
f92 on
-I
~
Reprinted from Georg Philipp Telemann, Musikalische Werke, G. Hausswald, ed., by permission Bdrenreiterof Verlag. The textural structure ofthe first digression partiallyquoted in Ex. 2-27a!is very different from that of the theme: it consistsof an alternation between contrasting textures, an alternation inwhich changeis variationalrather thansignificantly climacticallydirected or subsident. Measures 42-50 ofthemovement are comparable.! Space does notallow detailed
texture 263
development precedingthe m. 34 cadence.Here are stages verysubtly contrasted indeed. Compare, for example, the implied two-voice texture with which it begins, onein which only one of two implied voices is significantly active in pitch-line; from this tentative diversification there is progression reaching a level of determined opposition of the two albeit still implied! strata after m. 30, and the subsequent, late recession into a condition of relative interlinear accord m. 33!. Further insight into this extraordinary progression canbe suggestedin the comparison of analogous measures23, 25, and 29-32. In what important sense ism. 27 different in textural qualities from m. 23 ? How, throughout this process, istextural releaseachieved at punctuating intervals as the directed motion is otherwise continued ?! The importance of interlinear independence achievedby the determined, insistent, opposing demands of concurrent textural components isnowhere more potent than at the summit mm. 30-32! of this progressive shapean intensity of texture momentarily resumed at m. 4lHf!. This fact is critically suggestivewith respect to an overall, broadly defined textural progression at the highest architectonic level. Finally, a spectrum of textural values and conditions might be identified for this piecein a symbolization of content of quantitative and limited qualitative distinctions Ex. 2-27e!. Ex. 2-279. 11
l1
34
T 'I
Densities 3and 4 represent, ofcourse, valuesof infiated importance in so restricted a medium. Qualitative diversity of three voices, one of them implied, is maximal for this particular context; yet, even the importance of
%!, which in is there determined differentiation components of the by means noted above, must be emphasized if Telemanns structure is to be understood. The movement is, in summary, a seriesof textural progressions and recessions,the former developing at precadential points! toward maxianalysis ofthis passage, nor of the textural structure ofthe final digression mm.63-70!actually aninterruption in the final theme statement, one ofsubtle, restrainedcontrasts. Interlinear intensity in the climactic portion of the movement issuggestive ofa textural featureof importance,that in which diversityis achieved by projectionof lineswhose relative independence is heightenednot only by directionaland rhythmic opposition butby dramatic contraintervallicrelation aswell, i.e.,by theprojection ofintcrvallic featuresofthe highest possible dijerentiation-that of semitonal asopposed toprevalent succession, in the competing stratum,by leaps.
texture
mal quantity and/or maximal diversity and activity in which the texture is significantly complementedby other intensifying factors, notablytonaljfuctua tionanddissonance. Intense qualitative developmentin the extendeddigression at mm. 21 34 Ex. 2-27d! is crucial in textural structure at the broadest hierarchic level. Bach, "Denn das Gesetz des Geistes," from Motet, Jehu. meine Freude
So excellent is Ex. 2-28 for the study of textural structure and process that it is quoted in full. The opening nine measuresare a counterpoint, occasionally imitative: 2> !,4' 4sbetween the alto and soprano components,the latter in a consistent doubling in 3rds; the texture is thus one of two real voices within a density of three lines with restrained imitative interactions and generally contrarhythmic-contradirectional relation between the two essentialcomponents. Someimitative interactions are traced asdiagonal relations in Ex. 2-28. Following m. 9 there is a progressionin which total interlinear diversification emergestentatively m. 11, m. 15K! but in which this tendency is restrained by intermittent recessionto the textural conditions of the beginning m. 10, mm. 12 14; note how the textural component consisting of paired interdependent lines undergoesfrequent recoloration!. The incipient tendenciesin the qualitative progressioncan be analyzed further in observation of the logical extension of the trends noted above. The consistencyof two real voiceswith occasionalimitation in mm. l 9 thus representsan initial stage; the following bars, with compromising interdependencesand consistentimitation, constitute a secondstagein the direction of maximal, but not yet achieved, interlinear independence; after m. 15 there is interlinear independenceto the end. In an extreme foreground the rhythmic identification with considerabledirectional contrast between outer voicesin m. 15 is a still intermittent step in the processof diversification, as is that of m. 17.!
Quantitative contrast in the final stage and momentary relief within the parameter of density-number! is achieved by the opening of the texture to a single voice, with reaccrual of the others, after the m. 19 cadence,the cadential effect of courseenhancedby this textural device. Increasing rhythmic interdependenceas well as relative inactivity of the lower voices especiallyalto! help to prepare the final cadence,which is nonethelessa relatively suddencessationof competing contrapuntal activity, enhancedin its provisionally conclusiveeffect by the fermata and the metric 4 The symbol denotesimitative interaction.
texture 265 Ex. 2-28. Bach, "Donn dasGesetz des Geistes" fromMotet, Jesu,maine Freude.
S°P'-1 :¢=:=
' ::
=l_=v::=;=-
"-" "-*
Denn Gedas setz - des Gei stes, -da- le der
swf-2
JJ
5*
J = ===-
Denn das Ge - setz "`des
Alto vu; __ .5 ='i|'{ 5=F=F -1 :wiQ11 l 11 N
Gei -stes,_de_r` _-_da"le _hen - - ` dig__
3 ' 1- __J* 92 JT 1E
Denn, denn
-/In i
5E§§§§5:-IE f 0
as Ge - su;
1.11 _ .""' -|
_Q
_Ie -
rf - lr:=:=.== I/»:__|!|!"-I Nl- _ 1i 1
--
des Gei
___ V, V1 =e su, in
°*' =
Chri -
-j
e
__
- sto
me =
_, ,_ .i_. 1 _le - su, in Chri -sto =,"' ._ =E 7 c == §Z.111 7 '' |/..'i1i|1»1 11111 !'Q§_jl!92-'2§l*92i_ 9292|'mz§'lQ|-I-:-2-_Q "_ In "Y-2i;_ 1 _ _-Z £5 - stes, der dale - ben -- dig mach et - in chfi - sto m.9 -J In_ _ =i"-' 'l
:u °
je -
:" ' -
LG; 1
su, hat
e
_Ie :" I/._' _I-'D- Z____j 9292-92' I je -
.;= '
,_ ' F=
F=~ '
mich 92 frei 8°
!i
3°- macht von
~-_I1======:i:=5 e : '
su, hat
mich frei
a=
` frei
:' =l-|!P =-_- -g-f 0 su, 1
hat mich frei ge
m.l3 4I'|. ! U. U-l!7_-_-_-||lZ 2 §l-.-I la. IIN9292-92' -7--!_2!_YQ
ge -macht, _'
- macht,hat mich :nf -L_
5:
frei ge
- D-' _ Y_ __
hat mich frei ge - machtvon dem_ Ge 1'| Q -bf-- -:_--I! _ Z.. e E_2{F=§i'$===-?=F:'2{=='|i== l'=| 9292l' lY1'l1 _u _Q il-IP_ l hat mich frei _ gemacht von dem Ge -setz,von dem Ge-setz der Siin - de,
I! |! - macht, , _
der
266 texture Ex. 2-28 continued. m. I7 J 4V _
i_
i: '*-.-.u;=2f" E
I"
- de und des
To - des, = ` _== -.1if3
.--li-To ~ _"|` lu. lj-C ._* I ._-I"
` -'l- mr
,_
12
=' .aéesi fi /I i
_
vo%Ge-setz der 92 ` '
-~--- des, _ L1- lQh1
_ -Btu'--'Q
QJ
Siin-de und des To
"Y-
hw-ll:l|.°: ! - des,
von dem Ge-setz derSiin de
D
und des
m.2l , ,._92 _'I'-1 L! Y.-Q--_'-|!|j-|-Il--IV _ QI!
A l lm IU ' I 92 -'Z-9 __ 0 QI-l|===2 2 YQ ll= dem Ge-setz der Siin-deund des To - des. , /B I'|' _ .I ~n _f _ Y;f-=§u'==f=?}lF|='§li»==i=U;u_.¢-:fi --' --' ° '2 ,ZF -' I !-I __ von dem Ge-setz derSiin-de unddes To - dcs. lfl' ` 11.1 Y. _ 2L 2
lin _ . § To -
--
'1 a
2
des.
strength of the final accord. The final phrase, with its stream of imitations
at the, isof course a lower-level textural progression in itself; like the prior section beginninghat mic/zfreigemacht. The reader will be interested, in further referenceto this extraordinary example, to consider therelation of other elementsto the textural structure. For example,consider theoccurrence ofthe highestessential pitch in conjunction with maximal density and complexity of texture, and the concurrence of the tonal structure, which is modulatory and whose shiftof tonal reference also occursin conjunction with the area ofmaximal density and intratextural activity. The persistence oftextural complexity, after the noted progressions, to a late point might well be felt as complementaryto the open tonal structure.!
Brahms, Symphony No. 1 in C minor, Op. 68, first movement Un poco sostenuto!; Introduction
Of necessity,discussion centershere on a portion only of Brahms first symphony, the introductory first thirty-seven measuresof the first movement." It will be possible in this context to consider in an orchestral context a number of featuresof texture as theypertain to a broad structural function, that of preparation of the main body of the movement. As so often in orches-
texture 267
tral symphonic introductions, the tempo is slow in preparation for the allegra main body of the movement. The example is, at the broadesthierarchic level, anacrustic, consisting of a powerful initial attack and subsequent growthand decline to a condition of minimal textural substance,low dynamic level, halted rhythms, relatively settled pitch-lines, and prolongation on the dominant, the most likely harmonic areaof anticipatory function. Broadly speaking, thereare two textural progressions of compelling spatial and qualitative growth, the second of these havingits own, lower-level, progressiveanacrustic preparation. There are thus two releaseareas: the first of these ismonophonic, then chordal in texture, the second more active and diverse texturally but with dialogue rather than overlapping interlinear imitation. To a considerable extent, the senseof releaseis accountable to dynamic reduction and a decreasein sonority, aswe shall see, but the resolution of textural complexity is vitally complementary. The entire introduction is quoted in Ex. 2-29a; then the features of texture and textural progression andrecession ineach of its sections aretreated in detail. 0ne striking fact to be emphasized inthe analysis is the unmistakable manner in which textural and complementary! structures are reflected in analogous shapes at varioushierarchic levels from the broadest, that of the entire introduction, a single motive.
to the most microcosmic,
that of
It seemsdesirable to pause briefly to seethe introduction as a macrounit, a broad anticipatory gesture shapedalong the lines suggestedabove and below. These linesrepresent a path of growth and decline, an intensity curve, asrepresented inEx. 2-29b; the graphic treatment includes asummary curve and shows theoverall metric function of anacrusis in a discrete symbolization.
Detailed analysisof textural events andprocesses inthe introduction is best treated with reference to its four divisions:
mrn. l-9;
9-20; 21-24
and
25-29 i.e., 21-29! ; and 29-37. Certain basicfactors shouldbe kept in mind: the broad sweep of the introduction at the background level in which its anticipatory function is preeminent and in which it is broadly seen asa I-V progression preparing the Allegro; dramatic technique in which elementchange is at times violently severe and abrupt, as when texture explodes under the pressures of developed intensity; distinctions between real voices as opposed to doubling lines, and among textural parameters of density, space, and qualitative interlinear activity; and the operations of complementary forces,to which adequate, consistentattention cannot always be given-events of coloration dynamics, articulations, orchestration!, tonal-harmonic factors
such as dissonance and chromaticism, the
drive of
melodic line powerfully inflating the texture-space!, and rhythm in all its manifestations e.g., surface accelerations, as of m. 7 or m. 24, metric elonga-
tions, like that of m. 8, or the constant useof syncopationand metric displacement, etc.!.
268 texture Ex. 2-29a. Brahms, Symphony No. 1in C minor, Op.68, firstmovement. Un poco sostenu to
2 Hs. ;egato q Fr I/if _;_ == gt F f"92 or/'* 2 Obs' .legalo dd 3 L f"92 2 E f.|eSato E: kf V r. . "-* : ° 2 Bns.1' °E
Q : é `r:
`r__ _ f"fg7
` rr
Cbn- E125 ==== =-- "
_ _i
"1 Q C _ fffffQ`¥"
_, ` or _,
r f¥-.,/
4llns. *92 .A in 1552* ___f r __ 2
f ====
or o
..
; % 4 ___ r __
-¥ V-_ 'REE I Y r .~ o
-_ ,
I'
--i n
r
~
f£_./
V ._ ;J;;___§i fa
r 5 r rm1
--»--Ti Z' f" =.¬b=::=
I --
.
or 2:1
2:5
vm. §§*i§ 24,?§E£-assi E iég A
" fespr.
e eggto
_e
diml-jg, Q: . . 92 I
V-
fespr. eegato "__
Dr, a=;=:§g=§?;? __. ~92
~&___, f pesante
' |11
9 = or 5* .
|11
3 2
E r.
texture 269
Fl.
" _'Srv m_¢ 12-I S
fx _
/""`y,92 f
1"l'l'_ Q#-1_'1l|
flfwb_
I `- L
_ Qlnjll Q-
-'i_|l1'_ ?'{
ob. g5:= .1;==:. _..==.-gg:5§_;;E;_5§55£5§§
f ' jim =Ei:E§£::ET =`$E'-&?.?§'EE2EE§:§'l*' Q/____,
Cl- ~T== =°' W`
Y lhfnL_ _
1 ~_l _=-I
1 Q 1l lC _lf
_é
sn.I =:'§=....-.¥..===::'==-..=:£:::=;::s:E=:E:':=%§ 4i ulg!l==l= 192°_l'?.e 4` i cbnlk ` '_'
-i-i i-_
-7- f J'
-
_
_
-_ E:EE=::=.='=."~=;?. aiu | 92/ .f
i'
H-np. NV
-a {'_' 92
-Vlmyi .
4»
92 /-92 !
__ _
.f
. "WW |'."°""""` Pizz_ _
1' :;a=_*W-_ =§§ EEEE'£EEEEE£`-EEEEEEE-1*-**=EEEE===§ V92|'I.4l W nm-_ cf P } -"lm 1 "'*""'_ "`v l:' ::===
' 921'l_
"-;=°='Q-'r'92
= '-='-'£==EEFE - 'PI ll . 'Y'|
Db.92@,,=.i . °
;..._?.....-..-_. _llfg =Q
4 .D
J- 2 h-_ Z II1!_DYlH.L -|-
vna. ::::= ===¬-=.i.=::::==:==.=_:=.::::=='===:.= __ __ .A .. _ .3 vc. E_ ="= ==§=="-=§§§:EEE£EI§E§;E§? __; _ _lil ____-_T l __i____Tl*'=
`
,
f
.
P
l.|l`92l$. _P . ""°
=5EE: 7 7 EE
P
Ei:-§§E§§E-§§EE3E§=é " _____
270 Ex. 2-29a continued.
Fl. Ob. CL Bn.
Vln.
Vl Vc Db
Fl. Ob.
Bn.
Vln.
Vc.
texture
texture 2
71 fd _;___ ,,/`
EE *kd
Q; r"| _._;=%§Li i.lffl
-:-Q;-;_-;|
W Ev
, 7 / 92
0,§__,
» 7-
W gli; f
§-'-;_
Inu ;
/"'_ i
*T
1'
__.___. kj; gf*
n
272 texture Ex. 2-29acontinued.
#mae @ ---- ---_:ni rv-
_;_; ;_ »
-
Q2-n i-.;=- =====_:;
Ob g
$55 §E§_5§5§=r::-:::5EE?E§§;.--==
-,»._. ....1i:_,, ...,._ -..' _ °_§§££E ""d _
I
Cl {5"'l=EEE
1 -.'. éJ.-.$ _ ° -A _° I
nn.| EE;
;E==
cbn. L@
E'
Hn.
''
@ 13
Tpt 3;
f P- ,
-
"P
°
-
JP
--
'
_
-#Q
V
Q __ % -
_ .._ _ ;! Q_.
1 552*"""""" Vln. if*
" """
,.a V 5,§_&====-:.:§ 3
I.
____ 9~ = . _
__
I '
dzxisii zgér- * -':.:= -@:;:.. '.W _ G_ '_ _!é-¢. vu ;-;. :: =!:=i ':'!: F Db
l
_
I |
' -I
I
t6XlUI'8
; '
273
_|
QPF: F _ 1" ii`,`fE4~ ' 1f=i
'
#.@_ -
' _
1 4
V Q1 » » Vw
u# -
'I
' "'
V -
EI I I'll. IP
X
' _ nv _.Q -1 P_
@ -1 '92
PP
EF _ PP _l _ _ '?§1§.l ; 11-
`E"% lllljllllilill
-_ -Q
_'i1"1l
texture
274
Ex.2-29b. Intensitycurveat two levels,asshapedby texturalandotherevents,in the Brahms introduction. 24 13
68 57
Intensityof sonority andtexturalactivity density
10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 29 31 33 35 37 Areaof Broadestspaceand tentativetextural forceof anacrustic interactions, build-up;areaof intense reduceddensity Areaof densityaccrual interlinearactivity andsonority
Secondprecipitaterecession within all texturalparameters
Precipitaterelease; areaof low density, minimaltexturalactivity
In Section I there are three voices,powerfully stated, each doubled and reinforced. Betweenvoices 1 and 2 there is very little coincidence of movement i.e., a high degreeof rhythmic and metric differentiation!; vigorous contrapuntal conflict is apparent too in directional opposition, this factor applied very consistentlyand fundamentally. Voices 1 and2 movewith great and inexorable drive, intersecting as directions of motion change, dramatically opposedto the rigid insistenceof the fixed basspedal. Voice 2 woodwinds, horns 3 and 4, violas! is fortified by doubling in characteristic 3rds and later 10ths and 6ths!, providing increaseddensity with interdependence, a technique largely responsible for the richness of sonority. The sonority of this component and of the texture as 6 whole! b
is of course further enhanced by octave doubling: pedi
poet,t,i
b di i
'*i'
it
o ityby
d II'!:
The td
bbtt: . The total
texture 275
complex is thus one of three sharply differentiated, heavily doubled components.
The pedal voice is activated powerfully timpani, double-bass, contrabassoon! by repeated notes in throbbing regularity-a rhythmically insistent factor against the variegated movements of other componentswhile the first and second horns sustain the tonic PC. The pedal, while a tonally-melodically static component in and of itself, thus constitutes astrong contrapuntal factor against thedrive ofthe other voices,to which it is obliquely related. The senseof rigidity opposed tomovement the latter in surging chromatic successionsof great dynamic intensity! implies an impact of severe tensionnote suchdissonances as C, C# ; C, D; C, Bl- ;etc.; and finally C, Fl?!which builds to a really febrile state, rupturing under its own pressures at m. 9.
The progression, as to texture, maintains and does not compromise the firm independence of the three components; in this sense there is no qualitative change. The vital parameter in which there is significant change in the texture is that of space. Thedetermined, largely chromatic rise of the upper voice and subsequently that of the secondvoice! creates, as against the fixed pedal, a tense inflation of thetexture-space. very A acute factor in the increasing space,and increasing conflict of dissonance aswell, is the final chromatic movement of voice l-violins, celli-from F to F# against the dissonant pedal.! The texture-space, as to its upward component, is in its foreground a complex interaction of conflicting tendencies: rise of voice 1; rise of the original! voice 2 against descent ofvoice l; final rise of voice l against descent of voice 2 into urgently indicated, transitory resolution in m. 9.
The stark contrast of Section IIs condition of relative serenity is an aspect of characteristic romantic effect. The expression ofrelease is,with the confluence of complementary element-changes, considerablya function of radical change of texture and sonority, but cf mm. 29-37! with the allimportant, persistent, compensatoiy intensity ty" tonally dissonant prolongation. The relative prevalence of homorhythmic texture is an indication of textural recession,and in itself a radical contrast to the texture of the preceding phrase. Actually, of course, chordal texture accrues,with sustained woodwinds; and sonority is enhanced, incompensatory relationto the severe dynamic and textural reduction, by such techniques asfour-octave duplication in the initiating voice. At phrase level, this initial development has to be seenas a gradually expresseddensity-number progressionpreparing the chordal, homorhythmic texture which becomes explicitat mm. 11-12. The tonally distant harmony of m. ll, derived enharmonically, is of complementary effect in relation to pronounced textural change to explicit homorhythmic conditions.! The only contrarhythmic factor is the continued,
276 texture
residual, now motivic, iteration in the low voice: a subtle activation spilling over from the preceding pedal voice. The accrual represented in Ex. 2-29c is repeated in sequencein mm. Ex. 2-29c.
-f.., e
gg", f
2 *_
a, i
"H HEI' 'C 1371 'np: "
'
n r
i i5
lar! .
274' 3
92 '
gg
_=
13-15. But mm. 15-19, which correspond to but are an extension of mm. l 1-12, achieve moresubstantial progression:texture and sonority developin complementation to crescendo, the pitch-line rises, there is strong chromatic leaning, etc., and the harmony of m. 15 is itself still more distant tonally than that of m. ll. Again, there is doubling in enhancement ofsonority, but more significant is the inflation of texture-space cfi Section I! which is the consequenceof pitch rise; tracing the progressively increased distance between upperand lower components revealsan inflation of the spatial field
i
from to
bv
The abrupt dellation of space m. 19! is analogous indrastic, summary effect to that of m. 9, although of course within smaller dimensions.The complementarity of linear descent and diminuendoare strikingly persuasive. Measures 9-20are, in summary, aset oflower-level textural progressions and recessions moving,at the level of the section, essentially to and from the syncopated attack at the end of m. 18. As a whole, this section represents a significant contrast to the preceding tumultuous, contrapuntally active conditions; thus,
at the
hierarchic level
of the introduction as
Section II is an area of relative sparsity and inactivity of texture.
a whole,
texture 277
Section III serves, inone of its functions, as areturn of thematic materials of Section I, but it is condensedfrom eight to four measures andnow is essentially an expression ofdominant rather than tonic harmony in keeping with its position in the broad functional tonal-harmonic curve of the introduction as a whole. The anacrustic portion by which it is prepared mm. 2124! contains, as dosurrounding sections,references tothe movements principal thematic material. Somewhat like Section II, whose materials contrast, it is built texturally of a compromised monophonic statement with an accruing chordal background and, like recurrentpatterns in the introduction, a large spatial inflation by which intensity is mounted and in relation to which dynamic and rhythmically accelerative progressionsare again complementary. The main body of SectionIII mm. 25-28!, to which the preceding four bars are anacrustic, alsorepresents intensespatial expansion, as in Section I, but without the deviating subsidences enroute hence, the condensation referredto above!. The technique of inflation of the texture-space by persistent, determined rise of the upper stratum against a fixed pedal Ex. 2-29d! is thus a recurrenttechniquein; Section III it is fundamental to the expressivecharacter of both anacrustic and subsequentportions. EX.2-29d.
/*if* z ll.: 2
/.
IZ
/, e I/ ~ 3
I
1| 92 Q
The development of progressively inflated and expanded texture and sonority in the sustainedchordal background against which the ultimately rising and accelerating line of the strings movesis representedin Ex. 2-29e. Exo 2'29el
J
ft,
_- -7-
I
2- 7 1, 0 -If/, !_ '-_ ° -2-
278 texture
Here againis asubsidiary progressionseen tohave important expressive effect within lower-level functional shaping at the level of the phrase. Once again the complementarity of other elements mustbe noted; and once again there is the dramatic device of stark contrast of pitch level and range, dynamics, texture, sonority, etc., between Section III and the following. But note the compensatory continuing stasis oftonal-harmonic condition-static in its fixed content, but intensifying in the prolongation of dissonantinstability.! With the repeated severeand abrupt change at m. 29 a fourth section emerges, likeSection II one of relative tranquillity within all e1ement-structures except that of tonal function.Here, for the first time in the introduction, there is significant qualitative as opposedto quantitative and spatial! tex-
tural development. If this is paradoxical,it is not uncommon:at the point of quantitative decline with complementary reductionof dynamics,deceleration of rhythmic motion, settling of pitch-lines-horn, bassoon, clarinet motive, and final cello line, etc.!, Brahms interposesthe activating stimulus of interactions among components-the only point in the introduction at which significant imitations arise in a brief, abortive animation of generally declining textural states. Attention is directed to the individual components ofSection IV. Again there iscontradirectional relation, a characteristic, potent feature of Brahms counterpoint, between the descendinglower motive derived from the inner voice of the symphonys opening bars! and the ascending ana' compound upper motive a mirror relation freely derived from Section II ?!. Texture is again comparable to that of the first section and subsequent manifestationsof the same, unifying principle: that of afixed element counterpointea' against two compo-
nents forcibly driven in opposing directions: --""__""__"_:The fixed component IS the lower stratum of compound lme: texture thus is remarkably analogous to the very different opening of the movement.!" It is suggested that further attentionbe givento comparisonbetween Sections I and III. The main bodyof SectionIII the recapitulative mm.25-28! isagain characterized by determinedly opposed upper voices,severely contradirectional and contrarhythmicin relation, againstthe fixedpedal beneath. Here, however,the originalsecond voice fails to make ascent subsequent to its initial descent.It levels off; hence,the voicesdo not intersect as before, because of theformal condensation as wellas thehigher pitchof theopening m.25, violins andflutes! andconsequent restrictions of range.In compensation for this restriction, imposed as well onthe originalupper voice,the illusionof consistent rise isattained byoctave transference by doubling violinsat dwérent times. "Reference hasbeen madeto the compound linearstructure ofthe primary motive of SectionIV see the oboepart, mm. 29-32!. Here is a fascinating studyin texture and textural progressionat a microcosmic level. For this line is, in its microcosmic projection,a manifestation ofthe shapingof texture-space like that of the monumental SectionI and other segments of the introduction asdescribed inthese pages. Within that line, and at its lower impliedlevel, a pedal is established bythe recurringdominant pitch; against this another impliedlevel effectsthe lines primary risinginfiection toa high point from which
texture 279
A number of imitations should be traced. The motive taken up by the
oboe, flute, and cello imitatedGJ., at then twice IJ.! at effects an upward linear projection in the oboe portion and subsequently an implied linear ascent of PC, but in descending octave registers, in the succession from 1.
to to
, a striking
use of compensatory progression of higher pitch within the opposing context of recessively lowerregistral placements. The other motive is taken by horn, then bassoon andclarinet, first at
a distance of 3 twice!, thenaatdistance of SJbetween clarinet the entry of m. 32 and the final hint of the motive, the viola descent ofm. 34. In the progression ofimitative interactions noted above a modest one! it is to be noted that the function of stretto, restrained though it is, in mm. 32-33, is significantly supportive of the 'achievement at the same time of a peak in the linear rise beforethe final cadential settling. All of this constitutesa lowlevel shapeof coursesubsidiary in relation to the ultimate textural-dynamicpitch releaseof the introduction broadly heard. Section IV might, in textural structure, be representedas in Ex. 2-2911 Ex. 2-29f. A representation of qualitativeprogression and recession, and the curveof densitynumber changes, in SectionIV. 29 30
3| 32
33 34
35 36
37
'' !1 L é-2 E LL ll L 3- '" 2E
!
Sounding lines I curve of density-number! : 35
22
!
1
17-8 4
| Maximal interlinear independence. maximal density
*Symbols notin vertical ordering ofvoices. '|'One unisondoubling: a differentiation of sonority and coloration, not of densitynumber. 1In m. 34, twounison doublings. melodic motion and with it, the spatial relationbetween thetwo implied strata! recedes. 'I'his issymptomatic oftheextent towhich analysis can goin considerationofa singleelement or characteristic of structureat variouslevels ofprogression, as well asthe remarkableparallels thatcan pertainamong suchlevels.
280 texture
There is increased intensity within this subsidiary shapein densiggin space, and inqualitative interlinear interactions; itis illuminating to see this progression-recession inthese foreground details and then to stand back to see and hearl! it in its broader context as a releasearea yet anticipatory of the following Allegro, with its vigorous resumption of energy. In Section IV, as throughout the introduction, a basic issueis apparent: the achievement of textural diversity by the rhythmic and especially directional differentiation of lines, an aspect of which is the movement-against-stasis relationof pedal to moving voices. In relation to this fundamental technique, theimitations of Section IV are of ancillary importance, nevertheless significantlyfunctional within the foreground level of that section itself Finally, we should recall in broad summary the significance of all textural parameters in functional contribution in the expressive effect and character of the introduction as a whole, and in the often strikingly analogous shapes of its parts at various levels.Texture functions in complementary relation to the structuresof other elements, in the end achieving the release of spentintensity in a number of aspects,texture included, a releasewhich is compensatory to the persistently static finally dissonant! tonal condition resolved only in the main body of the movement.
Dallapiccola, Goethe-Lieder "|n tausend Formen"!
for soprano and three clarinots No. 1 .
Dallapiccolas Goethe-Lieder, like many of his works, are without exception of special interest for the study of textural conditions and processes. Primary attention is given in the present analysisto the first of these songs Ex. 2-30a!. The works twelve-tone setwill be found within a brief supplementary referenceat the end of this chapter.° Brief observation suggests thatthe song is divisible by cadential punctuation andby disparityqf technique into three sections: thefirst, from m. l into m. 10, is divisible into two subsections, with the second after the m. 7 cadence! presentingthe voicesof mm. 1-4 in mirror variations and with all four forms of the row presented in a context in which imitation is free and occasional ; the secondsection, m. IO to the upbeat of m. 14, is altogether based on the three-note cellular unit formed by order numbers 1-2-3 and 6-7-8; see the twelve-tone set!, so that there are constant imitative interactions on varied motive
forms at close distances; the third
section, in which
the disposition of the row is entirely linear m. 13, following the cadence, to The variation in mm. 7-10 isessentially textural, with thevoices restating material in multiple counterpoint aswell asin mirror relations.
f6XfUf6
281
Ex. 2-30a. Dallapiccola, Goethe-Lieder No. 1,"In tausend Formen"!.
Voice '
LentoJL69! 4°f§'=
l'
|'
P; SOSL 1 5_ AA c1. in Es -5;E:;=_1:::§§F?=EEiF.-=.-r.-==E::r.-1:2EE 9292|'l*:"_--lg" Il LT Pé
|| '°
va -..: L. |g '|
i ._
cn. in ni ~5;i'°§§: 92 ij;-1 *5 "":EE3*r§==-"'i'=.*==r1=.1:;f".'.:;::: 1T p; espress. -_
Bass Cl.
in Biff` _
*l1 ll`Y'l[lu
poch !iss.
_
;?;;_ poco espress. i' rn- 5 /92
pp _-;=""`
_`__i_ |-is
- L-:7LQ_:7==_1A._=:| -"" 11 ' "1 For- men magst du dich ver- stek - ken, Doch,__ Al o» 0 Ps" '3 I -__,
1 `f ` - let - lieb - ate»
1; '=
o do
i
V _iii gfg AXP'.-U ' |1
|
, -_
.7 '
92 u
_ I_
'
p;¢sprm. 27.1"
, -2
J* V "J" _ _i
m_8 [QQ] dolciss! X_ !_60! gleich er
- kenn __i_____ ich
U°
'V
2n
- ;~
|
- J*.so! 1- _
V!
3 Kwik
* :-
*All notation at soundingpltch.
_ -5;
_
-
W
'ch; Du
poco ¢`"I""=?¬E-?:E __
,
A pochxZss.!
_ é- ~ __,.
'- .
~
l "' I II
rg
__ ___
282
texture
Ex.2-30a continued.
J 69!
gyp abocca sc michiusa!
! Copyright 1959byS A EdisioniSuviniZerboni,Milano.Usedbypermission.
texture 283
the end! features acontinual concurrence of inversionally related set-forms Bb clarinet with bass clarinet, and voice with Eb clarinet! producing two rhythmicalbf free mirror canonsthe relations are contrarhythmic-contradirectional-homointervallic! with a considerable number of imitative relations, those onceremoved° close enough to have significant impact. There are at various levels important and expressiveprogressions in the textural structure and these will be summarized. Already clear in the above prefatorycomments isthe function of texture in delineating the songs three major divisions; naturally other element-changescontribute to this delineation note rhythmic or dynamic shapes,for example!, but the contrasting implications of texture classes andtechniques areof great importance in this respect. It is of course generally true in a serial work that where the row is deployed in horizontal distribution, imitation, if the texture is of more than one voice, will result in some way granted that it can be elusive if rhythmic nonconformity is extreme and persistent!; in this sense the first three measures where the prime form of the set is distributed in vertical and diagonal linesthroughout the three-voice texture!contrast with, for example, mm. l4~l5 where each voice presents a given set-form in linear ordering-I'°, P°, and PS, then I5 in the Eb clarinet!. T/zefunctiom qftextural change in cadential ddinition are particularly evident in this piece, notably in the final cadence. It is significant that conditions of severe dissonancetend to be maintained at points of cadential resolution; release is critically a consequence of element-changes other than that of harmonic content. To some extent cadential expression is rhythmic e.g., longer valuesat evententative cadenceslike m. 4, clarinet parts; at m. 7; and at the more decisivecadences ofmm. 9-10 and the end!. Dynamic change plays a part too: except for the punctuation at m. 13, very much suppressed in the interest of preparation for the climactic section following, cadence is characterized by diminuendo-particularly deliberate in the final bars, where there isrecessive colorationdenoted bysuch instructionsas psubito, pit!p, ppp, a boccasemic/ziu.sa,"° and perdendosifl And melodic descent is also palpably functional in cadential expression. If three voices ormore! participatein imitation on asingle motive,the earliestfollower isin direct or contiguous!imitation while the next follower, in direct imitation with the secondentering voice,is at the sametime in imitation once removed with respect tothe original statement.Other voicesfollow in imitation twice, three timesremoved, etc.,with respect tothe originalvoice andin closerdiagonal relations to precedingvoices subsequent in entry to the original.This is a significantfactor in imitative textures,resulting in triangular and multiangular relations, because, especially where varied subjectforms appearin a single exposition, thelines ofsignificant relationassume very complex patternsin perceptible not just theoretical!interactions whenthe distanceof imitation is not extreme. Whenthe imitation onceremoved iscloser inshape andintervallic contentto theoriginal subjectthan isthe direct imitation, triangular relationsare particularlypersuasive. Note the Dallapiccolaafter m. 13, where imitationsonce removedare homodirectional,as comparedwith more proximate contradirectionalimitations.! 5°With half-closed mouth. 5Vanishing.
284 texture Related to the factor of melodic descent in cadential formulation
is that
of the contraction of texture-space aasdevice in cadence. The reduction of texture-space tothe compassof a minor 6th in the final cadence is an indication of this: in fact, the final chord achieves theworks smallest space. Comparable recessionsin which the contraction of texture-spaceis an important factor can be seen in the synopsisin Ex. 2-30b; the superimposed pointing bracket is intendedto draw attention to composite recessive contraction of the third,
fourth, and
fifth units
at a broad hierarchic
level.
Ex. 2-30b. Texture-space contraction in the Dallapiccola phrases and ata broaderlevel.
=f` WG.
I 'Zi f- '92
FI!
'-a t /"/ L.
In other respects too texture functions in the expression of cadence. While the cadences tendto maintain a density of 4 except for the initial progression fairlyconstant in the work in general!, they aremarked by increased interdependencecomponent of lines-againwith the exception of m. 13, where cadential stability is not appropriate in view of the approaching, climactic progression. The tentative cadence of m. 4 is marked by the accord of all three voicesand is prepared by qualitatively recessiverhythmic association between uppervoices inmm. 2-3. That of mm. 6-7 is prepared by rhythmic
identification between upper clarinetsat m. 5 andamong allfour voicesat the start of m. 6. The cadence intom. 10 is ofcourseanalogous tothat of m. 4, since it concludes themirror variation of the opening bars,but it is enhanced here by the interdependent bass clarinet and singing voice, each of which pauses ona note of long duration, the voices coincidentin attack. And the
interlinear interdependence of thefinal cadence is themost marked of all; and its homorhythmic, homointervallic, homodirectional except for mirrored outer voices-a final surviving factor of textural vitality! conformity is again gradually prepared by qualitative recession. Interlinearinterdependence as The texture-spaceconfiguration ofthc introductoryphrase, mm.I-4, and that of its mirror, mm. 7-10, arcof specialinterest; thespatial shape of thefirst phraseis onein which the initial compass isthat of a major 7th, expandingto the same intervalin its compound form, andcontracting tothe originalinterval transposed but in a proximatearea.
texture 285
expressed byDallapiccola is often marked by considerable directional and intervallic conformity as well as rhythmic. In a context of unusual rhythmic diversity, interdependenceswithin the texture units of 2, 3, 4! have particularly strong effect and are obvious factors of shaping control. Brief attention should be given to the factor of time interval and to the considerationof possiblefunctions of changes inthe distancesof imitation. In m. IOHI, the first section in which imitation is prevalent, indeed constant, the trichordal cellular subset mentionedearlier appearsin numerous transformations. If we take its original occurrence as order numbers 1-2-3 as definitive and take as referential the IC succession 1-2: a rising minor 2nd, then a descending major 2nd!, we can note the following specific variations, with rhythmic formulations generally involving two sixteenth-notes with the third tied into a longer note or, later, eighth-notes throughout or triplet sixteenth-notes with the third tied into a longer note: mirror e.g., Eb clarinet, m. ll!, retrograde e.g., Bb clarinet, m. 12!, retrograde inversion e.g., bassclarinet, m. 12!, and occurrences in which compound and complementary forms of the intervals are used in expansion of the intervallic content e.g., bass clarinet in m. ll, or voice setting of Zauberschleiern, also a distinct augmentation!. The grouping is easily identified and the interactions among voices unmistakable, although of coursethose imitations in which two like forms or closely similar rhythmic and intervallic formulations follow directly bring the effectof imitative interaction suddenlyinto clearestfocus: e.g.,Bb clarinet and Eb clarinet, beginning of m. 13; or the samevoices in mm. ll-12. The effect isone ofdense entanglementof texture in which four voices,all of them constant in activity, project a singlelimited idea obsessively inreferences that relate diagonally to other voices and,if one considers imitationsonce removed see dottedlines in Ex. 2-30c!, a setof interlinear relations of great complexity. It is in this senseperhaps morethan any other that this section is a pronounced contrast to the others.
In mm. 10-13 the distance of imitation does not undergo significant change. There is imitation at 4 3 between
between upper clarinets, m. 10! and at
bass and Eb clarinets, across thebar-line separating mm. ll
and 12!, but otherwisedirect imitation.: are consistentlyat a distance of2 -in
one sense a preparation for the tightest stretto, atm. 13, at 105 . This does not take into account the augmented forms of the motive in the singing voice, which while involved in the web of interactions seem at the same time some-
what removed
in relation to the faster-moving clarinets,! Thus, the pro-
53The lineof text set here,Du mags!mit Zauberschleiern dick bedekken You mayclothe yourself inmagic veils!,seems a very suggestive basis forthe musicstexture.
texture 287
and lab Bb and bass clarinets!. In the consideration timeof intervals in this sectionthere seemto be three important conclusions tobe drawn regarding the functional effect of imitative distance and its changes: ! there is constant modification of the time interval-any regularity of spacing seems insistently avoided in favor of constant flux ci the preceding section!; ! the rhythms of the component lines are so contrived that absolute concurrence time intervalof 0! is achieved only incadentialjormulation, as noted earlier; and ! the abruptly and maximally tight stretto withwhich thesection begins lower
clarinets lgb! at contributes complementarily climactic toand energy, coincident is with _/brteand crescendo, the performance direction appassionato, and other factors.
It is noteworthy in the texture as a whole that density changes are slight; in general the four component voices arerather continually engaged apart from the progressiveaccrual at the beginning and the repetition of this pattern in variation a few bars later!. A charting of the texture throughout, however, revealsa seriesof qualitative successions in the direction of maximal diversity and subsequently in the direction of resolution; it reveals maximal textural complexity and interlinear interaction in the songs climactic measures 3-15! and the section approachingthese. With reference to the presentation given in Ex. 2-30d, we can make the following summary observations:at point a!, progressionto and through
Ex. 2-30d. A schematic representation ofinterlinear independence and interdependence throughout theDallapiccola song. l2
34
56
78
9 10
ll°l3 14-17
18 I9-20
llll°l3.l_l.l!l_l.ll|llllL°""Lll"l'l4 liz 232 llllll Ill'-" l!ll'l`l 11
92, I
1 lll2l
/92 b
/92 2
11- --2
1 1-
/d
11
g/92 c
*Symbols ofinterlinear association-e.g.,!, denote rhythmic interdependence and often directionaland intervallicconformity aswell.
The time interval8ofAoccursdirect in contradirectional imitation between bass clarinet andsinging voiceacross thebar-line betweenmm. 13and 14.
288 texture l2
briefly manifest diversity Land density 3!, recession through diversity T 1
to cadential accord; at b!, with density 4 constant, fluctuation between three and two real components to the cadential accord, 4; at c!, density progression from3 to 4, qualitative progression tomaximal diversity-activity in m. 8, then a recession ; at d!, quick but graduated! progression todensity l
4 anddiversity T,maximal diversity continuing into the cadential bar with
T T
punctuation a function of rests-i.e., brief opening oftexture by the releaseof the singing voice and bass clarinet;at e!, again, quickly progressive accrual to density 4 and total diversity as above,then a recession tothree real components, followedby highest degree ofaccord at the cadence. Again, analysisat variousstructural levelsis indicated-e.g., at onelevel the considerationof the shape within a phraselike that of mm. 7-10, and at a broader level the consideration ofthe function of this shape andits quantitative and qualitative states in the course of the directed line of intensity reaching itsculmination in subsequent sections. In thebroadest view, the texture is seen to attain through the various stagesnoted a condition of maximal complexity, vitality, and density in the internal, climactic portions: the 1
diversity T,tentatively developed in m. 8, is increasingly persistent in mm. T
T 11-16.
The final cadence isachieved primariiytexturalbf despitethe actions of complementary element-structures; it is the point of resolution of highly complex and active texturesprogressively developed,and it is characterized by almost total interdependence of components, an interdependence compromised onlyby the contradirectional relation of outer voices in a persistent but subordinate manifestation of final textural vitality.
Seria//sm and texture; texture as a product of
chance operations
What might be referred to as classical serialism has of course important implications for the content and character of musical texture: the usual avoidance ofoctave doublings,with consequences for maximal heterogeneity of simultaneous PC content, and the frequency of imitative polyphony
texture 289
induced by twelve-tone procedures are two such implications. A further implication is in the predetermined interlinear heterogeneity of PC content resulting from combinatorial applications of serialism. Densities andother aspectsof texture registral placement, therefore its spatial configuration! have been prescribed in serialization as extensions of the serial prescription of PC and other elements!. Of course, therecan be no generalstatement about the methodof serialization of densitiesand other nonpitch elements; one can only cite this or that method adopted by a particular composerin a given work. The PC set or derived interval set! can be made to yield a numerical set, of course, by any of a number of means: the set of numbers representing the intervallic distances between the initial pitch of the set and each remaining pitch; the set of numbers derived from such intervallic distances betweeneach pitch of the set and the next; the set of numbers denoting PCs as ordered in the set usually 0-l l, 0 = C and ll = B! may be used as the set, or basis of derived sets, of densities and durations; or the composermay establish an independent density or duration! set, or an arbitrary or designed setfor any element ofhis work. Texture in serialism means quantitative aspects susceptibleto purely numerical analog.! One exampleof specificmethod is provided by Kiienek in discussion of his Sestina,a work for soprano, violin, guitar, flute, clarinet, trumpet, piano, and percussion.Ki'°eneks derivation of densities concerns both the original row and the rotational principle of the poetic form, sestina." No presentation of combinatorialityis possiblehere, butthe readeris referredto the following sources, especially toBabbitt, whois ofpreeminent importance in the development of combinatorialprinciples andapplications, andto other sources towhich thesemay lead: George Perle,Serial Composition and Atonaligy, 3rd ed. Berkeley: Universityof California Press, 1972!, pp. 97-98; Milton Babbitt, Some Aspectsof Twelve-ToneComposition, in The Score, 12 june 1955!, pp.53-61; Babbitt, Set Structure as a Compositional Determinant, in _journal qfMusic Theory, V,1 961!, pp. 72-94.Schoenbergs often quoted statement oninversion oftherow transposeda 5th below, combinedwith the original form so as toavoid PCduplication within each pairof correspondinghexachords, occurs on p. 116 of Styleand IdeaNew York: Philosophical Library,1950!. Incontrast tocombinatoriality of this kind is proceduralfixing or invariance of segmental content among setassociations; see, for example, Babbitt,Twelve-tone Invariants as CompositionalDeterminants, in The MusicalQuarterly, XLVI,2 960!, pp. 246-59. 5°To quote Ki'°enek [Extentsand Limitsof SerialTechniques, in The Musical Quarterly, XLVI, 2 960!, p. 223] :The Sestinais oneof thepoetic formsdeveloped bythe Provencal poets ofthe twelfth century, itsoriginal specimenbeing ascribedto Arnaut Daniel. It may well be called aserial formof poetry, and its essential formative principle isrotation. The poem consists of six stanzas of sixblank verses each. Ithinges uponsix keywords which appearat the endings ofthe individual lines. If in the iirst stanzathe orderof these words is1 23 4 5 6, the wordswill appearin the second stanza in the order 6 l 5 2 43. The principle of rotation which is applied here consistsin switching the positionof every two keywords equidistant from the center of the series,proceeding fromthe end toward the middle. Accordingto the same principle,the positionsof the keywords inthe subsequent stanzas are 3 6 412 5; 5 3 2 6 l 4; 4 513 6 2; 2 46 5 31. The processends here,since the next rotationwould producethe originalseries. Thesix stanzas are followedby a Tornado of
290 texture
Ki'eneks consideration of density as serially determined is that of the number of components vertically coincident; in the following extract from his analysishe citesdensity-compression ourword! as another parameter, the location of the tones within the gamut of six octaves designatedas the ambitus of the work, subject to a different governing mode of operation. Ki"eneks twelve-toneset isgiven in Ex. 2-31, and below is further quotation of his discussion ofSestina. Ki"eneksgroup A is the sets first hexachord.! Ex. 2-31_ Twelve-toneset forK¥enek's Sestina, with deriveddensity series. /-Group A-l-92
Iz'I:#"_l, Derived densityseries: 6 3 5
4l
2
Reprinted by permission Bdrenreiterof Verlag. three linesin which the keywords,one ofeach pairin the middle andthe otherat theend of the line,appear inthe order2, 5,4, 3,6, l. Reprinted bypermission ofG. Schirmer,Inc.! Ki'°eneks self-composed text follows in quotation of the first two stanzas only,the translation asgiven in the cited source, butwith the rotation of keywords indicated.None of Sestinas music canbe quotedhere; Ki'°eneksarticle quotesfrom it only very briefly p. 227!. Vergangen Klang und Klage, sanjler Strom. 1 Die Schwingung der Sekunde wird zumMass. 2 Was inGeschichte lebt, wars nurein Zufall? 3 VeU'all, Verhall, zerronnene Gestalt? 4Die Stunde zeitigt Wandel, wendet Zeit. 5 Das Vorgeschrittne ordnet sich der Zahl. 6 In Schritten vorgeordnet durch die Zahl gestaltet sich Gedanlce, doch zum Strom wird strenge Teilung, uhr-genaue Zeit. 5 Ist esvermessen, solches Mass von Mass 2 dem Leben aufzuzwingen, der Gestalt? 4 Der Zwangzerrinnt, erzeugt den neuen Zufall. 3
6 1
[Bygone are sound and mourning, tender stream. l Vibration of the second becomes measure. the 2 What lives in history,was itonly chance ?3 Decline, fading sound, vanished shape? 4 The hour causes change, turns the time. 5 What looks ahead subordinates itself tonumber. 6 In stages preordained number by 5 thought takes shape, but a stream is theresult of!strict division, of clocklike, precise time. Is it presumingforce to such an extent J measure on IW,on shape? Force vanishes, brings forth new chance.]
l 5 2 4 3
Reprinted permission by Biirenreiter-Verlag. of The composers English translation, included in the articlecited above, reprinted by permission of G.Schirmer, Inc.
texture 291
D¢nSitY is the Nxt Pa1`am¢tC1` b¢todetermined serially. Thereare six degrees of density whose successiondetermined is by the positionof thepitehes in groupA. Againthe lowestC! is called 1,the highestGi! 6_ Consequently the initial series ofdensities is 6 35 4 1 2.In density l the two tone-groups A andB run off simultaneously in a sort oftwo-part setting in whichthe dural1l0I'l ofthelI1dlVlCll.lal 1165is dCtCl°II1iIlCbyl [bg mgghanism dgsgribgd____ In
density 2 the iirst and secondtime segments of groupA run eoneurrently with the first segment of groupB. In density 3 two Sggments of eachgroup are developed simultaneously, and so forth,until in density 6 six segments of each group, i.e. twelve all together, run off at the sametime_
Another parameter is thelocation ofthe toneswithin the gamut gfsix octaves designated as theambitus ofthe wc,r|¢_ The serialstatement adopted for thisarea reads that thetones ofeach segrnent should runthrough asmany octaves as there aretones. Thedirection ofthe motionis determinedby the d.iI`CCtiOI`lthC of COI°I'CSpOl'ld.lI`lg lfltCI'V2.l ill the originals¢|°ie5_ Since many Sgg.. ments contain less sic! than six tones, they eoyer less than six oetaves and
therefore could extend overvarious bands of theeornplete ambitus This too, is regulatedby specialserial statements. Needless to say thatall theseserial organisms are subject torotation accordingto the sestina pattgrn,which is th¢ 5UPf¢m¢ law S°V¢fni8 CVCTY m0V¢ Of everyvariable within the whole composition."
In thereference from which theabove istaken, Kifenelr explains further devices of density serialization in hisSeah; Vernzsssgne, a setof sixpiano pieees_ A later serial work of mine is a set gf six piano pieces, ealledSgghj Vermessene. This German titleis a play on Wgrdg, sincg vermessm German in means completelymeasured aswell aspresuming, a pun that eannot bg reproduced in English. While the time mechanism is similar to that gf the
Sestina, the construction differs from it in that for thefirst threepieees system a of five layers isset upin which the first has density 1 i_e_,one noteat a time!, thenext hastwo tonestogether, the third thrgg the fourthfour, andthe fifth six t0I'lCS. The tiITlC II`lC3Sl1I'ClT1C!'ltS fOI` thgVarigus layers arg 3 rgsult Qf summing up the interval magnitudes involved in the eonseeutiye toneeom-
binations. Forexample, thetone series of thiseomposition being; .
the first combination of tones in density 2
_
is; _
The
numerical values derived fromthis progression are 3 a minor third frgm G to and l. half-step fI`OI'fl E to F!. COnSCqu¢ntly first the timgSggmgnt of the firstlayer hasthree units,the firstof the ggggnd has fgur _|_ ]!_As the density ofthelayers increases, the numberof simultaneously sounding intervals and thus the numericalvalues oftheir sum; heeome higher_ Therefore the .$`8g77l¬l'ltSlonger become ,m8anS #18 Chgfdy,t0ne-¢-lujtgfs, of incfeasfng 57Ibid., pp. 225-26. Reprinted by permission of ;_Schirrner [ne_!
2.92 texture thickness are spaced farther apart, while the singletones Uthe first layer _follow each other more rapidb. Computations of this kind form the basis of the whole composition.
In illustration of Ki"eneks basic methodof serialization of layers and densities inthe first three of these pieces,an extract is givenfrom the first Ex. 2-32! _Although the quotation is limited, the density progressionis wellunderway, as is the progression ofincreasing durations.Identifications of these are included in the example.
Ex. 2-32. Kienek, Sechs Vermessene, No. 1.
SD .
9.60 W /92 /-92 ||
J
_______., 2 °""
I 'ff 1:fP |Q +l
LI
|I
| |2
"' f
V| Density 4
t
+7 2 | NPC!!
3:22223 ;___I" , Density 5
|1g +5
IL
|
_ fi
.....-.- ----a T
*Notes withwavy stemsand numbersindicate subdivisions of the eighth-note. Numbers withinslurs showdivisions intoequal parts,numbers withinbrackets thegroupings of the unitsof subdivision. Reprinted permission by Barenreiterof Verlag.
In another operation, procedurally opposite yet often paradoxically analogous asto effect," and commonin certain contemporary styles,textures
are theresult ofchance bywhich theperformers choices are madewithin often considerable latitudes.
53Spaced farther apart in attacks, i.e.,in relativelylonger durations,not in separation by rests. 5°Ibid., p.229; emphasis added. Reprintedby permissionof G. Schirmer, Inc.! °°While in the Kienek piano piecea contextually functional, palpablysignificant successionserially is predeterminedthe progression of densities having increasing durations!, Kieneks methodis unusuallysimple inconsequence, with accrual ofdensity andtemporal! values ofdirectly increasingconsecutive orders.
texture 293 Cono/ud/ng notes
The foregoinganalyses anddiscussions illustratethe modesof textural process as a structure-delineating element in music, and they are a demonstration of the significance and vitality of this element of structure. Although texture in music is relatively little studied albeit much referred to!, its effect is almost always an important factor in structure and expressive effectin some degree and at some level. Even a slight changee.g., theabrupt and momentary engagementof the bass oran inner voice in interaction with another melodic entity-can give significant vitality, however abortive, to the texture at the foreground level. One of the evidences of the significance of textural structure is its immediacy of effect. If this is particularly apparent in quantitative progression e.g., the graduated accrual of textural components as in so many musical works!, it is also true, although subtler in effect, in qualitative changes-for example, the overt passage fromhomorhythmic to contrarhythmic texture, the fleetingvitalizations of texture by momentary diversification, or the rhythmic and other devices of activation of textures of persistent simplicity of character and content. The textural class of a musical instance is almostcertainly one of the first attributes of which we, in listening, become aware; and textural progressionor recession or in static situationsthe obstinacy of unchanging texture! can be especially compelling and direct in expression ofeffective, affectivemusical process. A specific indication of the significance of musical texture is seen in the apparent fact that complexity or simplicity of texture is important in distinction betweenwhat is commonly referred to as art music as opposed to folk, popular, or commercial music. Clearly, one of the primary factors bywhich art music is,at its best, relativelychallenging andinteresting is its broad range of textural attitudes and attributes, and modes oftextural complexity. The textural uneventfulness which, in part, characterizes so many commercialand popular genres sets them clearly apart from the elaborate, provocative, sometimes monumentalprocessive texturesof Western art music. So evident is this that to cite examples seems pointless. It cannot be suggested thatthis factor is altogetherdecisive andconclusive insuch distinctions art music of small forms, and unpretentious intent, is sometimes relatively, even extremely, uncomplicated in texture!, but it may well be that in those instancesin which art works project little vitality of texture they most approach in that respect! the realmsof popular forms. Where the range of textural possibilitiesis, in a great art tradition-say, that of liturgical chant-severely limited, it must be said that ! the limitation is that, a boundary which circumscribes theexpressive potentialof the
294 texture
literature by restricting the parameters within which processivedevelopment can take place; and ! the subtlety of structural delineation, and range of expressive projection,affecting and characterizing otherelement-successions is signQieantlycompensatory-and probably of magniyiedvalue ana'importance-in relation tothe imrnobilityof textureseen asan independent factor. Both of these observations are valid with respect to liturgical chant, the most obvious instance of highly expressivemusic in which texture is by definition simple. That great composers insubsequent traditionshave sorarely devoted themselves toworks of monophonic texture is evidenceof the inherent limitations of monophony and the critical importance of textural vitality in highly developed formsof art music. As this chapter concludes, it should be said again that literatures involved with the study of musicalelements otherthan tonality and harmony especially thoseindependent of specificity of PC content! leave much yet to be done.Texture and color timbre, articulation, dynamic intensity, registral coloration, etc.!° have been much too little explored in their structural implications. We need especiallyto understand process better,exploring all musical eventsas to their expressionsof such fundamental structural functions as progression and recession; this has of course, with respect to the various dimensionsof texture, been theconcern ofthis chapter, and if examples to which we have turned have, with accompanying comment,served to point out some approachesto this kind of understanding, they will have served animportant purpose. Rhythm, especially meter, manifest in the functional, proportional interrelations amongaccentually articulated groups of events, isthe concern of the chapter which follows.
"Unfortunately, referencesto functional and expressiveevents and processes of coloration havehad, andwill have,only tentativeexploration inthis book. But of all processive functions of element-structures, those ofcoloration areprobably mostaccessible to analytical identification,even thoughrelative intensityvalues oftimbral differencesmust be attimes thebasis forhypothesis of plausible sometimes seemingly self-evident! but notyet empirically demonstrated function andeffect.
wnwe AH NOTES Study of the varyingspecific intervallic content as to relationsbetween voices l-2, 2-3,and 3-4 numberedfrom top down! in the recurrentchordal factorreveals thatthe M7 ll semitones! is alwayspresent exceptat m. ll , whose firstchord nevertheless contains overlappingM7s with the m3 the intervalof overlapping.Relative dissonance intensities in the chords are complementedby dynamic stress m. 6! or counteracted bydynamic underemphasism. 3! in what can beinterpreted assignificant functional relationsin articulation of structure. Generally highdissonance value in the chords is underscorednot only by prevalenceof the M7, but by its frequent occurrence betweenvoices l-2 and 3-4 it so occurs in half of the chords!; the interval betweenvoices 2-3P4, m6,m3! is generally relativelyconsonant, although the crucialforte chord of m. 6 projects the M7 between theseinner voicesas well, standing out as the only exceptionto a prevalent normof intervallic content and distribution. The chord of m. 6 is the most dissonant,with three 7ths M7s! superimposed; ithas the formal function of launching the songs second part at midpoint. Measure 6 is unique too as the only point of two consecutive chords note thedissonance progression between them!.Least dissonantchords areprobably thoseof m.2 piano!and m. ll pianissimo!at relativeextremities inthe form.The content ofthe chordsis ofcoursegoverned inpart bythe setsthree equal trichords3 r_° l--1
7 if
0-he 3
3 , and related
_,_ ____i_i
IC 1
31
31
of order numbers 6-7-8or the 9-10-ll.
33 configuration of order numbers
3l Fluctuations inthe spaceor density-compression! of chordalevents arealso of interest inevaluation offunction orfunctional possibilities. Again, theattack at m. 6, _/brte, stands out asthe chord of broadest space and necessarily least density-compression! as wellas highest dissonance value; on the other hand,the pianissimo chord of m. l l has by far the mostrestricted spaceand necessarily greatest density-compression!. These two events,of 32-semitoneand 19-semitonecompasses, respectively, are distinctly anomalous; otherwise spatial fluctuationamong chordsis limited to the ambitus of 5-26-27-8 sernitones. The relation of PC content in the chordsto that of concurrentvocal phrases is alsoof interest:the degreeto whichthe chordsfunction responsivelyi.e., punctuatively!. There is considerable intercomponent interaction of 'thiskind at m. 3, less at m. 5, most ofall at m. 1l, wherethe chordsdo seem responsive-punctuative especially in a reduced tempoin which such relationsare verydistinct! in an aspectof the cadential process. bl tis interestingto notein theforegoing thatwhere fluctuationis relativelystatic rather than progressivee. g., mm. 19-22!complementary effects of ostinatoare to be seen. Or, in mm. 21-22,the compensatory effect ofmotive truncation-analogousto time interval contractionin effect,is adevice ofprogressive rise in the intensity curvewhile
the time interval change is Huctuant between 2, l,and once only! SJ! The . device
of progression illustrated here,which hasimportant implicationsfor metricstructure, is highlycharacteristic ofBartok asother passages in the fifth and other quartetswill show!, asof many other composers of imitative polyphony in all times. °It is noteworthy in the abovedescribed circumstances that there is a series ofveg' localbf progressive accumulations of sonority inthe strings,which arechiefly responsible for theostinato factor.Thus, complementary to localprogressions of pitch and hence of texture-space! within eachfragmentary unit,is thegrowth in sonority ascello and
296
f6XfUf6 viola, thenviolins, enter.The texturecannot besaid tobe materiallyaH`ected within these low-level progressions, but sonorous and spatial growthis in the immediatesurface afactor in the vitalizationof stronglyrestricted tonal-harmonic content. In the ostinato texturein which the strings prevail the essential PCfactors areA and C occasionally E,and later D is added!; of these Ahas thegreater tonalimportance, and thehorns andtuba, echoingthe ostinato,concentrate altogether on A. But last bar of the quotation!as thetwo texturalcomplexes become concurrently veryactive, the A-C distribution is altered in a direction of instability see thelocal, tentative, contradirectional relationbetween double-bass and upperstrings-a slight hint of the technique ofdisplacementessentially of staticPC factorswhich can contribute to the activation oftexture!. dNo impression is intendedthat Stravinskystextures are generally qualitativelysimple, activated byrhythmic, articulativeand otherdevices, although this isoften true.An instance ofvery complextexture highly diversified qualitativelyand quantitatively is found,for example,in the remarkable canons of Part III of the Canticum, where the delineation of formal units is very significantly oneof differentiation in extent of textural complexity.A caseof texture of highly individuated, intenselyinteractive, voices isthe canonicweb beginning Ego autem humiliatus. ..! at m. 219 in Part III. The factthat eachline isa linearapplication ofthe twelve-toneset indicates of course a necessaryconsequence of interlinear relations;if one considers these relations as extending to,for example,retrograde imitations, and imitationsonce ortwice removed like that of contrabasstrombone and third viola!, the entanglementof diagonal, multiangular, andintersecting linesof imitation becomes an intricate maze.Several
kinds of canon are in process, including mirror canon l f !, retrograde canon Z !, strict canonf f !, as well as imitations of other kinds homorhythmic-contraintervallic-first and third violas;cf. mm.224-H1 and 22 7H1; homodirectional-homointervalliccontrarhythmic-second violaand contrabass trombone; etc.!.Rhythmic differentiation is controlled determinedlyin interlinear individuation aswhen theviola voices enter atvarying metricpositions inotherwise strictimitation! aswell asby expected, usual nonduplicationof PC content in vertical coincidences. In the passage there is progressive accrual of density-number and complexity,with recessive process into the cadence m.236! achievedby gradualinterlinear rhythmic accord without density reduction. Thereader shouldpursue theanalysis ofthe canons,and of the striking usages of textural contrastin formaldelineation throughout Part III as inthe heavily sonorous butmonophonic statements which begin and end the movement!. The example isone oftruly organictextural shape and content,and intensetextural energy and motivation. Comparable examples of complementary element-successionsexpression in cadential of and other functions will easily befound. The anthology whichincludes thePurcell Gloria seefootnote! containsmany excellentobjects forstudy of textural factorsin functional processes. For example,the factor of increasinglyintense contrapuntal interaction amongepisodes infugal andother imitative forms isitself a fruitful area of investigation;this canbe evidentand unusuallyaccessible in works ofeven minimal textural complexity,as in the Telemannmovement fromSonata inE minor for two
Hutes, whose imitationsincipient are mm. in 5-6 at J.,then J,progressing ex-to
tended interaction at Jfollowing25 m.and at the metrically anomalous 2 J as well as J and4 J!in the Final episodic development following m. 33; a number of element-structures function complementarily inthese progressive operations. fSo commonis developmental, functional texturalprocess in tonal worksthat examples for referenceseem almost gratuitous. Thereader might,however, bereferred tothe first movementof Haydns Symphony No.102 in Bl. One of this movements most striking passages for studyof textureoccurs inmm. 122-85in the development. Ina process of powerful density progression out of asingle voice, there iselaborate, gradual
texture 297 qualitative progression culminating in an intensecanon onone ofthe basicmotives. Complementary processes of otherelement-structures are of greatinterest: tonalflux is themost evidentof these,but analysisof concurrentelement-actions should include review ofchanges inthe motiveitself-e.g., in the sizeand dissonance of its interval of anacrusis-and consideration ofthe functionsof such changes.! In the canonic segment, voice 3 oftenfunctions asa nonconforming, deviant presence, adding density and impact, asserting thecontrast of relative unpredictabilitywithin the canonic regularity, andmaintaining, especially rhythmically, thecomplete motivetruncated in much of the canon. The canon ispunctuated, thenresumes atmm. 168-69; at m. 176voice 3doubles voice2 in qualitative recession at thecadential approach, a recessive event in which sonority is maintained. Considerable canonicenergy spills into the c:V cadence ending this sectionof the development, instriking texturalcontrast to the extremely simplethematic statement which follows.In studying this example, attention shouldbe directedto analysisof varioustextural eventsin theseprogressive and recessive processes, and perhaps toother passages of functionaltextural change sometimes intensely, often abortively,imitative and polyphonic, asat m. 2l0ffi!. gln this finial texturaldiversification thereare severaldegrees intcrlinear of independence andinterdependence: heterorhythmic-heterointervallic-homodirectional relation, g
, occurring, for example, within a spectrum of
textural conditions extending to true contrarhythmic activity mm. 62-63! and resolving in final textural`accord in which modestdiversification evensuggestive, abortive imitation! is maintainedto the antepenultimate bar. Much further comment might be made: the textural progression andrecession partially by which thefirst fourphrases are unified arean instructiveobject offurther study; or, for additional exploration, onemight considerthe meansby which the texture ofmm. 27-33 partially quotedin Ex. 2-16!, still strongly imitative,are contrived toproduce, withinthe imitation,a momentaryhomorhythmic relationbetween the two voices inpreparation forthe variedreturn of Part I. An example of textural structure in this broad sense, sharplydelineating severely contrasted sections of a freely structuredmovement i.e., highly variegated, nonrecapitulative! ofbroad design,is thePrelude toBachs PartitaNo. 2 in C minor. Its first section, slow, is essentially chordal-a largely homorhythmic accompanying texture underlyingan upper voice melodysometimes doubledin 3rds! and occasionally setting off brief imitative responsesin other voices. The second section, moderately slow andante!, isan embellishedupper-voice melody against whichthe bass voicemoves ina relation considerably subservientand far less intricate! but having contrapuntalvalue infrequent contradirectional relation toand in occasional imitative interactionswith the upper voice.The third section isovertly polyphonicconsistently imitative,a kind of invention,the sectionof far greatest contrarhythmic and contraintervalliccomplexity andintcrlinear independence. The lastsection ispresumably fast;tempo thusemerges as the mostapparent complementary element of delineation.With texturaland tempostructures, apalpable tonal structurelinks thethree sections into a broad progression centering ontonic and dominant levels;moreover, thetonal structureis oneof progressive degrees of fluctuation through the Preludesthree sections. There isthus complementarityamong the various relevantelement-structures atthe broadestarchitectonic level,as often at more foregroundlevels; thevarious structures so projectedconverge infunction to serve parallelends of formal delineation and progressiveintensification and subsequent release. JA furtheruseful example in this connection isthe third of SchoenbergsPiano Pieces, Op. 19,in all four phrases of which although minimallyin the second! contractionin
298 texture space isa significantfactor in structure andin cadentialformulation aswell asin the pieces overall shape smallest spaces occur in the third and fourth phrases!.The following showsthe spatial configurations ofthe four phrases withup-down distances given insemitones; note recurrences of 37 and27, ascompared withcadential spaces of 14, 12, ll:
If, I
Tl l.@4.,f ifji* b bv
ll
b*92l1'I/
*In the Bartok Divertimentofor stringorchestra, aninstance ofstriking verticalordering at mm. 58-61of the second movement! is attributableto texture,sonority, and coloration. Theaccompanying fabrichas tight density-compression close in spacing between double-bass and viola; and this thick textural backdrop, givena rumbling activation byrhythmic andarticulative diversity,is astriking foilindeed forthe void, two-octave doublingof the motive, presentedin muted violins. Here, the densitycompression can be significantlydescribed onlywith referenceto the distribution of events within the texture-space.Cf. other instances ofprovocative texture, e.g., mm. 66-68,in the same movement.! The madrigal, Moro, moro, first part, is suggested for furtherstudy ofsimilar questions. Again, formis delineated by linesof text,and themanipulation oftextural components in and out of cadential punctuations should beconsidered. The text inducesat Corre volando, literally runs flying! functional devicesof animationand texturalintensity comparable tothose ofFuggesi fiees,flies away! in Or, che ingioia. Aparticularly striking featureof thetextural structureof Moro,moro, perhaps recurrent inGesualdo, is thetendengr of each phrase to progress to evolve! texturalbr, both quantitatively and qualitatively, with interdependent relationsqualifying these paths of progressive diversification. Atm. 3,for example,voices 3and 5start thefirst process of diversification with homorhythmic but contradirectional relation;voices 2 and 3 are often doubled inrestraining theprogressive tendencies at phraselevel, asat mm. 4-6.! The tendency ofeach phrase to progress in thedirection oftextural diversity nowhere fully contrarhythmic-contraintervallic! isassociated with the settingof theintense opening words in typically arresting chromatic manner.These words I die, I die . _.! are, with heavy chromaticism, set inrestrained andsimple texture:they arethe focus of anguishedfeeling outof which the entirepoem andcomposition! arises. As commonly, the intense chromaticismsuggests atexture of relative simplicity-near homorhythmic, chordalsuccession. This restraint oftexture isfunctional notonly in exposing therich chromatic vocabulary, but in exposingthe crucial words of text too; in each phrase,with this initiating precedent,subsequent diversification of texture follows from this condition in a highly important, and intensely perceptible, aspect ofshape. The important textural issueof compoundline in this movementcannot really be developed inthe space available; butideally thisaspect oftextural diversityshould be pursued independently.Note, for example, theextraordinary contrapuntalinteraction atmm. 40-41,at dramaticallyopposed registers:
l
F
!
texture 29.9 In suchinstances there is truetextural diversitywithin ostensiblymonophonic texture, with interactionsof vital directional, rhythmic, and intervallic opposition inwhich dissonance strongly is felt.Understanding ofthis kindof concealed textural diversity and interactionis of course absolutely vital to performance, ifdissonant andother interactions ofthe sortsketched above are to be broughtout. F or further studyof Brahmsin theseand relatedconnections attention is drawn,for example, toconditions oftexture andtextural process in the shaping ofphrase structure in the openingof the Quintet in G, Op. lll, for strings. Anotherexample of great interestfor thestudy oftexture isthe Adagio of theTrio in Eb, Op.40, for piano, violin, and horn. Somefactors representative of theextraordinary rangeof significant textural processand functionin this movement canbe suggested in hasty summary. The role of textural events indelineation ofform is vitally important throughout. For example,the entry of contrastingthematic materialat m. 19 isthe subjectof startling exposure lgv virtue qf thechange texture, in quite probably themost arrestingof a complex of complementary element-events, and textural change isthroughout an important variational device in thematic developmentand recurrence.! Shaped successions textural of eventsare ofcompelling significance: for example,that of the progression from m. 19,which canbe tracedin variousstages, including that of contrapuntal interplayin which a time interval of imitation undergoescontraction from
12 ,D to 6f; continuation of the process, counteractive to diminuendo but complementary to extreme tonalshifts, achieves at m. 30 a time interval of 3 , with the motive undergoing complementary truncationin a manifestation of intensifying metric function.With the time intervalfluctuating between3 and 4- , the motive is variedin sizeand in placement inrelation to the bar-line:complementary actions are thoseof rhythmic motion e.g.,stringendo and increased activityin the piano!, metric instability, intense coloratione.g., morebrilliant violin and horn registers, and crescendo!, and especiallythe intenserise in the pitch-line; tonality undergoes compensatory stabilization but with rich chromatic embellishment maintained as in the Neapolitanharmony ofm. 35.There isgradual recession into theoriginal themes restatement eightmeasures afterm. 35, and thereare like processes and events of textural, structuralsignificance throughout the movement.But note,at m. 59ff., the condition ofextreme texturalstasis-absolute homorhythm and nearlyabsolute homodirectional association of horn and violin, as wellas virtualinactivity of the bass,all complementary to slowed harmonic rhythm, tonalstability, andrelaxed dynamic level. Toward m.74 starklysimple textureis counteractive, on theother hand,in relationto intensifying events of pitch-line, rhythmic acceleration,and especiallydynamic level, texture remaininginsistently andrigidly uncomplicated.! °No. 2 is a strict canonbetween thesinging voiceand the Eb clarinetat a distance of seven bars; and thecontinuation ofthe leaderis, fromm. 9,a retrogradeofthe original canonic subject. Thus, simultaneous with strict canon isa retrogradecanon between clarinet, asleader, andthe singingvoice concurrentwith it; the retrogradecanon is
at 3J . No. 6 of the Goethe-Liederbasically is a mirror canon, the canon in a sense framed by the tonally suggestive pitchrepetitions ofthe beginning singing voice on a! and end bass clarinetssettling onc!. Adjunctive to the canonic procedure are developments of the IC succession l-2as in Eb-Eh-D!, twicea trichordal subset of
the row:
IE' k', t t . The occur`;__l rences ofthis trichordalcellular setwill be quickly identifiedin analysis.With these recurrences, thetexture isof greaterdiversity andgreater complexityof interlinear interaction thanthat of No. 2; the anglesof subsidiarydiagonal relations change con-
30 0
f8XfUl'6 stantly evenwhile thecanon isin course.In fact, Dallapiccola concentrates altogether on the three-note subsetorder numbers1-2-3 or6-7-8! with the resultthat concurrent with the broadercanon isconstant imitationon that cellular unit--imitation in mirror andretrograde duplications and inmany rhythmic,directional, andintervallic variants. It is strikingthat only in m. 7 nearthe middle!do immediatelyconcurrent intervallic and directional formulationscorrespond, althoughin a contrarhythmic relation. Theprinciple of progressive, then recessive process in the framing of the very activecanon andimitated cellularunits, withmelodically andtonally morestatic sections of simpler texture,is functionallyimportant.
CHAPTER THREE
rhythm andmeter /ntroductory notes
All element-processesare rhythmic. In an important sense, the study of rhythm is thus the study of all musical elements, theactions ofthose elements producing the effects ofpace, pattern,and groupingwhich constitute rhythm. To study rhythm is to study all of music. Rhythm both organizes, and is itself organized by, all the elementswhich create and shapemusical processes.
While there are many compelling factors suggesting the critical importance of rhythmic and metric analysis, one of the most persuasive isthe fact that metric analysis, in its proper range of implications, is a vital basis of construction and interpretation of phrasingand articulationin performance. The study of rhythm, and especially meter, thus proceedsfrom questions which are truly indispensable to critical, insightful interpretation. Some of these questions canbe stated in the following ways: What are the chief events in the hierarchy of musical impulses in a given work, the points of intensity toward which others are oriented? Or, where are the points and where is thepoint! to which and _#om which musical processesare directed at various levels? A related questionwhich puts the issueof meter in a different way is: Where are
the true
bar-lines at
diverse levels in the
musical structure?
The present study inquires into many aspects of rhythm beyond that of meter, but questions like the foregoing are a recurrent, underlying preoccupation in the discussionsto follow. It may well be that rhythm and meter, seen asa part of rhythm, constitute the most persuasiveand immediately perceptible quality within the range of musical effect.The rates at which events changes!take placewithin the various structural parameters, and the patterns into which eventsgroup themselves, areof decisive significance in expressive effectin the musical experience. 'Grosvenor W.Cooper andLeonard B. Meyer, The Rhythmic Structure of Music © 1960 byThe Universityof Chicago!,p. 1; all rights reserved. 301
302 rhythm
and meter
The analyses and expository materials which follow will showbeyond somewhat ancillary references made in preceding chapters-how the operationsof rhythm and meter permeate andinfluence the entire range of elementsconstituting the musical projection. It has been suggested throughout this book that contrasts in the operations of various structural elements the element-structures! and the progressive and recessive linesof change in those elements underly morphology and meaning in music in one important sense. Rhythm too undergoes changeswith functional consequences formusics intensity scale, playing an essential andtelling role in the delineation of processes ofgrowth and decline, climax and subsidence, stabilityand flux. In this sense, itis no absurdity to speak, for example, of a metric rhythm asthe rate and pattern of metric change, or a temporhythm in music. Contrasting rhythmic events function with levels.
those of other elements to delineate musical structures
at all
A relatively recent effort toward comprehensive theoretical treatment of rhythm gives necessary acknowledgmentto the subjectivities and complexities of rhythmic interpretation. Rhythmic grouping is a mental fact, not a physical one. There are no hard and fast rules for calculating what in any particular instance the grouping is. Sensitive, well-trained musicians maydiffer. Indeed, it is this that makes performance an art-that makes different phrasings and different interpretations of a piece of music possible. Furthermore, grouping may at times be purposefully ambiguous and must be thus understood rather than forced into a clear decisive pattern. In brief, the interpretation of music-and this is what analysis should be-is an art requiring experience, understanding,and sensitivity.2 Because every rhythm isunique, havingits own organization and hence its own particular analytic problems, no selection of examplescan possibly cover the rhythmic permutations and analytic problems which may arise. The task of organization . _. becomesmore difficult once one leaves the hothouse variety of example behind and ventures forth into the world of real music. Here, variables do not operate singly. Nor is it generally possible to classify the rhythm of a given example under a single simple category. Not only do groupings vary from one architectonic level to another, but particularly on lower levels changes of grouping are the rule rather than the exception.3
We shall have to be continually aware that the importance of a theoretical problem is not invalidated bythe dwicultiesof approaches to solution 2Cooper andMeyer, TheRhythmic Structure of Music,p. 9. Cooper and Meyer, op.cit., p. 60. Secalso Howard E. Smither, The Rhythmic Analysis of20th-Century Music, in journalof MusicTheory, VIII,l 964!, pp. 69-70.
rhythm and meter 303
or by the uncertainties or equivocations of proposed solutions. Indeed,the significance ofa question is at times inverselyrelated to its simplicity of treatment. In many instances inprior studies, we have had occasionin analysis to note the factor of interpretive judgment, not of course capricious or arbitrary, citing criteria of analysis in support of such judgment, as opposed to the determination of right answers to the questions from which analysis proceeds. The range of significant, plausible interpretations of rhythmic structure is often of particular breadth and diversity; and the possible
validity ofdiffering conclusions must benoted asan importantobject of analysis. The subjective and often elusive criteria at the root of particular rhythmic interpretations are especially evident in the study of accentdelineated metric
structure.
The awesomecomplexity of problems ofrhythmic structure and analysis canbe seenwhen one appreciates thatrhythm isa generic factor, one aspect of which is meter. Yet meter is onb one ofnumerous manyestations ty' grouping. And meter, as conceivedhere, is dependent on accent-a phenomenon whose existence noone would deny, yet in which many qualities of impulse event, attack! interact variously at different levels of structure. Thus, questions of rhythm lead at some point to questions of grouping, and in turn to questions ofmeter, which rests uponthe difficult questions ofaccent. Inevitably, one manifestation of the complexity of rhythmic theory is the problem of terminology, the problem of the medium by which theoretical discourseis carried on in expression ofbasic concepts.The meanings of such words as rhythm, accent, and meter, not to mention terms like syncopation or even duration Does the duration of a rhythmic impulse include surrounding silences?!, are variously usedin the literature on music. A constant effort is made in the present study to establish fundamental premiseslinking essential terms and concepts, with terms defined so that their use, if not standard or conventional, will
be clear.
The broad basis for the present study might be shown graphically as in Fig. 3-1, in which rhythm is presented as a generic classof pacing,patterning, andpartitioning events in music;a facet of rhythm is grouping, a subcategory of which is represented asmeter. The position of meter in the graphic representation in Fig. 3-1 must not be misconstrued asindicative of subsidiary importance. The fact is that a very significant, often perceptually immediate, functional grouping is delineated in music by accent, by an events superiority of content and projection in which surrounding anticipative and reactive! events are absorbed into an accent-governedmetric unit. When accent-delineated grouping is firmly established, opposing accents, which are very necessary andusual in interesting music, may be felt as syncopated against counteractive to! a prevailing, precondition-
OV tp Cg hs V he N
V lO0 EO lg
VV
~~
Ul !C lO EO tO E La
0 O. tD
V 0C .
I-I IL
rhythm and
meter 305
ing, still underlying metric basis. Or counteraccent, imposed against a pattern of units not referentially establishedand preconditioning, may bring about fluctuation in the metric structure: contiguous and concurrent units thus emerge in Huctuant,
Fundamenta/ concepts
asymmetrical relations.
of rhythm
While it is sometimesbroadly described as ordered time or meter, it
seems essential that rhythm
narrowly as
be viewed as both manifold and
specific: the sum of a broad range of factors eachof which is in some way a manifestation of pace and grouping, the former a product of relative frequencies of events, the latter of their relative qualities and the means by which they are unit-ordered. The factors of rhythm can be detailed as follows. Rhythm is:
l. Tempo, which has two aspects: the eventfulness of music degree to which the temporal continuity and flow are filled with articulate impulses or related silences! andthe frequency of pulsation at some given level. In the latter aspect, note the recurrent concept of a norm of tempo-e.g., that of a physiological manifestation like heartbeat or stride.! We shall refer to these two aspectsof tempo as activity-tempo and pulse-tempothe degree of eventfulness andthe rate of pulse succession, respectively!; tempo is thus the quality of rhythmic motion and drive. As to controlled, functional changes in activity-tempo, see Ex. 2-22, pp. 250-52, one of many extracts cited in which the factor of shaped degreesof eventfulness is described as essential infunctional effect.! 2. Pattern or motive cfi rhythmic mode!, as expressed indurational and other strong-weak combinationswhich have in a given context motivic significance at some level, or characteristics of pattern by which a style, genre, or work can usefully be identified. Rhythmic pattern is explicit Pulse will be understoodas thefelt, underlying,at timesregularly recurrentunit by which musicstime spanis measuredand its divisions feltat somespecified level-the basis for counting, or conducting, and for metronomic indicationsof tempo. Pulses recur regularly only at certainlevels inmost music,conspicuously at the levelof the notated bar; some music however, notablyliturgical chantof theMiddle Ages,must beregarded as indifferent toregular pulsation. Impulse isregarded hereas the event itself attack, stimulus,integral silence!superimposed onand relatedto the stream ofpulsation. 5As toshaped controlof pulse-tempo relations, see Robert Ericksonsdiscussion of his Duo for violin andpiano in_journal of Music Theory, VII, 2, 963!, pp. 174-92. Inthis poignant andarticulate article,entitled Time-Relations, Erickson states and enlargesupon his creative interestin thecontemporary relevance and usefulness of aconcept ofaHuid incommensurable! time field of which the traditional fermatais representative.
306 rhythm
and meter
especially in the sequenceof attack and changeby which the musical line is delineated; an important item within this area of rhythmic effect and identity is that in which motivic units become recurrent within broader units and fundamental to the delineation of musical form e.g., isorhythms, etc.!. 3. The prqfiles expressed in element-changes manifest individually and in confluence! as thesechanges involvepattern, rate, and degreeof change. Thus, melodic rhythm normally seen asthe rate and pattern of pitch change or attack without pitch change! within a line, harmonic rhythm, textural rhythm, and other element- and subelement-rhythmsconstitute this aspect of rhythmic experience. 4. Grouping,or partitioning of musics time span by associationsperceived within and among punctuated or articulated unit-orderings of events. One mode of grouping is meter; it is, like tempo, one of rhythms most telling aspects.
Rhythm as activity and
mot/'on
Although it is not to be a subject of extensive attention in this chapter, rhythm as the motor aspect of music pace, drive, activity-tempo!-the energy dispelled in relation to the extent of eventfulness in the succession of impulses-is exceedingly important and fundamental in musical structure and eifect.
The control of rhythmic activity is among themost elementalof musics shaping forces,one of the most direct of functional devices, comparable in immediacy to the control of dynamic intensity.° The experience ol, for example, activity-tempo acceleration in the direction of developmentalclimax, or deceleration signalling release,is a fact to which everyone conditioned to interesting musical experiencewill testify? An example of the shaping power of controlled changes in rhythmic motion activity-tempo! is afforded by the first movement of Weberns Op. 22, to which reference was made in Chapter 2 Exx. 2-22, and 3-l!. Analyses of other element-progressionstoward the climactic mm. 22-23 can be complemented by illustration of composite activity. Pulse-tempo is constant, but there is a distinct progressive curve in activity-tempo mounting toward m. 22 and receding toward the fermata between mm. 27 and 28, the subsidentaim of recessive operations following m. 22. In Ex. 3-1 °The quality of rhythmic motion can be an important and essential factorin the identification andcharacterization of style. Forexample, many literatures ofthe Baroqueare marked by relatively constantactivity-tempo ascompared with,for example,the more romantic, changeable impulses ofcertain otherstyles!.
rhythm and meter 307
there are three viewsof the rhythmic progression.The first of these,perhaps more a representation ofsignificant textural accrual, showsthe rate of attack of two- and three-note motives which are a basic unifying material; the second and third portions of the example represent in different ways the composite motion or rate and spacing of attack. The upper line shows this
by indicating the frequency of occurrence of rests separated abby distances of 3, 3, 2, ll, 15, 6, 5, 2, 3, 1, 1! and the lower line by indicating frequencies and durations of notes anywhere in the texture. The example is characteristic.
Ex. 3-1. Representationof activity-tempocurve ina passage from Webern's Quartet, Op.22, first movementsee Ex.2-22, pp.251-52, forextracted quotations!. m.l9 m.22
Motive 3
4
83
?§l2f.'2..¢, »» » R3
3 ll
E »»
»» »
35
|!» -1!!
3 fa
e eensusul-ns ~ -1 ' |! I6
gene!:/ 92 est
frequency
I units -.
i2
2 if _'_
liiiniiiiii liiiiiiiii-if-iii
. ./.
3I I
_L -ng*
lliiil _T
.
Recessive and progressive rhythmic actions and relations, tightly controlled and directed to and from relative levelsof stability, can be seen in works of Elliott Carter. Carter is known for a procedureof controlled change in tempo described as metric modulation by which pulse andpulse rate change gradually, often at frequent intervals, in a procedure determining an aspect of tempo-structure at various levels. This method of covert tempo change is illustrated in a passage from the String Quartet No. l Ex. 3-2a!. One factor in the quoted passage is contractionin the low-level metric unit. Compare the changes in activity-tempo as well as=. pulse-tempo; of the two aspects oftempo, it can beseen thatfirst they run parallel, then while there is acceleration in pulse-tempo activity-tempo is held back before the progressive actionsof m. 22.! Example 3-2b traces the process toward an
accelerated Pulse 4| from 72 MM to 120 MM.
308
rhythm and meter
Ex. 3-2a. Carter,StringQuartetNo. 1, first movement.
Maestoso J - 72
m. 15
Reprinted bppermission of Associated MusicPublishers, Inc.
rhythm and meter
309
gx. 3-2Q. "Metric modulation"in the Carterexample,
J=G.! i. e., MM 72
288
3=!! 9'.,
or 360
360
sP D 120
10 In a changed metersignature, >6. 6
Some composershave used notational spacesbetween sounds as they are representedon the printed page as impreciseguidesto the temporal distancesby which they are to be separatedin performance.One such example of proportionalnotation,but in which mensural temporal intervals are very specifically indicated, is the Berio Sequenza II for harp. Here, durations within notated "measures" are suggestedonly by relative distancesseparating the printed notes, but each measure is to correspond to a metronomic pulse at 40MM, so that tempo is rigidly controlled at that level. Metric units at phraselevels are controlled too, in significant degree,by their consequent relative durations, differences of pitch content, differences of intensities, punctuative devices like specified silencesof specific or proportional duration, or abrupt dampening of sound, etc.!, and other shaping factors. The element of chance in this work is, then, relatively minimal, amounting to a modestlatitude of possibilitiesin which basic rhythmic structure is relatively inviolate.
In the first phrase Ex. 3-3a!, within a stasisof pitch-line and opposed counterpoint of intensity changes,the rate of attack is one of acceleration, then deceleration in a recessive cadential action of palpable functional effect. This can be expressedvery simply Ex. 3-3b! as a progressive,then Ex. 3-3a. Berio,Sequenze II for harp. 40MM~
! 1965, Universal Edition.Usedbypermission of thepublisher.Theodore Presser Company, sole representative UnitedStates, Canada andMexko.
310 rhythm
and meter
recessive rateof increase and decrease in events per stipulated, regularly recurrent unit
of time
at 40MM,
the distribution
of events
within the
mensural unit suggested bygraphic proportioning but not specifically controlled." Comparable processes characterizeother phrases. Ex. 3-3b. Accelerative anddecelerative rhythmic changes inthe first phrase ofthe Berio Sequenza ll.
Number events of + per 40 MM unit:
10
22
+ + 2 -2 24
66
-2 42
l _._
Rhythmic pattern
as mot/'v/'c
Especially at more immediate levels of structure it is readily apparent that durational and other weak-strong combinations and distributions have great and constant importance in the projection of distinctive, identifiable, associable motivicand thematic ideas which are exposedand developed in the music of most Western styles. Rhythmic patterns have in various systems undergone theoretical classification, often as rhythmicmodes corresponding to and derived from the classes and terminology of standard feet of ancient Greek poetic theory. The most common of these modesare six: trochaic long-short, or strongweak: - U!, iambic U -!, dactylic U U!, anapestic U U - !, spondaic - - !, and tribrachic U U!. Other modes e.g., amphibrach: U - U ! are noted in some systems. 7At the same time,the metricformulation within the phraseclearly hasto beseen as one ofvertical noncongruitybetween thetwo linearcomponents of pitch activity, the lower stratum havingaccent atits inception,the upperhaving anextensive anticipative wind-up in preparationof its later, noncongruentaccent. 8For briefdiscussion ofespecially thirteenth-century!rhythmic modal theory, and some bibliographicalreference, see Willi Apel, Harvard Dictionary qf Music,2nd ed. Cambridge: TheBelknap Press of HarvardUniversity Press,1969!, pp.535-36. Whilerhythmic modal theoryis of great importancein certain music for example, inthirteenth-century motets, wherethe applicationof suchtheory isoften highlysystematized!, its applications in music broadlyviewed caneasily beoveremphasized, with a resultantsimplistic viewof what is commonlyhighly complexand diversifiedrhythmic organization.This is not to say that analysis andidentification or conjecture! respecting the substance of rhythmic pattern at various levels has novalidity; but it is a cripplingpreoccupation when it is taken asthe end of analysisin which rhythms areconsidered invariably reducible toone ofa fewtraditional classifications, ormodes. Musicalsituations ofreal interestpose acomplex variability of patterned associations not readily or usefully classified withina limited range of assumed referential norms.Nevertheless, the classifiable modal rhythmic pattern may at times be relevant togeneric combinations upon which thematic substance and structuralprinciples are based ; andthe traditionalterminology andsymbology of rhythmic modesare oftenuseful.
rhythm and meter
311
There are, as might well be expected, instances of applications of other systemic "modes" indeed, the theoretical possibilities are without limit. In certain of his works, for example, Olivier Messiaen, a figure of considerable importance in contemporary explorations of rhythmic technique, employs rhythmic patterns derived from exotic systems.In Ex. 3-4 the music is based on three Hindu rhythms two of which undergo pre-
scribed transformations eachattack ofthefirstincreased bya sIIateach repetition, durations of the second decreasedby the same value at each repetition, and the third reiterated without change. In the brief extract, the threemotivesare circled; in their immediaterepetitions,prescribeddurational modifications are indicated.
Ex.3-4. Messiaen,Reprises parinterversionNo. 1 of Livred'orgue!.
Man.
Ped.
Reprinted bypermission of Alphonse Leaguc 8 Cie.,OwnersO' Publishers for all count's.
312
rhythm and meter
The concepts of rhythmicostinatoand isorhpthtathe latter denoting a recurrent set and ordering of durational values, commonly in tenors of
early motets! relate to another practiceof rhythm applied motivically. Every student of music is familiar with isorhythmic structures, for example,
in many fourteenth-century motets,'althoughmore recent applicationsof analogoustechniquesare of comparable interest and, perhaps, significance. The third movement of Webern s Variations, Op. 27, for piano illustrates the isorhythmic principle: thus, the opening four bars plus one undergo immediate isorhythmic repetition. That is, the initial twelve attacks are duplicated in rhythm by the next twelve Ex. 3-5!. The application of rhythm as thematic ' is again explicit and significant as a parallel to other relations in concurrent effect in the Webern example e.g., that of the twelve-tone series,occurring as R" on EI in the first twelve attacks, then as RI on El> in the next twelve!.
Ex.3-5. Webern,Variations,Op.27, for piano,third movement.
RuhigfliessendJ - ca.80!
Copyright 1937,Universal Edition.Usedbypermission of thepublisher.Theodore Presser Company,solerepresentative UnitedStates, Canada andMexico.
SeeHistoricalAnthology of Music,Apel and Davison,eds. Cambridge:Harvard UniversityPress,1949!,Vol. I, Nos.43 and 44, for examples.A fascinatingstudyof isorhythmic andsymmetricalproceduresin Machauts MesseNotre-Dame, including discussionof remarkably consistentcenter-axial"horizontal" symmetriesin the work and extendingto treatment of comparabletechniquein other works,is Otto Gombosi,"Machaut's MesseNotre-Dame," in TheMusical!uarterly,XXXVI, 2 950!, pp. 204-24.
rhythm and meter 313
One could of course continue to cite relevant examples of motivicthematic rhythm at various levels ofstructure throughout the vast historical span of Western music. Consider, for further reference, the fifth symphony, or the sixth, of Beethoven as to rhythmic motive, in extreme foreground manifestations, having vital and consistent significance. And the entire range of techniques associatedwith the serialization of rhythms and rhythmic relations is of course relatedto the principle of motivic rhythm; some of these arenoted near the end of this chapter. But concerns ofmotivic pattern are recurrent too in plentiful studies of thematic process in musical form, especially in literatures dealing with the tonal period. It seems important, then, to move on to more problematic areas of rhythmic theory.°
The rhythms of e/ement-successions
We have reiterated the important principle that every structural element is, in its distributions andqualities qfevents, expressive qf rhythm. There is thus a rhythm of pitch-line rhythm seen ofcourse asincluding meter!, a harmonic rhythm, a tonal rhythm, and a rhythm of each of the other elements and parameters ofmusical events.These rhythmsare preeminently: ! of pacing or tempo-the rate of event articulation and change; ! of pattern, as manifest in varying durational combinations; ! of proportions, comparative durational relations amongunits, or groups ofevents; and ! of relative qualities of events and event-successions-degrees distances! of change, of accent."
The extent to which element-rhythms function concurrently in complementation to or compensation for progressive and recessive,underlying tendencies is invariably striking and illuminating as to function and expressive effect.Take, for example, a brief example from the Diabelli Variations of Beethoven Ex. 3-6a!. Quickened, sometimes chromatic, Men double-dotted thusmotivic! harmonic rhythm in the Beethoven example reveals action within this parameter decisively complementary to an overall rise in pitch-line, a tonal progression toward the mediant typical of much Beethoven, and a variation on progression toward the dominant in the theme!, and a driving creseendo. Balanced against all of this is the extraordinary poise of constant melodic and iterative motivic rhythms within a slow tempo-the entire complex of element-structures a confluence of deeply affective character. A comparable pattern pertains in the second halfof the variation. As to the harmonic-rhythmic parameter, the reader should note in detail the progression out of broad tonic, dominant! values of the beginning, characteristic of the theme, into controlled
acceleration.
Let us focus briefly on mm. 4-8 of the variation. Of harmonic rhythm
314
rhythm and meter
Ex. 3-6a. Beethoven, Thirty-threeVariationson a Waltzof Diabelli,Op.120,VariationXIV. Grave e maestoso
it can be said that all changesare pronounced in degree; roots are usually a 5th apart, once a tritone, once a 2nd the latter in the cadential iv-V!. In the harmonic rhythm the rate of successionacceleratesfrom to
etc. The tonal rhythm is characterized by references to F, and e the F-e successionrelatively strong in distance!; a brief reference to a is also crowded into thesesame bars. Hence, there is tonal-rhythmic eventfulness of strength and frequency. Within the element of coloration, as it concerns dynamic intensity, there is an increasedrate of change
f
so< P f
P ! inmm. 5-8 ascomparedwith the
broad pace of dynamic change in mm. 1 4.
316 rhythm
and meter
over, while density undergoesonly slight changes,- theshape delineatedby space and texture-compression! is demonstrably functional: note, for example, the essential stepsuccessions of upper voicesin mm. 4-8 as compared with the ascending successions by leap in the bass. In summary, the general activity profile within various elements biseets the example ina manifestation of the role of shaping element-rhythms to projectgrouping in a broad sense analogous to thoseof meterand phraseolog_y.° The vital importance of study of the conjoined interactions of rhythmic element-structures in music is evident in references to complementary and compensatory relations of concurrent element-successionsthroughout this book. At the same time, it must be acknowledgedthat these considerations, and with them the considerations of individual
and confluent element-
and subelement-rhythms," require exploration far beyond the possibilities of this book.
It is apparent that element-rhythms correspond in diverse ways: some are of course totally or substantially inapplicable in particular situations while others are of primary assertiveness andimportance; some may be accelerative and others decelerative or passive, relatively inactive, or static! in particular contexts, with the weight of movement in one or another of thesedirections, or in balance, or in inert condition; and someare clearly more applicable at lower levels of structure e.g., pulse-tempo!. It is also apparent that proportional groupings or units manifest in areas within individual element-rhythms, and their interrelations of asymmetry or symmetry and relative strengths of projection, constitute a fundamental aspect of felt rhythmic effect. Moreover, all element-rhythms are subject to analysis and interpretation at various levels; thus, for example, harmonic rhythm functions at the level of most immediate detail but also at the level of broad changes. For example, theessential prolongation of a dominant may represent in one sense broadly inactive harmonic rhythm and in another, more local sense a foreground harmonic rhythm of activating, embellishing changes. An ideal and fully comprehensive analysis, if such were possible, would deal with all element-rhythms individually and collectively, and with their interrelations-all of them projecting and the analysis of all of them illuminating! that complex, many-faceted, element-structure which is the rhythm ofthe musical example in question. 1°See pp. 78-79 ontonal rhythm; Ex. l-45, p. 137,on tonalrhythm in the finalmovement of Beethovens Symphony No. 2; pp. 201-4on textural rhythm; Ex. 2-5, p. 202, as to certain aspects oftextural rhythm; and other recurrent references to element-structures as rhythmic.
rhythm and meter 317 A theoretical approach to the consideration accent-de/ineated grouping
of meter as
An iterative or neutral! rhythm pertains among successive, regularly spaced impulses stimuli! in a series lacking parametric changesof any kind. But functional rhythmic shapes involve grouping occasioned by dWrentiations among contiguousevents. The question of meteris the question ofaccent, since metric units are initiated by accents, impulses of relatively strong projection. The question of accent is a very complex one indeed; nevertheless, itis an indispensable issue for the analysis of this vital aspect of rhythmic structure in music." In embarking on a study of meter, we shall make the assumptions! that in the vast majority of instances musicaleffect rests upon the experienced orpreconditioned, imaginedsensation ofa seriesof pulsespunctuating and articulating the time continuum as a psychologicalfact not necessarily manifest in physical events, such pulsesreferential as a mental imagery in the musical experience, anda basisfor perception of the rhythmic relations of events; ! that impulses-the actual sounds andsilences ofwhich musical projection consists-are superimposed on the established, felt stream of pulsation, and commonly groupedby distinctions of variouskinds; and ! that metric partitioning is one of severalkinds of grouping in music. Thus, the analysis of meter involves certain primary questions: ! Which stimuli are perceived as accentual? ! What is the nature of the structure effected by metric grouping and the interrelations among its units? ! What is the weak-strongorganization by which the metric group is characterized at any given level of structure? ! What is the relation of metric partitioning to other modes of grouping, and of metric structure to other element-structures? From these diflicult but fundamental questions, of course, many highly important subquestions issue; moreover,as implied above, all such questions are subject to diverse implications of different levels ofstructure ranging from the most immediate apparent in the smallest manifestations ofgrouping! to the most broad in which it is at least conceivable that an entire form might usefully beregarded ashaving a palpable organization-e.g., weak-strong~weak-of significant, essentialeffect!. "One trusts,in usingthe termaccent, that it is clear nosuggestion of overt emphasis in performanceis necessarily implied; indeed,the propertiesof accentare generallyinherent in the various parameterspitch, duration, etc.! of the eventitself Sometimes,especially at .higher levels ofmetric structure, subtle and restrained interventionin performanceis necessary to bring out important metric functions. Butoften nointervention isindicated at all, onlythe avoidance of counterinclinations of timing,articulation, andpunctuation. These are vitaljudgments required in performance;but to say thata particularimpulse event!has accentual value is generallyto makean observationrespecting itsinherent properties.
318 rhythm
and meter
Meter, then, consists ofunits large and small at various structural levels! _farmed by d@rentiations in the musical events inwhat we shall describeas diverse impulse functions. If there is diiferentiation it is expressed in some parameter or complex of parameters. Meteris thataspect of structure articulated as accent-delineated groupings within the attack event!sequence, and the proportional interrelations of such groups at all levels. Metermay be symmetrically ordered, asymmetrically ordered, or ambiguous in its ordering. Moreover, it may be symmetrical at one level, asymmetrical at another; and of courseindividual textural componentsare oftenof contrastingmetric ordering at the same level. Thus, the analysis of metric structure, whose application in examples in various styles is the principal concern of this chapter, is the evaluation, identification, and/or interpretation of its two primary factors: ! pattern within the metric unit, its weak-strongcomponents andassociations, and! the accentual
articulation of
the units
themselves, with
consideration of
their proportional interrelations. The functions of meter in these respects are a necessary, constantconcern to which analysis should ideally proceed, involving descriptive conclusion or conjecture respectingthe role of meter, and the functions of its varying qualities, in shaping the expressive content as well as the structural unity and diversity of music. To retrace in summary the broadeststeps thathave led to this point in the consideration of rhythmic-metric structure, it seems desirableto note again that rhythm is regarded as a generic class of techniques affecting pacing, patterning, and grouping of all events at all levels; that meter is one of this classof techniquesand phenomena,defined asaccent-delineated grouping whose structural and expressive functions are manifest in proportional relations of metric units whether contiguous or concurrent! and the weak-strong associations internal to such units; that meter, like other aspects ofrhythm, is felt at various structural levels, perhaps even at the ultimate macrolevel; and that meter is not to be equated with regularity, so that metric fluctuation,
however extreme, is not meterlessness."
The concept of meter as, by dehn/tion, subject
to fluctuation
True metric structure is neither necessarily regular nor necessarily coincident with notated bar-lines at the mensural level." A great deal of It is possible thatthe term arnetric has useful applicabilityanalogous tothat of the term atonal,with referenceto structuresof relatively extreme instabilityand ambiguity. This study will adopt and employthroughout certainterms whichrefer to classes of structurallevel in which thenotated measure is regardedas aconvenient objective point of reference:mensural refers to groupingapproximate to, or derived hom, or otherwise demon-
rhythm and meter 319
interesting and expressive musicis of irregular accentuation, ry' irregularmetric grouping. In situations of altered interval of accentuation, meter fluctuates, and the idea of Huctuant asopposed to regular meter is absolutely-necessary to the understanding of music other than that of unequivocally regular proportions. It is on this basisthat the assertion ismade that in highly fluctuant contexts like those of recent stylesmeter is not extinguished, but assumesa character more often far more! Huctuant than those of certain traditions. We shall see as well that even much music commonly regarded, in submission to the deceptive impression of the notated signature and bar-line, as regular in metric grouping is indeed highly variable atleast atcertain levels. We thus avoid any suggestion ofa terminological equation in which meter = regularity of unit grouping.
Meter as one manifestation
of grouping
in music
We have systematically revieweda number of factors as rhythm-aspects of musical structure generically classified asrhythmic. One of these has to do with relations and interactions among, and weak-strong orderings strably corresponding in scopeto thatof thenotated measure without ofcoursenecessary conformity ofaccent tonotated bar-line!;intramensural refers to smallerunits andlower levels; intermensural refers to broaderunits andhigher levels.There isnothing absolute,as weshall see, in the identification ofthe mensural unit; often, to describea particular unit or accent, or level! as ofmensural significance is tomake judgment a of comparability relations. cy' Inthat judgment, complementary motivic groupingis oftenrelevant, asmay bethe notatedbar-line. "Compare, forexample, concepts of meterstated orimplied inEdward Cone,Musical Form and Musical Performance New York: W. W. Norton and Company, Inc.,1968!, p.82; Cooper andMeyer, TheRhythmic Structure of Music,p. 4; or Smither,The Rhythmic Analysis of20th-Century Music, in journalof MusicTheory, VIII,pp. 7| and 72.The Smitherand Cooper-Meyer comments in this regard describemeter asonly primarily, 'not necessarily, regular; andof courseHuctuant meteris at times considered rhythmic, not metric. In an interesting study entitled Extra Measuresand Metrica1Ambiguityin Beethoven [in Beethoven Studies, Alan Tyson, ed. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc., 1973!, pp.44-66], Andrew Imbrie makes on p. 53! the following statement:The idea that oncean organizationin time becomes flexible rather thanmechanical itthereby becomes rhythmic rather than metrical stems from the notion that meter has to be absolutely regular. Imbries own proposal as to a useful distinction betweenrhythm and meter proceeds fromthat important qualification, inwhich heasserts thatmeter isnot necessarily regular. The identificationof meter with recurrent,regular denominators renders extremely problematic the understanding ofmetric structure in highly Huctuant low-levelcontexts before and afar the tonal period;moreover, it induces asimplistic view of interestingtonal music, whosemetric structureis sooften morevaried andfluctuant than meter signatures would suggest, especially athigher levels.It is argued herethat a concept ofmeter ispossible and valid! which isapplicable tomusical experience in general,even tocontexts ofhighly fluctuant metersuch asthose ofRenaissance polyphony and the present century. Compare the increased ratesand distancesof fluctuation in tonal reference, in melodic line,and in other elements.
320 rhythm
and meter
within, units manifest in grouping of a number of kinds. One of the phenomena by which events are grouped is that of accent, relative impulse superiority, in relation to which surrounding impulses at various levels can be seen as reactive, anticipative anacrustic!, and conclusive Meter is thus an aspect of grouping, or partitioning, which is in turn a vital aspect of rhythm. We might identify the following as factors ly' which, ingeneral, events in music are perceived as grouped into leveled units of structure. 1. The grouping of class-ajiliated element-events is one such mode. For example, eventssubsumed withina particular tonal system,embraced within a given tonal reference,are grouped in this sense, as are, for example, events within a particular kind of textural activity e.g., imitation!, within a particular timbral unity, within a generalized melodic inclination, or within an harmonic complex of associated factors,etc. In Ex. 3-6, bisection within certain element-structures-e.g., tonality-is on this basisas wellas onthose of phraseology and other modes of grouping.! 2. The grouping ry'tendency-ajiliated events crossing distinctionsbetween elements isanother mode of unit delineation. For example, a concentration of events an element-complex! functioning in the direction of intensity within a given, unified process atsome level, is a factor of partitioning. In this sense,the Beethoven excerpt cited earlier Ex. 3-6! is bisected in the marked acceleration of conjoined, intensifying element-eventfulness in its second half This factor might be characterized asgrouping by the delineating effect of the composite profile of change in degreesof activity, in the line of element-rhythms; a segment of that line, where it can be seen to have unity in relation to a given progressive or recessive tendency, is an important area of grouping in this special sense. 3. The most subtle factor of grouping and partitioning, and perhaps the most elusive of perception and penetration, is the groupingof pitch structures at various levels in associations of linearfunction. Such grouping is expressed in the orientations of relatively auxiliary pitch events toward and around more essential ones, the sortof associationmuch discussed in Chapter 1. At one level of structure this can be seen asthe punctuation of broad time spans as to relative durations of structural prolongations-manifestations of predominances! of underlying, background events-the areas of hegemony of certain, elaboratedevents. This concept isof courseclosely related in much music to item 1, above, where the specific element of tonality is concerned; but it has to do as well with grouping around structural pitch factors in melody and harmony whether or not such factorsare supportive of, and predictable within the norms oi, given systems oftonality.! The unit delineated hereis one consisting ofthe essentialpitch event again, at some
rhythm and meter 321
given level! together with affiliated eventsby which it is elaborated, and by which its manifestations arelinked in passing motions. This mode of segmentationis particularly important to distinguish from what we regard asmeter: thus, the event which is central to the intrarelated linear complex, like the event central to the set of tonally related events, is often central to a specific tonal system; but that event is by no means necessarily thebearer of accent. Weshall find, for example, that the cadence
on l, which has of course that degree as central to the linear-functional grouping of eventspreceding it,is very often nonaccentual-recessivein the metric sense. That is to say that the cadential event is always central within the unit to which the cadence pertains!to the group definedas tofunctional, linear pitch affiliations or, in tonal music, to the tonally centered unit!; but it is often not a point of accent central in delineation of the metric unit see Exx. 3-7 and 3-8!. Ultimately, the tonally-cadentially central event is metrically recessive atpertinent levels, a point on which we shall dwell at a subsequent stage in further theoretical discussion ofthe distinction between primacy of tonal and linear function as opposed to metric accent. Within the concepts developed here, an often assumed necessary interdependence, evenequivalence, betweenbackground tonal and metric structures issharply denied. 4. Extramusicalfactors may condition, even determine, the grouping of musical events in still another sense. Textis of particularly critical importance in much music in unit delineation at diverse levels. See Ex. 2-14, where the setting of text is functional in bringing about a recessivesuccession of units expressedboth textually and in musical meters.! The kind of condition shown
in Ex. 3-7, in which textual
and accentual
musical inflec-
tions exactbtcorrespond, is by no means universal in vocal music. Textual accents on thou at the broadest
level, on me at another level,
and on stole
at the lowest level are in perfectly balanced concordance in relation to musical accents of pitch and duration, as can be seen in the example as represented at three different levels of metric structure. Other modes of grouping, too, are in correspondence, but the distinction between the pri-
mary linear function ofGzl and I, as opposed the to metric accent with which the phrase begins, isclearly evident.! That the issue of textual infiuences in grouping is one of substantial complication will be evident in other examples to be cited. One literature in which this issue is of very great and not well understood! significance might be mentioned in passing-that of Renaissance polyphony, where grouping of all kinds is often of very greatly Huctuant character and complexity, the nature of fluctuation of meter in an important degree the outcome of prosodic infiections which often have to be decided in editing and in
performance, and
which are commonly for
example in josquin! in
rhythm and meter 323 Ex. 3-8. Haydn, Symphony No. 104in D,fourth movement. m_3 Allegro spiritoso
l J,
# J.
J ,J 2/ 3,/` oo.»-- TJJ J
g ,Q
J
*See discussion of contrastinggrouping oflinear functions, pp. 320-21,and below.
In the Haydn excerpt Ex. 3-8!, as in the Dowland phrase quoted earlier, phraseology corresponcls meter: to thatis to say that the phrasebegins at the same timeas thephrase-level metricunit." The initial event is regard-
ed asaccentually superior for eachphrase-as initiatinga metricunit at the same time that it initiates the phrase. We shall considerother examplesin which this correspondence does not pertain.!
Group unity,moreover, is simply andclearly evidentin the afiiliating relations of all eventsin the Haydn theme to eachof severalelement-classes: tonality, texture, and color.
The metricand phraseological ordering themetric structure at phrase level evidentin thefact thata bar-line for eachphrase would immediately precede the first impulse!are inopposition tointernal orderingof grouping as to linear functions, here of course tonally conditioned. An analysis of the phrasesas to orientations of tonally subordinate events to essential ones
would regard the cadentialnote of each phraseas fundamental-points toward which other eventsincline in underlying stepwisedescent. For
the entireperiod, thelast note,l or harmony, I!is the essential event toward which all others, at the level of the period, incline, the cadential
Q V! of thefirst phrase a broadlyviewed auxiliary. But theultimate cadential eventis recessive at all but themensural and lower levels of metric structure. "The symbolsused inexplication ofmetric structureat variouslevels roughlycorrespond tostandard conducting gestures, except that the final impulseof the metric unit is represented differently depending onits functionin endingthe unit as opposed to functionin anticipation of thrust toward,anacrustic preparation of! the next unit. The symbolsare in no wayprescriptions forconducting, norare theyconsidered tohave anycomparable practical applicability;but theyare usefulas away ofthinking about relations within the metric unit. BT hecorrespondence of metric structureto phraseologymay well be characteristic of Haydnmore than,say, Mozart.
324 rhythm
and meter
A great deal of distortion in musical performanceis accountable,we believe, to failure to appreciate this usual metric function of cadential arrival."
Meter as opposed to the notated bar-//ne
It is fundamental that meter is often independent of the notated bar-line, so that a necessaryquestion in all analysis of meter is: Are the determinants of metric grouping in accord with the notated bar-line, and if not what is the real meter? Or, where is the true bar-line ?! A simple kind of accelerating metric change is illustrated by Ex. l-3, pp. 35-36, to which reference shouldbe made.Through much of this Chopin prelude the triple division of the notated bar is upheld by accentual mensural
factors of various kinds including the J anacrusis at theoutset!. But at m. 29 the triple unit yields to a duple division in intensifying contraction of the metric unit complementary to the rise in pitch-line and crescendo to m. 34, after which the normal situation resumes. One view of the metric structure might be represented asin Fig. 3-2. Fig. 3-2. An interpretation of metricstructure inthe Chopin Ex. 1-3!. m. 26 3+3 4
28 30
32 34
VF
2+2+2+2 2+1
Anomalies Restoration
/V 3+3
of normal mensural unit resolution
of metric dissonance!
Whether the mensural unit changes inactual as opposed toincipient metric fluctuation! or simply briefly admits an acceleration in accent frequency
in relation to which the original F and $1 units arefélt stillprevailing as and referential is a question ofperception to be taken up later as to the concept of preconditioning metric structure. A further example Ex. 3-9! is shared with Cooper and Meyer. It is necessary that, like many examples in this chapter, it be given in incomplete notation.! 1°Feminine cadentialarrival ismetrically strongat onlya stilllower level,at thelevel of somedivision ofthe notatedbar. 2°Cooper and Meyer, TheRhythmic Structure af Music,p. 90.
rhythm and meter 325 Ex. 3-9. Mozart, Symphony No. 41in C, K. 551 Jupiter!, secondmovement. cantabile AAndante ` IIHIQMMI EM'1llllI.11Mi'FMl.M
_Af '592 YIMII-IQ_M11"_`
B? P M II' I 1 1: E:=:*=-§»=- : 33 VV
.':E 'i
QV '
= v-iI
'' °3
P `M 921_= 1
'=
.i ..='
3 5 .3+2! VV
,»":5 ~5 v
V
'iv I
v
1
Broadened, J asymmetrical unit Point of resolution
What is provocative in the example is of course the accent given the second beat in every one of the first six measures:by durational superiority, by dynamic intensity stress!, by texture, by anacrustic support, by superiority of pitch, or by combinations of thesefactors. Uncertainty, as between the real and notated meters, is at least for a time resolved by the strong afiirmation of the downbeat of m. 7 intensity of dynamic level, duration, harmonic change, initiation of new motivic idea in complementary group-
ing, changed motion, leaps, §tc.!, but what of the first six notated measuresLet ? us assume the thatVnotated meter has no preconditioning effect, forgetting that we know the piece and its notation and ultimate metric order.
Cooper and Meyer insist that the triple meter must be expressed in performance. However, the melody is, as Mozart makes clear, definitely in triple, not in duple meter. Nor should the theme be thought of or played as metrically
ambiguous orvague. Thereare tworeasons for this. Froma historicalpoint of view, the rhythm is that of a sarabande, with its typically heavy secondbeat. Since this rhythm is normally precise, it would be stylistically wrong to perform it ambiguously. From the point of view of internal structure, too, the triple meter should be decisively articulated. For if the latent duple organization is permitted to obscure ordominate the manifest triple meter, the meaning and character, not only of the theme itself but of the whole movement as developing out of the theme, are considerably weakened."
We shall take a very opposite view of the example. Let us argue that the first two notes are anacrustic to the double-dotted a,
and that Mozarts
2'Cooper andMeyer, TheRhythmic Structure qf Music,p. 90. References toexamples are omitted. "
326 rhythm
and meter
anomalous accentsdo shift the bar-line-i.e., do impose a real meter out of accord with the notated bar-line. What is expressed isnot a latent duple meter but a triple meter which is temporarily displaced in relation
to the notated bar-line. Cnly harmonic rhythm accords with the notateél
bar-line establishinga subtlecountergrouping!. There is asymmetry in the size of the unit just preceding m. 7; Mozart makes this very persuasive by continued melodic descent, while faintly recalling the preceding irregular accent with harmonic rhythmic change underthe Bbof m. 6 partitioning the 5 3+2
unit as'O !. The entire point ofthe metric structure the of passage
seen inthis light is asa succession toward metric resolution in which the shift in real bar-line and consequent asymmetry of the unit just described are stabilized and brought into accord with the notated bar-line! at m. 7. Dynamic stress must not be overlooked as contributively meterconditioning, nor should the real metric anomaly be thought weakening. Mozart has created a provocative structure involving recession out of a condition of metric ambiguity dissonance! and it is vital that this be expressed; m.7 arrives as a point of emphatic clarification resolution!, not of course decisive for the entire movement.
/mpu/ses and the/7 functional different/ations
The analysis of meter requires the evaluation of differences amongmusical events, thedistinctions by which somemusical impulsesare felt to be strong and others weak, and by which weaker impulsesare grouped in relation to strong, metrically initiating impulses. In these connections, questions like the following arise: How do impulses events,stimuli! differ in character, strength, and function? Can such functions apply analogously to silences? How might these functions be classifiedand, perhaps,symbolized? Canan impulse have more than one function-a duality elision, conjunction! of function? We shall take a fundamental position that musical events, which include silences through which established pulsation continues and whose functions are preconditioned by established grouping, are capable of classification on the following exclusive basesof functionalidentity anddjzrentiation. In the concept ofmeter developed here, afactor ofmetric dissonance and resolution is essential; this concept isperfectly illustratedin the Mozart. It is now clearthat meter and metric here referonly toaccent-delineated grouping, andto noother type.In the unfolding discussion of meter,this essential understanding is no longerexplicit.
rhythm and meter
327
l. An impulse may initiate a metric unit cf. "downbeat"!: it is then an initiativeimpulse accent!. The initiative impulse is strong at the level of the initiated unit.
2. An impulse may conclude a metric unit: a conclusive impube.The conclusive impulse, the last in a "reactive" seriesat a given level, is weak at the level of the unit which it concludes.
3. An impulse may simply carry forward, within the unit, accentdelineated thrust motion, energy!, in a senseabsorbing the force of the initiative impulse, reacting to its accentual "energy" and predominance: a reactiveimpulse R.eactive impulses relatively passive, absorptive! are increasingly weak within the unit." 4. An impulse cf. "upbeat" or anacrusis! may direct energy toward an initiative: an anticipativeimpulse.The anticipative impulse is weak at the level of the unit initiated by the impulse it prepares, or, put another way, at the level at which it is anticipative. The meaning, or sense,of the anticipative-initiative anacrusis-thesis! relation is that the former weaker! impulse is in someway allied to, thereby
supportive of, the latter stronger!: intemporal proximity ofattack cf. 3! and;
in actual articulative aftiliation e.g.,;
e.g., tonal !,
in systemicrelation-
probably only contributive; or by established
precedent e.g., when a series beginseI ~,etc.!. Contrarily, the absence of this anacrustic-thetic! relation means that in some way the two impulses are functionally separate. The initiative impulse determines the position and occurrence of the metric unit; the nature and number of associated,weaker impulses condition its character. The presenceor absenceof anticipative impulse the question of iamb or trochee, anapest or dactyl is fundamental in determining the character of the metric unit.! Whether a particular impulse is seen as weak or strong greatlydepends on the level within which its positionis viewedits position in the structural hierarchy taken as the context of reference.Think, for example, of the opening two measuresof Brahms' third symphony: each of the two chords is metrically strong in the most local sense,yet clearly anacrustic to m. 3. Essential to the foregoing theory of four impulse functions is the idea that "impulse" at a higher level is a complexof lotverleveleven-ts. The concept of impulse thus expands to the extent that the level of referenceis broader. 24Thephysicalanalogyof an object set in motion, impelling another object, which setsin motionanother,etc.,is tempting; the diminishingforcein sucha series seemsdistinctly parallel to that perceivedin the relationsof impulseswithin the metric unit.
328
rhythm and meter
We can illustratethis very simply by referring to an anticipativeimpulse beyondthe foregroundlevel. In Ex. 3-10, the initial group of four sixteenth-notes is clearly anticipative to m. 1. At the level of the, the groupformsa low-level intramensural!metric unit of four events: initiative s !, reactive .-~!,
reactive~-"!, conclusivet !. But the entiregroupis anticipative; thus the "anticipative impulse" at the mensural level consists not of a single event but of a complex of four events. The principle goesfurther in theextendedanticipativeimpulseto thephrase-level initiative at m. 4; here the anticipative impulse is an extended complex of events consistingof metricunits at severallevels.In Ex. 3-10, symbols are varied in representation of impulse functions of such different levels. Ex. 3-10. Beethoven,Sonatain B!, Op.22, for piano,first movement.
We can seein the foregoinga modestillustrationof the ambivalences of impulsefunctionsat digeringlevelsof metric structure i.e., the multiple, or multileveled, functions of impulses!. An initiative impulse may initiate more than one unit at different levels !. Or an event may be initiative at one level, while subsumed within an anticipative group at another ~!, or initiative at one level, while subsumedwithin a conclusivegroup at another !. There are functional dualities conjunctions, elisions! of the samelevel too, as when the final impulse of a metric unit is both conclusive and an-
ticipativeat thesamelevel Q, or conclusive andinitiative at thesamelevel
~, seeEx.3-17andothers!. Conclusive andinitiative impulses arethus exactly conjoined in formal elisons,situations in which the inherently weak conclusive assumesvicariously the accentual strength of an imposed initiative again, Ex. 3-17!.
330 rhythm
and meter
Although tonal function can of coursesupport metricfunction as in the Beethoven motive!, it is in and of itself metriealb neutral:
is
not, apart from rise in the upper voice a discrete parameter!, more or less
plausible gR l J than cf e !. The I l!orcan be initiative, as when preceded by anacrustic factors or otherwise accentually superior; but it can also bemetricalbr weak, as at the end of a metric unit at phrase level see Exx.3-7 and3-8! _The allegratheme ofthe Haydn Symphony No. 102 is a useful further case in point Ex. 3-l l!. Ex. 3-11. Haydn, Symphony No. 102in BL,first movement. Allegro vivace I= 00 oo v '_" Z_` rn»z1.41u===1=111:_.|.a.;.?.;.;I.; I o1.v .Cfi'HI1l 1 l22_12!!|1__]2l!-1-1-I I 1§l._l1 I..li ' _ 5 Y * l_1 ' 11l11lI111Z111.llIl -I2--lm 92|| `°¬ iu V I 0 / ,92 9292 '|lI$_§ll11l11l1§_I.l.L..l.l.l..l_-I I l U/ .Cfiii31111111-1ll1l1'Ill-1114.1 /__1' r *~ |1i1 qv *-1
00 '1
00
»'92 _ Iii 11 .
I1
ff "
i. e., metric structurein the consequent phrase:
II *v ;Il'$.L.§_.l I .__'-!--I---uf |-'/ --1 l..92iYil-_1!-!--92|'_l`;!!-H!-_ YTI*-»_ ul 92
Z'*§..} 11.1 Y .J I or I--II-2-I--1.1 "Il-lf?-QQ! H_.l..1 I-
The Haydn phrase isa metric unit analogousto that of the level of the notated measure.The phrase begins with a strong anacrustic thrust toward the highest pitch: from that phrase-level metric initiative impulse there is consistent descent decline within the melodic parameter! in a pattern in which it seems inconceivableto interpret the cadential recession settling, resolution! as metrically strong, despite itstonal primaqy.Again, the example suggeststhat the cadence ismetrically recessiveexcept when it has the vicarious accent of elided interjection i.e., is simultaneous with initiative accent!, or is-as rarely-the actual point of superiority within its unit, and
rhythm and meter 331
except that it commonly occurs within a preconditioned scheme of intermensural units
so that it initiates
a unit at a lower level as a means of local
enforcement. [The Haydn cadence Ex. 3-ll! is initiative at the local mensural or intramensural level; more broadb,it is recessive metric in function.] Central to the concern enunciated here is the concept of downbeat, of metric accent, as initiatingthe metricunit. A further way of regarding the cadential arrival as metrically recessive is to consider that it is, after all, the culmination of a process in which element-actions of decline necessarily surpass thoseof growth; unless this balance is effected in the sumof cadential actions, thereis compositethrust, which is contradictory. This does not mean that progressive forcesdo not often attend the cadential process; indeed they do, and they often lend compensatory emphatic affirmative force to cadential resolution, or they convey an active quality in cadential expression-a sense of instability occasional even in Hnal cadences. But it means that in balance, to the extent
that thecadence stable is andconclusive, recessive tendencies prevail,and cadential action is normally strong at only locallevels ifmetric structure. Let us point to one final example in which this principle is manifest Ex. 3-12!, considering the extent to which misconception of the arrival as metrically strong would resultin grossinterpretive distortion. Ex. 3-12. Beethoven, Sonata in C minor, Op.13, forpiano, second movement. Adagio cantabile m5
uv t,r l - e 11 nil é f'92--*
/'%-Y
No principle of cadential metric function can be conceived inflexibly, and the foregoing discussion,amply illustrated in accompanying and earlier examples, canperhaps bestbe further amplified in treatment of a situation which is exceptional to the stated principle. There are times, for example, when the final event in the phrase canplausibly be interpreted as the object of anacrustic thrust on the part of every preceding event i.e., by the entire complex ofpreceding eventswithin the phrase!. Whenthat situation pertains,
332 rhythm
and meter
it is to be expected that the late phrase-level accent in an end-accented phrase! setsin motiona balancingmetric unit,often an anticipative group of parallel content in the phrase following. Consider, for illustration, the slow movement of Mendelssohns SonataNo. 2 for cello and piano Ex. 3-l3a!. Ex. 3-13a. Mendelssohn, Sonata No. 2in D, Op. 58,for celloand piano,third movement. Adagio l'¢Y1. 7 X Ulf?
"
__
| _' _ 5 171 Y.|-`:§lll..1Y1 LVQZL- in!
111 _ l"§ H4 _ Qlf I¢ -2 Q .lllnl it , i'
,I _;. " l'.'lS -11 vI
I _-I /_
,_ l'92YII_ I YUII¢f1"*2'1. _ I - -'_ e --
| _ _ 'ITD ml Ill L .l'-'1!'_T ' __ .il I ¬'='-'Q QW Qlll 2| li;_ ll _ uw 1 _.._'_=1_ -H II ¥../
sempre arpeggiando con Pedale" / m.5 [1122 Bn: " _
_
A »°92 1721 I 92 /5 Y' $~ Ki? _ 92./ ' Y¢YfIPl' 92an:;92~ 941.114 _ 92./ `
h
''
-V
1. -_nli 11
cre . - 1- Z 1275 . , 1:1 1
=' izjrfj-11:55 V1 / _ll _-s in/i Zz 1 "
H,
= _1
! :nm inA-ll 1Q..¢il 1
71
Z1 l
_
-ii .
W _92
mcg M m'
fappmszonato edanimato
. »-_
ee s __ dam.
1 li ?"C i!11'l4l 2c
# g_/
1.1 _ X' _
-I _ Q-
111
rhythm and meter 333
The theme has a guileless quality of great simplicity, and in some respects e.g., texture! it is simple indeed. In others e.g., meter! it is intriguingly problematic. That its units at phrase level are asymmetrically related is immediately apparent in their varying lengths, and it will also be noted that apparently like events havevarying placement in relation to the notated bar-line.
The basic questions towhich, in analysis, anumber of possible answers might be suggested arethese: Where is the primary initiative accent in the first phrase? Does the theme phraserecede from an initial accent? Doesit advance toward, in an extended anticipative gesture, initiative impulse at its conclusion?! And, what are the relations between metric structure in the first and thoseof succeeding phrases? An interpretation with respect to these problemsis essentialto illuminating performance. Interpretation of the phrase as end-accented is supported by less equivocal structure in the units which immediately follow, especially the third phrase, where the concluding impulse has agogic, and pitch, and dynamic accent. The entire Part I of the ternary Ex. 3-l3a! could then be viewed in analysis as given in Ex. 3-l3b, embracing a recession in metric unit sizes i.e., a broadening! from 3 to 4 to 5, the latter quantity maintained to the
end of the initial
thematic statement.
The anticipative complex can thus be regarded asa strong preconditioning factor throughout the theme, and Mendelssohn brings into increasingly sharp focus its metric function in an end-accented phrase. Its specific form in phrase 3 where there is a four-note motive repeated in powerful ascent to the high point! has especiallydetermined preconditioning effect in structure of the succeeding phrases. The accentual value of the concluding attack is of course severely compromised in phrases 4and 5; phrase 4 undergoes reductionin dynamics "Let it be emphasized again thatno interpretationof metric structure impliesgratuitous, bruteaccentuation ofattack, althoughslight underlininginfiection, orunderplaying of potential accentsnot consideredmetrically valid, might be appropriate. An underlining inflection can be in timing asmuch as,and in place of,articulativc emphasis. In the first phrase of the Mendelssohn firstof four antecedents inan enlarged period!, experimentationin varying modes ofgrouping will render immediatelyclear the fact that profound differences of structuraleffect canbe effectedby evenslight differences of intensification andtiming; if it is decided thatthe first beat ofthe secondnotated measure is not ofinitiative importanceat any level, theperformer mighthave to underplay that attack, not merelyavoid itsintensification. Onthe otherhand, constructionof that event asprimary would seemto require for its projection somekind of intensification or other deliberate underscoring. End-accent of the first phrase isalso supportedby its recurrence atm. 29, which should beconsulted. While the firstphrase unlikethe others!has arelatively staticcharacter, beginning andending onthe samenotes throughoutthe texture,its repriseat m. 29 hasthe concluding bass moving decisively, the sense of redundancyat the cadence thereby lessened in expansionof the texture-space. Atthis reprise m. 29! the final impulse isalso longerin duration.
334
rhythm and meter
Ex. 3-13b. Representation of phrase-levelmeterin the Mendelssohn asend-accented.
4 +1!
5 +S! I' 5 broadening intervals separating phrase accents!
Weaker, but conceivably preconditioned as phrase accents
and linear recession,while maintaining the inherent motivic grouping of phrase 3; and phrase 5 assumesa background linear stasis now on g'! comparable to that of phrase 1, and it undergoesdiminuendo. Moreover, its concluding impulse forms a simultaneousinitiative for the subsequentunit. End-accent at phrase level may neverthelessbe preconditioned in the final two phrases." The notion of extended anticipative grouping, and a balancing subsequent unit containing the analogousgrouping in preparation of the next initiative impulse at the samelevel, can perhaps best be seenin a notational diminution of representative phraseswithin appropriately ordered, revised bar-lines. Ex.3-13c. A renotationof the Mendelssohn first phrases.
soWhilethe analysisseesphrases2 and 3 asconditioningothers,and confirmingtendencieslogically deduciblein all, an interpretationoffluctuationin the locusof phrase-level downbeatcannotbe ruled out.
rhythm and meter 335 Criteria of accentuation
Accent is a theoretical term denoting the relative projective, qualitative strength of a given impulse ascompared with others which precede andfollow it and, with it, form a metric unit at a given level. There are of course times in music when accentual impulses are of relatively unequivocal effect and function; no one, presumably, would dispute or fail to appreciate the mensural metric structure in Ex. 3-14.
Ex. 3-14. Haydn, Sonata in D for piano,third movementBreitkopf andHarte! No.37!. Presto, ma non troppo
-» 4
§'§[.li92=92Qa _ Yll|fi|-I I |l..]l1-11 LV' ' 92_.f. 7 YQYTI __ Yf1fI92f1"i1f- -;J!l_ 1 IQll ~ --
A' ' -I '_ 1I
I 2?
/ ` -l _ | _ I2 2
_ 121'-2!'!l'!
._
' 92
I
-._
1T
,
The mensural units in the Haydn phrase in conformity with the notated bar-line! are clearly delineated by relative accentual superiorities at intervals of
2.
: accent here is expressed in anacrustic thrust, pitch, and
duration. More subtle factors of texture, and such complementary factors as harmonic rhythm, could also be noted.! But music is by no means universally so explicit even at mensural levels, and surely not at higher levels of structure. In fact, the Haydn theme requires some interpretive thought as to the question of metric functions at the level of the phrase: it is not clear, for example, that the initial downbeat al is metrically initiative for the phrase-level unit. Is it, rather, part of an anticipative complex underscoring superior accentual value of a subsequent impulse? The question concerns metric structure internal to the phrase-not formal delineation of the phrase by cadential punctuation at four-measure intervals in another, often counteractive, and in the Haydn probably simpler, mode of grouping. In some degree, metric structure emerges in a purely neutral interpretation in performance-i.e., accents which are the result of such factors assuperiority of pitch and duration will, presumably, be felt in some degree in their central referential functions, and there are many instances in which metric grouping, whether in accord with notated bar-lines or not,
336 rhythm
and meter
derives from inherent accentuation of such clarity and decisiveness thata neutral approach is clearly in order. When it seems necessary to bring out the metric structure in instances deemedof relative ambiguity!, it is done in a variety of discreet ways: by slight intensification of accentual attack, usually no more than a kind of gentle urgency in projection; by the slightest hurrying of the anticipative impulse toward the subsequent accent; by understating the conclusive impulse, which may be a low-level accent, creating a perspective inexposure ofanother event; by nearly imperceptible adjustments in timing of attack; and by comparable means whichhave been implied but which, unfortunately, cannot be explored fully. No meter at any level calls for crude, overt accentuation, unlessin parody; at no level is the analysis of metric structure to be understood assuggesting gratuitous intensification of any attack. Although intramensural groupings of particular importance e.g., asymmetries of necessary vitalizing function! at times require awarenesswithin proper restraint, subtle performed intervention is more likely to be indicated at broader levelsof structure. We are obliged to put aside any extensiveinvestigation of these problems, but it is important that we recognize their ultimate significance and something of their nature, difiiculty, and scope. The above comments and questionsare indicative simply of the kinds of problems encountered in performance, the kinds of decisions andjudgments a performer must make; he must also decide, of course, the extent to which explicitness of metric structure is stylistically appropriate compare, for example, Wagner and Debussy; or Stravinsky and Webern!. If we accept the Cooper-Meyer definition of accent as a stimulus . . . which is marked forconsciousnesssome in way3°-and it is hard to imagine a more suitable one-we must then endeavor to understand the means by
which, in music, certainstimuli are so markedor are perceived asemphasized assubjects ofaccentuation. It is true that relatively little can beunequivocally known about accent, and that few studieshave approached the problem of accent perception in Cooper and Meyer, TheRhythmic Structure qf Music,p. 8. The authorsdo not go far in developingthe issue of criteriaof accent,stating p.7! that . _ . sinceaccent appears to be a productof a number ofvariables whose interaction isnot preciselyknown, it must forour purposes remain a basic,axiomatic conceptwhich is understandable as an experiencebut undefined interms ofcauses. How much isprecisely known about interactionsof any musical events?! Peter Westergaard notes thatCooper andMeyer aresubject tothe relatedconcern that in their analysesanalytic choice at lower levels isevidently largely determined by metric position even thoughoriginally meterwas definedas beingproduced byaccent!, at higher levels intuitively, bythe feel of theharmony. [Some Problems inRhythmic Theory and Analysis, in Perspectives qf NewMusic, I, l Fall 1962!, p.183, fn.l4.]
rhythm and meter 337
laboratory conditions. Those that exist dealwith the problem of accent and grouping in only relatively primitive contexts i.e., in circumstances concerned with rhythmic grouping as conditioned by few parameters of accent usually operating singly!. Nor can we with present understanding hope to deal objectively with the question of the influence on thesefactors of various degrees of preconditioning-the preconditioning structure of the musical work as a whole, or the preconditioning understanding most percipients have in some degree of the particular musical rhetoric and its norms. 32 Accent in real music usually involves many element-actionsoperating together; and while such actions cannotbe objectivelymeasured andcomeasured, there is no way out of the need to evaluate their cqfunclioning ifmetric "See, for example, JamesL. Mursell, The Psychology of Music New York: W. W. Norton and Co., Inc., 1937!, Chaps.IV, V. Mursells bibliography on the psychology of rhythmic experience is auseful source of reference, referring backas faras, andin significant degree to,the classicexperiments ofHerbert W. Woodrow: for example, A Quantitative Study of Rhythm, in Archives ofPsychology, No. 14 909!, and The Role of Pitch in Rhythm, in Pqchological Review, XVIII, 6 911!, pp. 54-77. Woodrows studiesconcern groupingas perceivedin seriesof stimuli variously differentiated induration, intensity,pitch, andinterval ofspacing. Inexploration ofdurational and intensityaccents, heconcluded thata subjectinterprets theaccent ofintensity loudness! as group-beginning and the accentof durationas group-ending, while heconcluded that pitch differentiationproduces neithera group-endingnor agroup-beginning effect. The Role of Pitch in Rhythm, p. 77.! Woodrows findings arefundamentally qualifiedand disputed inlater research,all of which appearsto becomparably farremoved fromthe complexity of real musicalconditions. Within the conceptspresented inthe presentstudy, of course, thequestion ofaccent isnot oneof group-beginningor group-ending,since accent is defined as metrically initiative; rather, theissue isone ofexistence of anticipative impulseor lack
of it,in the pattern l f*'! or &!| l &, every metric group, by definition, beginning withaccent. Theissue ofiamb ortrochee is,then, oneof thefunctional affiliation by
temporal proximity, by preconditioning, by articulative relating, etc.!of animpulse toa
subsequent, stronger ode, as in J I J
IJ
I J . Or,the trochee differs from the
iamb in that its weaker component is somehowfunctionally discretein relation to the following accent. It shouldbe stated,too, thatstudies ofpsychoperception of grouping seem often to be impaired by a failure to accountfor the perhaps potentiallydecisive! mannerin which a series ofdifferentiated stimulibegins. Sources ofrelevant bibliography,in addition to Mursells, are: Woodrowsreference in the first of the abovelistings, Chap. I! to still earlier experiments; andM. Schoen, Bibliography of Experimental Studiesin the Psychology ofMusic, in Proceedingsthe of Music Teachers National Association, Theodore M. Finney, ed.,December 1940, pp. 524-27. 32A toneor groupof tonesmay appearto beaccented, notbecause of any particular distinction whichit possesses per se,but becausea previouslyestablished grouping tends to perpetuate itself,making thistype of organization thesimplest. [Leonard Meyer, Emotion and Meaning in Music© 1956by TheUniversity ofChicago Press!, p. 104;all rightsreserved.] The notion of preconditioningstructure, whichhas itsbasis inGestalt concepts of psychological perception,arises laterin the present study;Gestalt approaches are alsothe basisfor the ideathat ultimately indivisible metricgroups areof only two orders-those of two, and those ofthree pulses.
338 rhythm
and meter
anajlysis isto be carried out. No penetrating approach to the studyof rhythmi¢ structure
is conceivable without insistent
efforts in
the direction
of a
wmuprehensive statementof criteria by which accent is evaluated. As often in seeking to understand the musical experience, onestrives in metric analysis to achieve plausible, reasoned hypothesison the basis of demonstrable features of the experiencein question where empirical verification is unlikely gr attainable in only limited degrees. In approaching such a statement of accentual criteria, it is important to recall the concept of accent initiative impulse! as initiating the metricunit at some level, the energy of the accent having a kind of radial impact of diminishing _!%rce through the reactive and conclusive impulses which followwithin the mgrfic unit.
The effort to understand accentual criteria in the experience ofmusic is, apart from the problem of incommensurability, the effort to enumerate
famvrs whichappear tocontribute to and condition the perception of groupingby aa-entuation certain of impulses as metrically initiative. It
is of course imperative,
gspgcially inthe listingof abstractcriteria, to remember thatthe necessity gf their contextual evaluation in real musical situations is ultimately decisive
indeed, criteriacan onlybe seenor expressed as tocontextual relations!. We must point ultimately too to the necessity of evaluation of accentual
impulse inrelation to a given structural level tj rehrence. The necessaryconcern with contextual relativity of accent means, of CQUISC, thatany change within any parameter of musical events can be
significant withrespect toaccent-delineated grouping in an atmosphere of rglative parity of events;hence, thelist of potential accentual criteria is long and comprehensive." At the same time, the list excludes mentionof factors
S¢¢ pp.320-22! expressive of other modes ofgrouping suchnonmetric or gxtramusicalfactors, notedearlier, as text, cadentially punctuated phrase, ¢¢¢_!; thesemay be mitigating or counteractive at times in concurrent
cxpressions of grouping notstrictly metric in nature. Nonmetric factors of grouping e.g., motivic resumption!, whenconformant in relation to mgfric grouping, may give the impression of _hrtyfyingthe accentual initiative
impglse and intensifying itsquality of accent. The following list of accentual criteria is punctuated by brief examples which are contrived for the purpose of focus on individual parameters in artificially simple conditions. Within the above twoessential concerns, context is normally viewedas thatof the Worx inquestion, butit may also beof styleor of genre, etc. It can be arguedthat everychange withinany parameterof structureis metrically initiative i.e., accentual! atsome level, although relatively minor changes i.e., relatively nonxccentual events! can affectmeter atonly verylow levelsof structure.
rhythm and meter 33.9
I. Element-changes toward accentualb' superior valuesconstitute aprimary class of criteria of accent.
l. Changeto fastertempo evident, : in inumerable examples,in the broad formal pattern with slow introduction; or a tempofollowing ritardando, or relatively low-level piu mosso,or a particular tempo event as the object of accelerative drive, may have metrically initiative effect Ex. 3-15a!. Less commonly, changeto slowertempo may have accentualeffect; seeEx. l-3 7!. Ex. 3-1Sa. . piu
in--' §"'
-"- l
rit. _ _ _ ,atempo ls
2. Pronounced change ejpitch : high pitch especially is of accentual effect, but the exposure of an event-distinctly high or low in pitch-by reason of pitch, in a context of lesser change in pitch-line, must be seen as potentially accentual; superiority of pitch denotes the usual accentual factor of higher frequency, but pitch exposure is an accentual value involving uncommon projection by reason ofpitch, high or low Ex. 3-l5b!. EX.3-15b. 1.
0 35 i
Q|
l`
I
3. Related to the above, approach by leapsin lines, especially leapsupward: compare thc factor of anacrustic approach, most often in a rising inflection; if a leap has accentuatingeffect, it accents theevent following the leap, or contributes to that accent Ex. 3-l5c!. Ex. 3-1Bc. | I
t,, I,
4. Longer duration agogicaccent! .°relatively long impulses often have initiating effect Ex. 3-l5d!; note, again,that the longer impulse in the iam-
340
rhythm and meter
bic foot is regarded as initiative of the metric unit, which contains the impulse anticipative to the next agogic accent." Ex. 3-16d.
5. Articulativestress:e.g., the goal of crescendo, or any relatively loud attack, may function initiatively with respectto metric structure Ex. 3-15e!. Ex. 3-16e.
6. Changeto moreintensetimbre: i.e., changes in orchestration from relatively neutral timbre e.g., clarinet in medium register at moderate dynamic level! to more intense, penetrating timbre e.g., high, muted trumpet, even moderately loud; Ex. 3-15f!. Ex. 3-16f.
' Ob. s Smitherdiscusses relativeagogicvaluesasto particular kindsof contextualassociations, treating "absolute"and "relative" agogicvaluesand implicationsof the positionof a note precededand followedby shorteror longernotes.The perfectlyjustified qualifications in Smither'sappraisalof variouskinds of contextualpositionand relation for agogicaccent indicateagainthe complexityof the problemof analysisof metric structure,evenwithin the concernsof a singleparameter hereof duration!. The readermight well referto Smither'sdiscussionof accentualcriteria asa supplement to in somewaysan extensionof, and in someconceptuallyopposedto! that undertaken here. " The Rhythmic Analysisof 20th-CenturyMusic," in Journalof Music Theory, VIII, pp. 61-69.! re In somecircumstances,e.g.
, durationof an event
beyond its immediate locusat a broaderlevel! may haveaccentualsignificance;but this factor is of unlikely relevanceasfelt accenti.e.,asa factorin meter!whereelaborativeprolongationis over very broadspansof time. Comparelinear functional associationasa nonmetricmode of grouping,notedon pp. 32021.!
rhythm and meter
341
7. Changeto denseror otherwisenoreintensetexture;thus, accompanying voices or other voices