Handbook of ethological methods SECOND EDITION
PHILIP. N. LEHNER Depdrtmenl of Biology, Colorado Stote Universiry
h-eo...
67 downloads
2682 Views
30MB Size
Report
This content was uploaded by our users and we assume good faith they have the permission to share this book. If you own the copyright to this book and it is wrongfully on our website, we offer a simple DMCA procedure to remove your content from our site. Start by pressing the button below!
Report copyright / DMCA form
Handbook of ethological methods SECOND EDITION
PHILIP. N. LEHNER Depdrtmenl of Biology, Colorado Stote Universiry
h-eol
F,.
J,uu Psigtilw,tZ -(z
-;)
C.m,rnnrDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS
\
-__/,l'
Contents
pagexw
Prefoce 'l
lt
IT r,,'l
I
INTRODUCTION
l.l What
I
is ethology?
2
1.2
Why study animal behavior?
2
1.3
What to study?
3
l.j.l 1.j.2 l.j.j
4
l*vels
oJ behovior
Areasofstudy Categorics
of
4 questiore
E
1.4 Scientific method
l0
t.s
ll
Ethological approach
l3
1.6 Descriptive versus experimental research
I
GETTING STARTED
t7
2 A CONCEPTUAL MODEL OF ANIMAL BEHAYIOR
19
2.1 Continuous stream of behavior
l9
z.z
Predisposition to behave: dilfering contributions
20
2.2.1 2.2.2
m
Genotype, envircnmenl (experience) ond htemction
Anotomyond physiology
2.3 A model for
21
a behavioral act
24
2.3.1
The
2.3.2
Stimuli
24
2.3.3 2.3.1
Behavior
26
Ethologicol model ol innate behavior
26
2.i.5
Associotive learning paradigms
28
2.3.6 2.3.7
Feedbock
37
Feedlorword
37
mimol
24
2,4 Application of the model
38
2.1,1 Cancuptuallzlng the oreos ol study 2,1,2 OrynkW thc typcs of questioa
2,1,t 2,11
Focuhgrutorch Dowhpht an .xpanM ot mon locutd
,,1t Dlqfit st tnlr,lI, tfu
38 39 39
nobl
4t
ltchst h otlmb
4
I' CONTENTS
CONTENTS
4
2.5 Summary
3 CHOICE OF SUBJECTS
6.5.2 6.5,3
xi
Experimmtal designs
129
Rondom, haphazord and oppoilunistic samples
t4
47
6.6 Relative efficiency of experimental designs
147
3.1 Species-oriented research
47
6.7 Determination of
t48
3.2 Concept-oriented research
50
3-2.1
August Krogh principle
5l
3.2.2
Suitobility verus owilobility
53
sample size
7 EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH z.l Thevaryingvariables 7.1.1 Noturol variation
4 RECONNAISSANCE OBSERVATION 4.1 How to observe 4.1.
I
4.1.2 4.1.3 4.1.4 4.2.1 4.2.2
Walching versus obreming
54 58
Fieldnotes
6t
Equipment
65
describe behavior
Empirical versuslunctionoldescriplions Cotalog, repertoire md elhogrom
4.3 Information 4.3.1 4.3.2 4.3.3
54
An exercbe in obserting
-- 4.2 How to '
54
resources
7.
z.r
8l
86-
Otherresearchers
97
Films ond videotapes
98
5 DELINEATION OF RESEARCH 5.1 Conceptualizingtheproblem
100
Whal are the questions?
100
5.L2
Statingobjectives
102
5.1.3
Research hypotheses
t02
t57 167
Field to laboratory: a continuum
177
COLLECTING THE DATA
Research desigrr and data collection Scales
of measurement
8.3 Sampling methods
-8.3.1 8.3.2 8,3.3
t00
5.1.1
5.2 Using the model for a behavioral act
t
8.2
155
167
8 DATA COLLECTION METHODS
93
l5l
7.2.1 In thefield 7.2.2 In the laborotory
II
8l
94
Artificial manipulation
7.2 Further examples of experimental manipulation
8.
Literature
1,2
150
Adlibit':um sampling Continuow recording sampling methods
E,5.2
Intro-obrner
183 183
€,
tc{ 197
206
2lo
8.5 Reliability
I
183
201
8.4 Observereffects E,5.
l8l
r89
Focal-animal (pair, group) versus all-animal samplhg
E,3.4 Time sampling 8.3.5 Summory
t03
174
212 or sef-reliability
Inter-observer reliability
219 214
6
DESIGN OF RESEARCH
105
ldentification and naming of individuals
221
6.1
Description versus experimentation
,105
E.6.1 Noturolmarks
221
6.2
Variables
107
8.6.2
6.3
Behavior units
109
E.6.3 Assigunent of
6.3.1 6.3.2 6.3.3 6.3.4
il0
6.3.5
Classifcation of behavior unils Spotial and lemporal aspecls
l14
Descriptions and definitions of behavior unils
il5
Number of behavioral units to meosure
t23 t24
Volidity
6.4 Rescarchcr-statistician
6.' Barlc oxpcrimcntd doltnl
6t,t l,fi',t r,rryt attffiitttltttr tfia*d
IN t26 _ 126
t,6
9 9,1
9,2
Copture and marking numbers and natnes
223 210
DATA.COLLECTION EQUIPMENT
233
Notobook and pcncil
235
Drb hnnr
236
9,2.1 Clnrclabtkt ol hhavlor unltt
236
11,1 Cohtmtai,ovt
217
tl *q )Al lrilrnlrlry*r,' '
238 238
CONTENTS
9.3 Clocks and counters 9.4 Calculators
CONTENTS
9.5 Strip-chart event recorders
250 253
254
9.8 Microcomputers
256
Datacollection Dala storage and manipulution
9.9 Audio-taperecorders
I 2 9.9 3 9.lo
I
266
267
Dato collection on uudio-tape recordcrs
267
9.9
Recording unimol sounds
269
Playback of sounds
2'13
347 348
Hypothesis testing
I 5 Selecting the alpha level ll 6 Poweroiatest I
282
I1
Computer analysis of recorded sounds
283
276
Still photogruphy
285 293
Filmanalysis
296
9.14 Metronomes
308
307
Determining geographic location
309
l5.l 9 l5 2
Global Positioning System and Argos Satellite System
3t0
Biotelemetry
313
IO SELECTED EXAMPLES OF DATA COLLECTION
AND DESCRIPTION
319
Individualbehavior
319
l0 I
1
Terreslriql tetrupod loc.omolion
10.2 Social behavior 10.2
l0 l0
I
Disploys
358
7.1 7.2
Sample disttibutions
358
Sumple mean, mode und median
358
7.3
Skevness
359
7.4
Location
359
7.5
7.6 I I 7.7 I I 7.8
284
299
9
I
275
Motion-picture photogruphy
352
Variubilily
361
Standard deviation
361
Sample mean confdence interval
362
CoeJfcient
of voriation
12 SELECTION OF A I
353
1.7 Sample statistics
276
l.l 2 3
351
I
Equipment
I
349
1.4 Type I and type II errors
I
9.12 Videotape recording and analysis 9.13 Stopwatches
lo.l
I
366
STATISTICAL TEST
I I Parametric versus nonparametric statistical 12 I I Paramelrictesls l2 I 2 Datu lranslbrmalions 12 I 3 Nonpurumelrictests
tests
369
377 379
lr I Completely randomized design l-1 I I Ttro indcpcndent sumples l -1 I 2 Thrta or ttrorc indepcnclent sumples I Randomized block, matched pairs or repeated l-l ) I Ttn rtlutcd or nul
For example, the genotype provides the 'innate template' in the young Swamp sparrow's brain that allows it to filter out all but swamp sparrow syllables from the
(G+E+D+(A+P)
irr
Behavior
Behavior
-+ ,,\, -a 't-a-
it hears in the environment (Marler and Peters,l9lT). The adult male swamp sparrow songs it hears provide the syllables to create the 'acquired template'which will be the basis for comparison when the young swamp sparrow begins singing its subsong, listening to itself, and improving it vocal output to create its crystallized songs
-s
-'
------
\\\r.
-)
Consequences
'-)
Feedforward
, (Contingencies)
,ttstl
Negative feedback
I ig.
2.2 The model for
a behavioral act.
primary song. 2.3.2a Endogenous stimuli
2.3
A MODEL FOR A BEHAVIORAL ACT
I rttlogenous stimuli arise from the animal's internal environment. An assumption of rlrc model is that all behavior is the result
of exogenous and/or endogenous stimula-
rron. Th&t is, we operationally define behavior as that which an animal does as the
2.3.t The animal The basis lor the animalin the model is the formula for it's predisposition to behave described above. That is, all the important components that predispose an animal to
perform specific behaviors under a given set of environmental conditions are provided by Gu, EB. IB, A,and P. Also incorporated into the animal portion of the model are endogenous stimuli (discussed below). Other parts of the model (Figure 2.2) are e-\ogenous stintuli, hehavior, ('onsequen('es, contingencies, feedbac'k and./bedforward, all of which are discussed below
rr'srrlt
of stimulation. We observe behavior being emitted, but we assume that all
1,,'lravior is elicited. Like Kuo, the Model assumes that'there is no such thing as ,lrorrtaneous behavior'(behavior thut is not stimulated) (quoted
in Marler
and
I;rntilton, 1966:605). However, there are behaviors that occur for which we cannot ,t('rcrmine the source of stimulation, and these are often called'spontaneous' I
1,,'lurviors (e.g. Hinde, 1966).
Although behavior does not occur spontaneously, it may be elicited by endogerr.rrs Stirnuli that arise spontaneously. . . . every cell in the nervous system is not just sitting there waiting to be told what to do. It's doing it the whole darn time. If there's input to the nervous system, fine. It will react to it. But the nervous system is
2.3.2 Stimuli
Stimuli are changes in the environment (internal or external) to which an animal normally responds. However, the environmental context in which the stimulus occurs will determine whether it is effective in eliciting a response. Therefore, an effective stimulus is a change in the environment which elicits a response at that point in the animal's stream of behavior when the environmental context (internal
L)r ('xiu.nple, Bekoff (1978a) found neural pattern generators in embryonic chicks rlr.rl slintulatc coordinateci limb movements and develop in the absence of any pat-
and external) is appropriate. Stimuli that are effective in one context may be filtered
I, rnr'(l sct'ri()ry inptrt.
(centrally or peripherally) in another context. For example, cat lirocl kibblcs will normally stimulate feeding only when the cat is hungry (interrral cttvirot.ttt'tcttt ) itrttl not involved in a higher priority activity (e.g. chasing a r.nousc extct'ttitl ctlvittrtl-
r)(,r
ment).
Stirrrrrli havc httll't trt'tit'tttirlrrrl ltttrl ttr,qtrtti:ttliorrrrl litttcliotts. only ittitittl,lrrrtl
olirll lrr'llrviot'rlit't't'lly,
'lltltl
is. tltev ttol
lrtrt lltt'Y ltlso lttr ililrtlt';tnr,l ttt,rittl,ttrr lt.'lt:n'
i0t lttttl ltrt'tlttltrttt';tll ;tllllllill l() l)ilt ltt ttl;tl lrt'll;tVtt,ts
prirnarily a device for generating action spontaneously IGraham Hoyle quoted in Allport, l986J
llrcrc rrrc llso bchirviors that occur in the absence of the exogenous stimuli that nrirlly clicit llrcnr (c.g. hirrls'nest building'without materials, cats'prey catching'
rtlrorrl grrt'v. tlogs 'btrryirrg'
'covering' food in a concrete floor). Lorenz ,.,1,1;rrrrt'tl llrt'ot't'rrrrerrr,'c ol'llrcsc'vircrrtrttr'behitviors its an excessive buildup of r( tr()n spt't'rlrt't'rrt'r;,y'1ullt'lt lirrcctl oPctt (ltc vltlvc itt lris Psychohytlraulic Model; l ,rrt'1t,,. l()\O) llorvt'r't'r.;tn()llrt'r Possilrilitv is lllrt v:rctrutu bclutvior is clicitctl irntl ,,r rt'nlr'rl 1,1 trrr,rl,nt;u \'('\ol'('n()lr\ slttttttlt't tt';tlt'rl lrv tlrt'ltttitttltl (ttlsrl sttl'llestetl by 'r
lrttl
A MODEL FOR A BEHAVIORAL ACT
A CONCEPTUAL MODEL OF BEHAVIOR.
Lorenz,l98l); lbr example, we refer to'hallucinogenic'play in cats, implying that
Releaser(s)
+
the'prey'is in the cat's'mind's eye'. Physiological needs are signaled through endogenous stimuli that elicit behaviors designed to meet those needs. For example, low blood sugar leads to the 'state
Key
Positive feedback
stimuli
v
of hunger'which stimulates the anirnal to seek, find and ingest food. There also appears to be a psychological need to perform a normal range of behaviors. Various terms have been applied to this psychological need, including 'ethological need' (Hughes and Duncan, 1988), 'drive' and 'instinct'. For example, Garcia et al. (1973:3) state that 'drive is the psychological concomitant of physiological need', and Dawkins (1986:64) states that 'instinct . . . refers to the inner drive
Animal lnnate releasing mechanism
--?-+
Behavior
+
Modal action pattern
F,...-
-) .r'
t-'-----
'... ,r,
Consequences (Contingencies)
-)
Feedforward
ootat'/-
Negative feedback
or motivational force that leads an animal or person to behave in a certain way'. The
model acknowledges that behaviors can be elicited by endogenous stimuli regardless
i\r
e--tt------isr\
I ig.
2.3 Where the ethologists' model of innate behavior fits into the model lor
of how you choose to perceive and label the basis for those stimuli.
a
behavioral act.
rrrtl physiology, and behavior. This hypothetical construct is diagrammed 2.3.2b Exogenous
stimuli
l,
as
rllttws'
Exogenous Stimuli come from the external environment (biotic and abiotic), take many forms (e.g. light, sound, odor) and arrive via the animal's various sensory receptors (e.g. eyes, ears, nose). The animal's anatomy and physiology are responsi-
Releaser(s)*key stimuli---innate releasing mechanism (IRM) J
modal action pattern (MAP)
ble lor receiving, processing and responding to exogenous stimuli, as well as gener-
ating behaviors which produce exogenous stimuli. Several terms have been applied to exogenous stimuli depending on the role they play in different behavioral para-
digms. Sign ,stirttuli, relea,sers, unconditioned ,stimuli, neutral stirnuli, conditioned stintuli, and disc'riminative stimuli are all discussed in the contexts of the ethological model of innate behavior and the learning paradigms below.
rilr'r)r irs .fi.rcd uction potterns. However. it is now recognized that experience also t,l;rvs un important role. For example, Gerard Baerends, who began his ethological
2.3.3 Behavior
Behavior results when an effective stimulus is received or generated by the animal. As shown in the model, when one behavior is elicited, an ongoing behavior may be
inhibited. This is required
A morphological structure and/or movement (releaser) emit one or more tey 'rrrttuli. The key stimuli operate on the innate releusing nrcchanism (at present, an ,rrritlcntified part of the animal's anatomy and physiology) which 'releases' a modal 'r, titttt pattern, a relatively stereotyped behavior pattern. Since these modal action l,.rllcrns are very similar between individuals of the same species, ethologists 1,,'licved that they are primarily under genetic control and originally referred to
if
the the two behaviors are mutually exclusive; that is,
they cannot occur at the same time (e.g. sitting and walking). A behavior may also
stop because it is no longer stimulated. For example. sleepingis inhibitcrl when an animal is ingesting food; ingestion will cease when the animal is satiated untl cirting is no longer stimulated by the sight and smell
of food.
r,.r'iu'ch six decades ago. recently concluded from his many years of studying 1,, ring gulls that'The infbrmation encoded in the lP.Mlinnate releasing mecha,,irrf iulrl the acquired inlormationfleurning]were found to work in combination.' r rt,rlrcs tninc, I9u5:37). llistoricirlly, cthologists and psychologists have been branded as having diametrr,;rlly o1-r1-rosctl vicwpoints on the relative contribution of the genotype and learnrrrj' lo bclurvior'. llowcvet. cthologists and psychologists are increasingly borrowing
lntl llrcorics. and overlap between ethology and learning tlrcotv ('rn nr()re relrtlily ltc lirrrrttl in thc litcr:rture. For example, adjunctive behavlrr )nr ('ir('l) olltcr''s rcsciu'ch
2.3.4 Ethological model of innatc bchavior
'l'lrcclltolog,tcltl tnotlel ol ittn;tlt'llt'lutviot llrt'lorv. ('.1'. l.ort'nz. l()liI)ist';rsrlv rrrtor l)()t;rl('(l tnl() lltt'tttorlr'l (l'llrut(' .' l)sttttt'rl rtrrolvt'r t'\,)1,('n()l\ slnnrrlr,;ur:rl()nlv
,,,r, r,l lt';ttttttrl, lltr'orrrl\'iu('r't't'y sitttilru'tlvltirtnicirlly irncl tirnctionally to what l,,rrt'lrt't'lt krrorvrr rrr llrt't'llrolol,it';rl litenrlrrrc lrs tlisPlirccntcnl irclivitios'(Davey, l't1.'t1 5t1,
A MODEL FOR A BEHAVIORAL ACT
A CONCEPTUAL MODEL OF BEHAVIOR
UCS/CS or SD
2.3.5 Associative learning paradigms
Positive feedback
\
Learning can be defined as the 'adaptive'modification of behavior as the result of experience (e.g. Lorenz, 1965). Natural selection has shaped various types of
k-"Animal
learning in animals (Staddon, 1983), but each type of learning incurs selective costs (in terms of fitness), as well as selective benefits (Johnston, 1981). Also, the
--?--> Behavior
+
(G+E+D+(A+P)
elicits
UCS
ta,
l'ig.
act.
The paradigm begins with a behavior being emitted and does not consider ,trrtruli that might elicit that behavior. However, the paradigm does include tliscrirurtrtrtit,c
stimuli. Figure 2.4 illustrates how both classical and instrumental condition-
lit into the model.
An unconditioned stimulus (UCS) elicits an unconditioned response (UCR). When a neutral stimulus (NS) is paired with the UCS, over time it becomes a conditioned stimulus (CS) capable of, by itself, eliciting a conditioned response (CR), a reasonable facsimile of the UCR. Note the similarity between classical conditioning and the ethologists'model of innate behavior (discussed above). The UCS is essentially the same as the etholois essentially the
ethologists'MAP(FAP).
) conditioning
The instrumental conditioning paradigm (below) states that a behuvirtr is emitted ancl
followed by consequences (So*, So ) that either maintain, increase or decrease the probability of that behavior occuring again (see Consequences scction bclow). is
Discriminative stimuli 15t), ttr SI) Ilcltlrvior' (r'rrriltt'tl)
i
v
e
st
imu
I
i
discriminative stimuli (S"*) predict that a specific behavior will be followed l,r l.rositive consequences, and Negative discriminative stimuli (So ) predict that a l'( )sitive
elicits
23.5b Instrumental ( operant
Feedforward
2.4 Where classical and instrumental conditioning fit into the model for a behavioral
D is c r iminat
UCR
UCR
--)
UCR
CS--.--..----.------*CR
gtst's releaser, and the
Consequences (Contingencies)
---------t'
elicits
NS+UCS \\ '..
,
Negative feedback
rrrs
.---.------------------.-*
+
-\trs
2.3.5a Classical conditioning
The classicul conditioning paradigm (below) describes how neutral stimuli become conditioned through association, thus gaining the ability to elicit specific behaviors.
IJCR
\/
benefits of a specific type of learning may be limited to the environmental contexts
in which it evolved (McNamara and Houston, 1980). Possible routes of evolution of the primary associative learning paradigms, clossic'al and instrumental c'onditioning, are discussed by Weisman and Dodd (1980) and Skinner (1988), respectively. Both of these learning paradigms are incorporated directly into the model.
Behavior
)
( tlnsetlrtcrtt't's
'Pr'cific behavior will be lollowed by aversive consequences (see discussion below rlrottt specific types of consequences). Since exogenous stimuli in the model include
l,,,th conditioned and discriminative stimuli as types of exogenous stimuli, rrrtcrcsting to note how Davey summarizes their similarity in function. . . . Pavlovian CSs or instrumental discriminative stimuli (SDs) elicit motivational state appropriate to the reinforcer and . . . this
it
is
a
motivational state in some way mediates the emission of the instrumental response. IDavey, l9B9:210J
llrt'rrbility to usc thc cnvirc>nmental context (including
SDs) to predict the consebcltrvior is a marjor benefit of learning. Tarpy presents a psychologist's t','rspcclivc without using the term discriminative stimuli.
,lu('nce s ol'
l,c:tt'ttittg is I'rrttrlunrcntally a process whereby the animal comes to crpcel rt lirlrrt'c cvcrtt birscd upon the patterns of stimuli in the ('n\'u()nntt. rrt or rr1'rtlll its own hcltitviol ITurpv, 1982:lB l9J
I tkr'ttlrt'. I r)l('tll l)l('s('ttls lttt r.'lltolollist's ltcrsPective rlrt prctlictirrg tlrc tltttcrllt.tc ol' rr rllrorrl rrrrrrll lltt'l('lln (lt\( tlnlnt;tltrr.slirnttlt.
l', lr,r\ lot. itl\o
A CONCEPTUAL MODEL OF BEHAVIOR
A MODEL FOR A BEHAVIORAL ACT
Reinforcing stimuli
Presented
Omitted
domestic chicks, during the process of socializatron, received 'self-reinforcement' from social experience during the early sensitive period. Positive reinforcing stimuli that meet these ethological needs are said to have an underlying 'hedonic value'
Positive (So*)
Positive reinforcement
Extinction
(Tarpy, 1982); that is, they possess a sub.jective quality of a positive affective state (Toates, 1988). Whether this is pleasure as we know it, or what Lorenz calls 'feeling
Aversive (So*)
Punishment
N egative
Table 2.1. During or immediately.follow,ing the behavior the rein/brcing stimulus is:
reinforcement
. . . a bird that 'wants'to carry out the beautiful motor pattern of nest building . . . learns to recognize the situation in which performing the
good'and'satisfaction'(Nisbett, 1977:138, 289), does not affect the operant conditioning paradigm or the model. Even reinforcers that meet basic physiological needs might be considered hedonistic.
I have see
nest-building movements gives the maximum satisfaction.
it.
ILorenz, 1981.291]
' Consequences
'
stimuli(So ) are stimuli which the animal perceives as 'bad'(e.g. pain). That is, under those conditions the animal will perform
Aversive reinforcing
s.
Positive reinforcemenr results when the consequence of performing a behavior is receipt of SR*. The probability of the behavior occurring again is maintained or increased.The SR*s which result in positive reinforcement are received under different contingenc'ies, discussed below.
behavior. Positive reinfrtrcing stimuli (So*) are often called rewards. They are reinforcing stimuli which the animal perceives as'good'. That is, in the appropriate contexts, the animal will perform the behaviors necessary to receive those SR*s. The SR*s are received by the animal under different contingencres of behavior called
'
Extinction ( omission ) can only occur after an animal has been positively reinforced for a behavior.
If that same behavior now
does not result in the
animal receiving the SR+, omission is the consequence and the probability
of the behavior occurring again deueases until it is extinguished. Extinction is not simply a dissipation of the response, but is an active
schedules of reinfbrcement (discussed below).
An SR* may be a single stimulus (e.g. food item) or the opportunity to perform a chain of behaviors (e.g. search, stalk, capture, kill, consume). Lorenz (1965) considered the consummatory act in a chain of behaviors as a reinforcer for antecedent
learning process (Davey, 1989).
'
Punishmen r results when the consequence
of performing
a behavior is
receipt of SR . The animal initially escapes the SR , and the probability the behavior occurring again decreases.
behaviors (appetitive behaviors). Also, behaviors may be organized in a 'preference hierarchy'(Premack, 1965) so that an SR* could be the opportunity to perform a
'
preferred behavior.
of
Negutive reinfbrcement can only occur after an animal has been punished a behavior. Ii under the same conditions in which the original behavior resulted in punishment, a different behavior results in avoiding the
lor
Although we often think of SR*s as meeting obvious basic physiological needs (e.g. food and water), ethological needs (discussed earlier) may stimulate an animal
Sr{ . then the probability of that behavior is maintained or increased.
to perform behaviors in order to obtain subjective rewards.
Anin-rals nt:ry pcrlrlrrn belrirviors llurl rrt'c pt'irttrtt'ily irttt;rtt' (r'1,, t'otttlsltip lttttl rrrirtirrg) ltccirrrsc tlrcy tc('('iv('rttlrt'tcttt. sttlrir't'tivr'tt'wttttls. lirr t'x;ttttplt'. irt Si1'rrrrrnrl's 1lt)t)1..)0i',i ) r'it.rr'. '1ll;rV rs tls ou'n t('\\'intl' (',rlllts (l()(r.)1 tlott'tl llt:tt
[Bolles, |9BS.450J
behaviors necessary to avoid those SR
Proximate consequences The fourproximate ('onsequences, in the matrix shown in Table 2.1. are the immediate result of a behavior. They are determined by the type of' reinforcing stimulus and whether the reinforcing stimulus is presented or omitted. Reinlorcing stimuli and consequences are defined by their effect on the animal's
. . . the opportunity to manipulate, to explore, or to merely observe is labeled a reward, reflecting the assumption that if learning hits ttccurrcd there must have been some reward, even though it citnttot bc crtrpirically specified. f (itrrusy'is not anthropomorphic, but rather refers to iunc-
Push
Sandbathing Ventrttm rub
Pat
Diggirtg ForePaw m()vemcllts
Ytwtr
Kick hirck Tttrtl lttttl lltrslr (lill'cllltw's ;ttttl lrtt'lrsl 'l'ttrtt lttttl lltrslr (ttosc)
Slr:r kc
M
Stretch
if not impossible, to do so (Crocker, l98l). It can be argued that
directly or indirectly; therefore, researchers sometirnes unconsciously slip into its trse (Kennedy, 1992). As Rioch (1967) has remarked, we are both limited and
Chopping with incisors
Rolling over the back Writhing
of how strongly one might attempt to avoid anthropomorphisrn, it is
cannot have knowledge of anything which we have not ourselves experienced either
Sifting
Licking
Side rub
very diflficult,
and c'ac'hing
Nibble
Wiping with the fbrePaws Nibbling the toenails
l,
Regardless
Holding with the forePaws Gat he'r ing t'bodstulf.s
lr
Sourc'e: From Eisenberg (1967).
Swallowing
Mouth Hauling in
Mouthing the fur
Upright Testing the air
QuadruPedal saltation BiPedal walk
Diagonal coordination Fore and hind limb alteration
Elongate, investigatory
Biting
Manipulatin with forePaws Drinking (laPPing) Gnawing (with incisors) Chewing (with molars)
Climbing
Gathering
Defecation
Diagonal
JumPing
Isolated animol exp loring
Stripping
Ingestiort
Bipedal saltation
Nest building
oltlittl'
)
lrolurll! rlctcrrrtinerl conccpts. In tliis regard. anthropomorphism might be useful as r rrtctlrphot' lot'tlcscrihing wlrir( urt irnintirl does (Kennedy. 1992; Ristau, 1986) and rrlrrrl ils'cnl()liorr;rl'rurtl ttroliv:rtiorrirl slirtcs ul)pcar ttr bc (c.g. I'car). witliout irlplyrttl llt;tl \()nrr' l('\r'l ol t'orrsciorrs tlro111,,l11 is irrvolvctl. l'or exarrtple, tlrc 1'lht'itsc'tltc ,ltt1l 7r/1r1s',' lltc sr';t l)r()\ trlt's lts rr itlr ;r r tstt;tl itnrr;,,.' ;ur;rlo1'ous lrl ;1 1;g,',t.t"t''s 1'llrlw ;,rtslttltl';t\l(lt'lltr",,,rl \\"rlr't (l')S I l('/)ttolt". llt.tl '11llrtttktttt':tl16ttt lpltpttttttili,.'t
I
R
86
HOW TO DESCRIBE BEHAVIOR
ECON NAI SSAN CE OBSERVATION
drawn from human interaclor manipulation in animal "ornpgnir:ation, analogies i,clude 'cJeceit" 'selfishness" and tions tencl to dominate'; commonly used terms 'these familiar words tnake visu,spite'. ( 1983: 167) concludes that the use of
I
mates the complete repertoire. The size of the repertoire will,
we should provide technical definialization of technical discussions easier', but misleading inf-erences' tions of the terms in order to 'guard against lir-re'l The saf-est approach is to avoid Where and how cloes the beginner draw the ttse only empirical descriptions' You using terms that could be misir-rterpreted anwed that the average
territory size of black-cappec'l chickadees (Prrru.r utricupillu.r)
r,'ariecl cluring
six stages of the breeding season: prenesting, nest building, egg ltrying. incubation, nestling. and fledgling.
ttt-t1lt>rtittlt thing seents to t-ne is not to miss the natural experiments anci yct to know when it becomes necessary to continue by planned tests.
ITinbargcn, 1955;259J
The use of natural variation has limitations which are both clualitativc arrrl cprirrr-
titative. Waiting lor the proper conditions to arise' ancl atternpting to gltlre r ir sulii-
cient number
of
observations st'rt'netinrcs clrivcs
thc cthologist to
rrrtilicirrl
manipulation:
''\ttolltt't t'tPt'tilttt'ltlltl lt1'rlttrxtclt trl thc sttrrlv tll'cirrrse
Systcntittic erltlrlitrrtirln ol'srrt'lr rtrrtrulrl t'xlx'riln('llls llr:rl r\. \\\l('ntitlt( r'()t)tl)irtisort ol tltt'stltt;rli()n\ \\'l)it'lt rlo trrrrl lltost'r\lrr, lr,l,) n()l tt'lt';rrt';t t'tt't'tt t('Sl)()ll\('
i.t.z Artilicial ntanipulation
t;ttt lrt'ltltllrtsl
ll\ l'()(,rl tlr ;rl,ttltt,',1 r'\lrt'l ttttt'ttl'.
lltt'
arrcl etlect
o{'behavior is to
l:tkt't'ottllol ol lllt'v'ltt'iltllles:tttrl tnlrrriprrllrtr'llrcnr irr thc Iiclrl rlr the labrt*tt.ry.
.\lllt()ttl'lt llrr'rrrlilill)ltlitltr)lt t\;illtlt(.t;tl. t.rcrY lrllt.nrgrl slrorrlrl lte rrrlrtlc t() lll)l)t.()xittt;tlr'lltt'rr,tr ttrtrl.,lrtrttrlt;tttrl llrt,n,tlut,tl, 1r,rrr1,,..,,r., t 1o,.1.11 :rr Por:iltlt,.
THE VARYING VARIABLES
EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH
t58
7.t.2t Elimination, disruption and manipulation When manipulating the animal or exogenous stimuli to answer questions about 'how'an animalperlorms a behavior there are dilferent levels of intervention which lead to differing degrees of validity of results. For example. in determining the exogenous stimuli and corresponding sensory systems (or endogenous stimuli and corresponding hormonal/neuronal systems) involved in a behavior you can eliminilte, di.>'rupt or nrunipulate variables. These interventions (all of which are usually referred to as 'manipulations') can be made on the stimuli or the animal's anatomy
(e.g. sensory system). Elimination, disruption and manipulation represent decreased levels of perturbation, respectively; in general, manipulation provides more rigorous and valid results than does elimination or disruption (see exan-rples of bird orientation/rnigration studies later in this chapter). When stimuli are manipulated (e.g. von Frisch changing the plane of polarized light received by a dancing p. 155), you can make predictions about the resultant behavior (e.g. orientation of the bee's dance will track with the changing plane of polarization); that is. you can invoke research and statistical hypotheses with higher resolution and greater bee,
159
Since the pigeons homed successfully, you might harve concluded that they don't use the sun as a compass and therelore hypothesized that they use the earth's magnetic field. If you then attached bar magnets to their backs (to tli.srupt the magnetic field
around them), but tested them on a sunny day, they would still have homed, but this tirne they would have been using the sun as a compass. Further, unless you recognized that you should have been controlling more than one of the variables at a time, you might conclude that the pigeons use neither the sun nor geomagnetic field as cornpass cues. Even if you recognized your design error and proceeded to disrupt
their orientation by applying magnets and testing the pigeons on overcast days. you would not have as conclusive results as you could have obtained by monipulatirzg the variables and predicting the changes in orientation (see experiments clescribed in section 7.3).
When you eliminate or disrupt an animal's sensory system you also run the risk systems which could be important
of affecting other anatomical and physiological
for the behavior(s) you are measuring. The lollowing story illustrates how attempts
to eliminate a sensory system can have additional elfects on the animal's behavior and the researcher's ability to interpret results:
statistical power (Chapter ll). With elimination ancl disruption you are only attempting to eliminate or disrupt the behavior being studied (often in an unpredictable manner). For example, in attempting to locate the circaclian pacemaker it was known that surgical ablation (elimination) of the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in the brains of mammals eliminated overt behavioral rhythmicity; those
A zoology student had succeeded in training cockroaches, ernd he proudly displayed the results of his long efforts to his professor. He had his cockroaches fall in, and he gave them the command: 'Forward, march!'the cockroaches marched lorward. 'Column left!' the student commanded, and all the cockroaches turned left. The professor was about to congratulate the student on this remarkable accomplishment, but the student interrupted him. 'Wait!' lre said. 'l still have to show you the most important thing.' The student picked up a cockroach from the last row, pulled off its legs, and put it back in its place. Once again he commanded: 'Forward, march!'
experiments provided sorne evidence lor the SCN being the pacemaker. However, conclusive evidence was provided when Ralph et al. (1990) conducted transplanta-
tion experiments with normal hamsters and a mutant strain with a short circadian period. They demonstrated that srnall neural grafts fl'om the SCN of donor hamsters restored circadian rhythms to arhythmic hamsters whose own SCN had been ablated; the restored rhythms always exhibited the period of the donor genotype, normal or mutant (short). If you eliminate or disrupt stimuli in order to determine their role in a eliciting or
The cockroaches marched as before. except, of course. lor the one without legs. 'Column left.'Again, all the cockroaches turned on command. except lor the one that lay where it had been placed. The prof-essor looked inquiringly at the student.
orienting a behavior, the behavior may come under the control of other stintuli anrl sensory systems. Thus, when the behavior does not disappear, or is not clisruptccl.
Thc studcnt said proudly, 'This experiment proves conclusively that cockroaches hear with their legs.' I Eigen and Winkler, l98 I :298 -299
you could draw incorrect conclusions. Indeed. this is what occurrecl in c:rrly cxpcriments designed to determine the environmental cues used for oricrrlulion by liu'rrg-
]
important questions: l. Was the manipulation appropri-
ing bees and migrating birds (Gould, 1982). For exarnple, if you wcrc an ctlrologrst
I Itis lrrlc grvcs risc to three
in the early part of the century and were interestecl in thc cnvironn.rcrrlirl errcs llt;rt homing pigeons use to orient back to the hornc lol't. you rrrighl lr:rvc rlcsiltn('(l('\lx'l iments to elimirtatc rlr rlisrrrgrt polcrrlilrl crrcs. ll'yott ltypolltr'sizr'rl tlrtl pi1't'ons usr' thc strn its:l c()n)l)ltss rilttl tesletl llrt'nr ott rl!'r'rt'lrsl tl:rVs (ltt t'littttttrtlr'lltt'srilr ;rs rr r'ttc). tltt'lli,'t'otrs rvrlrrlrl slrll lrlrvt'll()nr('(l ltstttl'lltt't'lttllr': ttt;r1'trr'ltr ltr'lrl:tr llrr't ttt'
rrlt'lo obllrin vllitl rcsults'/ If so,2. Was this severe a manipulation necessary to iurs\\'('r llre tescrrtclr tlrrcslirln'l Il'so.3. Wts the answer to the research question rrorllr tturkinl llris s('verc lt tnlutipttllttiort'l Sincc thc rttrswcr tt> questions I and 2 is 'No', lrllr
(':ur t ottt lrrrlt' tlr;rl llrt'slrttlt'ttl u'lrs t'illtcr rr rtlrivc or sittlistic rescarcher. If \\r';'11t' llrr'rlrrrlr'nl llrt'ltt'ttt'ltl ,rl ,r',.,unutr1' lltt'1 \\('r('{rnlV tt:rivt'tttttl ittscttsillvc. wc
THE VARYING VARIABLES
EXI'ER I M ENTAL RESEARCH
160
l6l
should recommend that they answer those questions befbre making any manipula-
tion in their next experinrent. Manipulation of variables (versus elimination or disruption) is the method being employed when the researcher uses rnoclels and dummies. or conditioning (all are discussed below).
7.t.2h Models and dummies
ll[ot{cl,t constructeci to rnimic animals. or parts of animals. and tlumnzre.s (stutled of animals) have a long history ot' use in ethology. Dun-rmies were used by Allen (1934) in his study of the courtship of rufled grouse (Bortu,su wnbellu:; L.), in Chapman's (1935) study of courtship in Gould's manakin (Murtucus vetellinus
skins
t,ircllinu,t), and by Lack ( 1943) in his study of aggression in robins (Eritltucu.s rubeczrla: also see Table 8.3). Models and dummies have the advanta-ee
of allowing the
(e.g. visr-ral. auditory, chemical. tactile) in a systematic
experimenter to vary stimuli way in order to measure the effect
of qualitative and quantitative
ditferences.
Tinbergen was an early and exemplary proponent of the use of models (Dawkins cl u1.,1992).
As a typical example. Tinbergen and Perdeck (1950) presented models of an aclult herring gull's head to herring gr"rll chicks. They fbund that the color of the spot on the bill (qualitative property)of the nrodel had an eff-ect on the number of pecks given by the chicks (Figure 7.3A). Tinbergen and Kuenen (1939) used simple moclels to demonstrate that the gaping response of nestling blackbirds (Turtlu.s nterulu nterula) and thruslrcs(Turclu.s ericetonnn ericelrtruni) is oriented by the relative size of the parent's head to their bocly (Figure 7.38).
Moller (1987) stuclied the role of badge size (extent of dark coloratiou on the throat and breast) on status signaling in house sparrows (Pusscr tlonrcstit'u,s) by placing stulfed male house sparrows (dummies) near nests. Stout ancl Brass (1969) placed pairs of dummies, or wooden-block models witli tiltable boclies and adjustable stuffed heads (Figure 7.4).
in glaucous-wingecl gull territories: tlicy
demonstrated that the head and neck are the parts of the bocly that relcusc territol'ial aggression displays in this species. Some researchers have incorporated movement ztnd/ot' ttclors lttttl stlttrttl irtto their models and dummies. For example. Esch (1967) used et wootlcn. tt.totot'-tlt'ivctt model in his research on communication ol'firod source locittion in ltortcybces. I lte rnodelwas the approximate size ot'the honcybccs bcing sturlicrl. btrt it rlitln't t lost'll resemble them physically: this probablv ltirtl littlc cl'll'ct sirtcc lltc cxlrerittt('lrls \\'('r(' carrieclttut in a tlark lrivc.-l-lrc rttorlcltlirl Ilrr,c tltc itle lrtit'rtl.,tlot rtl lltt'lttrr"s tltlrtlr ititnls itnrl ltcrlirnut'(l ;t 'n()nltitl'n'lr1'1'1r.' rl;rrrt',.'. lrttl no lrt't's lt'll llrc lttrt'l,r:t';lr'lt lol
litotl itr llrc tltrr'( ll()n ptrrl l;lttttt'rl lrt lltt'ntotlr'l r tlrtttt t' l ',,1t ,,,tt, ltl,l,',1 llr,rl ',r)lll('
lir
71
A. A cultlbtxrrrl n.roclelol a herring gull head being presented to a chick (adapted l'r'onr'l'inbcrgcn l9(r0b by Lori Miyasato). B. Presentation of models of the l)iucnl\'lrcrrtl. both'rrnrl tail to study the rc-lationships that orient nestlings' lrrpirrl' r'esl)()nsc (ltlirp(ctl ll'ont Tirthcrgcn 1972 by Lclri Miyasato).
EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH
THE VARYING VARIABLES
(Chocton aurign)
validity of his
use
to a cleaner (Labroides pltthrirophugus), he demonstrated of
the
a cleaner model through three indicators: pose duration, pose-
to-inspect ratio, and approach behavior of the host fish to both live cleaners and his models.
Not only must the
Lrse
of rnodels and dummies be carefully planned. but
the
results of such experiments must be carefully interpreted. As an example, in another
of Tinbergen and Perdeck's (1950) experiments on the begging response in neonatal herring-gull chicks, they changed the position of the red spot from the
aspect
model's bill to its forehead. The ohicks delivered significantly n"rore pecks to the model with the spot on the bill than they did to the model with the spot on the forehead. They concluded that it was the position
of the red spot on the head that
caused the decrease in the chicks'responses. Hailman (1969) re-investigated this
phenomenon by placing the rnodels at different distances from the pivot point of the rod holding the model. Further, he adjusted the height of the chick so that it was always at eye levelwith the red spot. He had created three models: a 'normal model'
with the spot on the bill, a model with the spot on the forehead and the pivot point the same ('slow model'), and a model with the spot as on the bill-spot model ('fast model'). The fast forehead-spot model received more pecks than the slow lorehead Fig.
7.4 Models and a durnrny (2d;
used by Stout and Brass ( 1969)
in their study ol
glaucous-winged gulls. la, upright, threat-postured body 1b, trumpetingpostured body; lc, choking-postured body; 2a, basic wooden model;2b, upright threat posture; 2c, model without wings: 2d, dummy showing upright threat posture with wings.
thing more than the dance was necessary to elicit foraging. More recent research used a motor-driven model bee which not only danced, but also vibrated artificial wings and exLlded sugar-water samples; this dummy bee was much more successful
in recruiting foragers (Mollett .1990). Hunsaker (1962) used a head-bobbing machine to move the model heacls ol' lizards (Sc'eloperu^r sp.) in dilferent species-typical patterns. He firuncl thiit l'emalcs selected those rnodels which head-bobbeci in the pattent typical ol' thcir own spccics. Jarvi and Bakken (1984) used three dummy great tits to str-rdy thc f'unction ol'tltc
variation in the breast stripe. Their dummies could be turned 360'. by radio cont rol. to keep them always oriented in the direction from which tlre livc hirrls irpprolclrctl.
Models shor-rld contain the important f-eatures ol-thc livc irnirrurl. irntl tlrev should be used in a normal context (see Cr,rrio 197.5 lirr an cxccllcrrt cxlrrrple ol extensive and proper use ol'modcls). Irt olltcr wortls.';rrr rrrrtlerlying rrssrrrrrptiorr ol the ntcthrttl is thlrt rcsll()ltsc to
1l19
tttotlcl tlcPcttrls ()ll r))u('lr lltt's;rtttt't';ruslrl slslt'nr
ilst'csll()llsclolltctt;ttttltlsttrrrtrltts'(l.ost'\'. l()77.).).1; llrrlor lrrn;rlt'lr titlttistlttt'lVr:tlirl;rlr'tl
llrrs;rssrrrrrP
llttttr'\('r lnl,r:t'\''st'\[t'1g1111'1ll:.onllrt't,",1)on'.('ol lro',l lr',lr
model, although f-ewer than the 'normal model,'revealing the elfect of speed of the red spot on the chicks'responses. Therefore, Tinbergen and Perdeck (1950) were correct in concluding that position of the spot is important; but I{ailman demonstrated that speed of the spot is also a contributing factor.
Models and dummies shor.rld be used with appropriate caution. They may be either too simple with the inrportant stirnuli absent, or too complex with extrarleous stirnuli confounding the experiment. As with any tool, however, in the hands of a skilled researcher. models and dummies can be an important means of manipula-
tion in the field.
7.t.2c Instrumental and classical conditioning
An iniportant technique fbr manipulating variables in the laboratory is through the ruse ol' instrurnental and classical conditioning. Conditioning is a powerful method lirr studyrng 'cirus;ution' ancl answering 'how' questions; the basic paradigms for irrstrtrrncntuI irntl classicaIconclitioning were discussed in Chapter 2.
('orrtlitronrng is thc busis lirr many psychophysicalstudies designed to determine
'lrrrw' rr spccics tliscrirninirtcs bctween varic'rus stimuli. For example, May et al. ( f ()lili ) strrtlit'rl lrorv .lrrprrrrcsc lnirca(l ucs ( lllttcuut.f ir.st'ulu) discriminate between diflr.'tt'rr( ( ()() \ot;rliz;tliotts ltv ttsutp, itt.ttt tttttt'ttlttl t'otttliliottitt,q lct trairr inclividr"ral lnir( ir(lu('s lo tlrst rrrnrr;rlt' \nroollt t';tr lr'' l;;1'11' ;trttl 'slt)orltlr llrtc ltiglt'ctlo sttttnds. Ilrt'rrrrrt rr,lu('\ \\'('rt'lrrrrrt'rl lo nr;rk,'lt,rtt,lt
o111:tr'l
tt'tllt;t tnt'lrtlt'Vlnttlct itt I'csPottsc
164
F.X
t'hlL l M
E
NTA L
R
THE, VARYING VARIABLES
ESE,ARCH
r65
to one type of vocalization and release contact in response to the other vocalization.
First, generalization tests showed that the macaqLles responded appropriately to both natural and computer-synthesized coo soutrds. Then acoustic f'eatures were systematically removed from the computer-synthesized sounds to determine the minirnal elements necessary lbr the macaques to recognize them as distinct coo sounds. Pietrewicz and Kamil (1977\ studied the ability of blue jays (Ct'rzrnc'ittcr t'risttrttt) to detect cryptic moths by instruntcntully t'onditiorting them to respond diff'erentially to the presence and absence of moths in projected images (slides). If the projected slide contained a nroth. l0 pecks on the stimulus key resulted in the blue jay being positively reinfbrced with half a mealworm. The jays were able to detect
the moths, but their ability was allected by the background upon which the motl-t was placed and the moth's body orientation. In a later study, Pietrewicz and Kamil (1919) used the same in.slrruncntul cotrtlitioning procedure to stucly search irnage formation in blue jays.
Often questions about 'how' an animal uses environmental
cr-res
begins with
studies of what a species'can'perceive (Miller 1985); that is, what stimuli they are capable of perceiving and responding to. For example. Lehner ancl Dennis (1971) hypothesized that waterfowlrnight use atmospheric pressure changes as a cue lor orientation during migration. They used instrumcntul t'onditiortirtg to train mallarcl
Fig.
ducks. in a barometric pressure chamber, to peck one microswitch when the pressure increased and another microswitch when the pressure decrensed. They then exposed
7.5 Coyote in tcst chambcr uscd by Horn and Lehner (1975) to dctermine the coyote's scotopic (clirrk aclaptcd) light scnsitivity. A stinrulus patch is at the coyote's eye level at the ccnter ol the right wall; it is not illuminated in this photo. Two loot treadles Are on the floor, separated by a plexiglas partition.
the ducks to sequentially smaller changes in pressure and demonstrated that the ducks could perceive atmospheric pressure changes as small as 0.4 psi. Kreithen and
at the coyote's eye level. They were then instrumentally conditioned to step on a foot
Keeton (l9l4a) used r'lrr.r'.ricul t'ontlitioninglt-t test the capabilities ol- homing pigeons to detect atmospheric pressure changes. The procedure was to place the pigeons indi-
treadle to their right when the light was on. and a treadle to their leli wlien it was ofl. Once they consistently perlbrn-red this discriminzrtion task, then intensity of tl"re
vidually in an airtight cl-ramber. change the pressure over a 5 second irrterval(neutral stimulus), hold the pressure steady lbr the llext 5 seconds, and then deliver electric
light stirnulus rvas pr"rt under the control of'tl-re coyotes. When they stepped on the right lbot treadle (indicating they could perceive the light stimulus), the light auto-
shock (r,rnconclitionecl stimulurs) to the pigeon. causing the heart rate to incrcusc (unconditioned response). After a f-ew presentations, the pressure change becullc it
ruatically decreased in intensity: conversely. stepping on the left treadle (indicating they cor-rld not perceive the light) ar.rtomatically increased the light intensity. The
conditioned stirnulus that caused the heart rzlte to increase (conditionctl t'cspottsc) without the electrical shock being adrninistered. Then, the pigcons'pcrccption ol'rlil-
irtterrsity ol-the light stin-rr.rlus was continuously recclrded resulting in a graph of the coyotcs'psychophysicaI threshold lbr vision at nigl"rt.
ferent amounts of pressure change was determinecl by observitrg chungcs irt tltcir' heart rate. They determined that the homing pigeon is ahle to tlclcct ltttttospltct'it' pressure changes of l0 mm of H,O. or lower. Kreithcn iurcl Kccton ( 197-lh) ttsctl lltt'
rrg
Ittstt'untcrttal contlitioning has sl
bc-en an irnportant technique in studies of foragnrtcgics. As ittt cxanrple. Har ct a/. ( 1990) irt.strLunentully t'ontlitioneclcaged gray
i;rvs ( /'r'r'i lr tt't'tr.:' r'trnttrlt'l,rn') to
As part of thcir rcscarch oll coyotc 1'rrctlrrliort. llont lrrtl l t'lrtt't (lt)i.l)rr;tttlr'tl t1r clctcnlinc tltc lrlu'csl ctt'u ilontnt'rtl,rl lilltt lt'rt'ls llt;tl t'ovolt's. tt lrtt lt lttttrl ;)ttnt:ll
altcrnate hclps on two perches in order to receive fbod l'lrc lirritgc corrltl in two'lirod patches', each of which had two perches irrvs ;rellcts. ;ttttl :t pt'llr'l tlispt'nscr (liigtrrc 7.(r). Thc tirod pellets were delivered on variable ratio ',t'ltr'tlttlr's (\11(. sr'r'('1t;tptct l) irr botlr 1'rirlcltcs. l}trth VR scherlules hac'l the salne ttt(';rtt(r'l' ntt';tll ()l ,l01lr'tt'lt ltops Vl{.10). l)ul ()nt' plrlclt lrirtll high vitriirnccabclut
ilyirl rrip.lrt.erlttltl Pt'tt'rrvr' ('rr\'oli's\\('rr'n)(lr\rrlrt,rlll ll;rtttr'rllo',l,ttttl ttt rt,l.ttL 1,"'l t'l11rtnlrr'r (l'i1'rrrt' / r)iilr(l lltr't';r rlrrrrrtlrt', lr1'lrl Irrllt't lt'rl ()tr .tn ill).t(ltrt' 1rl.r',1t, ,lt'1.
lltt'tttr',ttt:tttrl lltt'o(lrt't ;t lr)\\ \ittl;lltr'r' Ilrr't'r;1 rtt lltt' lrry'lr r,u r,ln( (' lor rtl P;rlt lr
same r'1a.ssit'ul t'ontlitiottittg procedure to detcrnritrc thc
ability ol. ltotttiltlt pigcotts lo
detect polarized light. a cue usecl by bces itt ot'ictttlttiott.
1;tf
: t'l)psr'ttl lilt'ltt]c
1t;ClcpClfliif
lly
ill EXAMPLES OF EXPERIMENTAL MN NII'IIT-ATION
EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH
166
161
7.2 F'URTHER EXAMPLES Otr EXPERIMENTAL
MANIPULATION 7.2.1 In the field
Many experiments arise from descriptive studies in the field and progress through mensurative experiments to artificial manipulation of the animal and/or its environment.
7.2.ta Manipulation of the animal
Manipulation of the animal involves altering the anatomy and/or physiology of the animal (A and P in the model in Chapter 2). For example, the role of sensory receptors and physiological state can be studied by manipulation of the animal per se. Layne (1961) studied the role of vision in diurnal orientation of bats (Myotis' uus'troriparius) by releasing normal, earplugged, and blinded bats (two types of sensory elimination) at various distances from the home cave. None of the eye-covered bats homed, suggesting that vision is an important in homing behavior. Ehrenfeld and Carr ( 1967) measured the role of vision in the sea-finding behavior of lemale green turtles (Chelonia m1,das) by blindfolding them or fitting them with spectacles containing dillerent filters (elimination and disruption of the visual sense). Blindfolded
Perch InPut lines Fig.
jay the instrumental conditioning apparatus used to study gray to perches attached two of consisted each patches loraging strategies. The two and an were reached' pellets clispensed which through hole a microswitches, operated automatic pellet dispenser. A microcomputer recorcled perch hops and
7.6 Diagram of
turtles and those wearing red, blue, and 0.4 neutral density filters had significantly reduced orientation scores.
ai'' the feeclers. as well as controlling lights ancl backgound noise (from Ha et Press' 1990). Copyrighted by Academic
Morphological changes are occasionally made on animals in the field, and the effect on the animal's ability to obtain and/or retain a mate, social status or a terri-
tory is then measured. In these studies, it is the change in behavior of other individLrals that engage in interactions with the altered individual which is usually being
opporLaboratory research using classical or operant conditioning provides the resultatnt animal's the measLlre and tunity to manipulate variables very precisely how tcl tritnsbehavior very accurately. The drawback is that we don't always know its normal crlvilate these laboratory results into what the animal actually does in whethcr tlrirt ronment. We only determine what the animal'can'do; we are not sllre
rneasured; but an ellect can also often be found by observing the altered individual.
As an example, Bouissou (1912) showed that dehorning and reduced weight tlecreased the ability of domestic cattle to obtain and maintain high social rank in
the herd. Harris sparrows (Zonotrichia quereula) signal their dominance status by variations in the amount of black leathering on their crowns and throat. Rohwer (l9l1l rankecl individuals into 14 'studliness' categories (Figure 7.7) and then
is'how'they normallY do it. If you are interested in more detailed inlormation ttn spccilic cotltlitiottittpr otl t'esertt't'lt methods, you should consult the primary literature tbr papcrs rcpot'tittg otl lcrtrtlitll' similar to what you are planning. Also, there atre scveralgoocl tcxt books l()S I (c'8 1)ilvcv' Itlctltotlology basic the and experimental psychology that present
:
Iverson and Lattal, l99l ).
rrltcrctl thc unrount of black feathering on selected individuals to determine the elll'ct orr thcir status. Subordinates dyed to mimic the highest ranking birds were still pcrsccrrlctl l'ry lcgitimate'studlies,'and bleached birds eventually exerted their rrornurllv lrrglr-rrrrking tlorninrtnce. The data suggested that 'cheating' (i.e. lowerrrrrkinllrrtlslreirruelcv:rlcrl instatrrssinrplybyhavingaclarkercrownandthroat)is '.,,r'rrll\ t onlr,rllctl Molle r'(l()li7) rrsctl sirrrilru' ttltrtipulittions itnd demonstrated a ',1.rltrr rtltr;rltttr'' lttttt'liott lirt lrlttl,'t'stzt'(tllttk r'olot;tlirlrt olt llttrxtl irtttl brcitst)in ft,,tt',,'',1);tt t(ltr'. ( /ilr \t r rlrtrttr'\//{ r/\)
I
hXAMPLES OF EXPERIMENTAL Mn Nll'trt-ATION
169
The role of the red epaulets of male red-winged blackbirds (Agcluius phoeniceus) was studied by D. G. Smith (1912) by dying the epaulets black on selected
rr-
males. He lbund that the epaulets were important in maintenance
o.
against rival males, but they had little eff-ect on the males'ability to obtain mates. N.
L
0.)
f-
$
territorial
of territories
G. Smith (1967) changed the eye-ring color of one member of mated pairs of sympatric glaucous gulls(Lurus hyperborea^r), Kumlien's gulls (L. gluuc'oirlc.r) and herring
o E
o
gulls. In all cases where the female's eye-ring color had been changed the pair broke
&
up. but alterir-rg the male's eye-ring appeared to have rro ef-fect on the pair's behavior. o
It
!
o.
(o
(f)
L L
()
important in all research where animals are manipulated and the elfects are studied in interactions with other individuals to observe the ellects on the manipulated animal, as well as on others responding to it. This is true in both intra- and interspecific studies, such as the effects of altered rnales on selection by females and is
altered prey on selection by preclators.
1o
7.2.th Manipulation of the envintnment
s !
Altering the biotic or abiotic environment (see section 2.3.2b) in order to study its resultant eftect on behavior ranges from gross-perturbation experin-rents to subtle
O
changes in one clr a lew stimuli.
o
-o O
$
() !
o
ro
o
L
()
'a c L
aJ)
E A)
U
a)
N
O)
a r-l) a)
,|)
t:
'-)
.,
@
'i
l-.
Stewart and Aldrich ( l9-51 ) were able to get an indication of the extent of the surplus'floating'population of unmated rnale birds the spruce fir tbrests by drastically reducing (by shooting) a large number of territorial holders on a 4O-acre tract. During nine days in June. they removed 148 territorial males, reducing the population to 19"/,, ctl the original. They continued to shoot birds as they moved into the area. and by July 8 they had collected a total of 455 individuals. This is a rather drastic perturbation experinrent, and as they adrnit 'the breedin-u territories were completely disrupted durring the period when the original occupants were being rernoved and at the serme time new adult males were constantly invading the area'. On a smaller scale, Krebs ( 197 I ) shot six pairs of great tits occupying territories and observed thitt residents expandecl their territories ancl fbur new pairs took up occupancy. In contrasl to these major manipulations. Tinbergen was prone to concen-
lrate on srrbtlc environrncntal changes in order to study ef-fects without greatly tlisturbing llrc nornralactivities of the animals. 'l lrc trick is. to insert experiments now ancl then in the normal lif-e of the :rrrirrurl so tlrat this normal lif.e is in no way interrupted; howeverexciting tlrr'rrstrlt rrl'l lcst nriry bc lirr us. it must be a nratter of daily routine to llrr';rrrirrrrrl. A rrurrt u'lro lrrcks thc lccliltg lirr this kind of work will nt('\ llltlrlV t'onttttit ollt'ttst's ittst ;ts s()ltlc l)c()lllc citttttttt help kicking and rl;rrn,rr'urt'tlt'lrt;rlt'llrtntlur('urir t()r)nl \\illtottl e\en ll()ticirrg it. f l'tttl't t..t"'tr. /(i-5-i /-i'\/
I
I
I
EXAMPLES OF EXPERIMENTAL MAN II'III-ATION
EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH
66606@ r:1
.3
b
r:1
.5
c
r:2.3
r:1
d
t
.3
importance of the various configurations in releasing egg retrieval was l. largerlsmaller;2. speckled>not speckled; 3. greenlblue, red>grey; and 4. shape, other than roundness, was relatively unimportant. The titation method used by Baerends and Kruijt is worthy of careful consideration lor other studies using models. This method allowed them to rank the models on a relative basis between and within the four categories of features. Our experiments with the size series showed position preference to be a quantitative phenomenon. A first choice for the smaller egg in the preferred position can always be overcome by increasing the size of the model in the non-preferred position. With our series of models gradually increasing in size it was possible to identify stepwise, in successive tests with the same bird. the minimum size of a model required to overcome position preference, when in competition with a dummy of a smaller size in the preferred position. Thus, through this 'titration,'a model was lound the value of which. in combination with that of the non-preferred site, could just outweigh the combined values of the smaller model and the preferred site. Empirically it turned out that the birds were acting in accordance with the ratios between the surlaces of the maximal projections (maximal shadows when turned around in a beam of parallel light) of the models. Different pairs of models, matching each other with respect to other parameters tries (e.g., volume), or equal with regard to the dillerence instead of the ratio in the parameters used, proved to be unequal in counteracting position preference. The ratio between sites olten remained constant for a couple of hours, and within that period the relative value of dummies with any kind of stimulus combination could be measured and expressed with reference to the standard size series. IBaerand.s ancl Kruijt, I973:30J
@666@@ r:1
F-ig.
X>:8
.3
r:1.3
X: 1 z=7 ==?*';1 a-':9 eP .- = - c. + :i a T = =F = =.= a3 5 ii== ieri; ,?-= SiiEEB ::? =i== E?i?;3 i.-feid ;? ll-ooc i.iEi$; e;ii= =1,= =1= v,i=-3F 3"iqa+ ;=-? === ll
il-
ll ll ll
rl
L''!
ll
Score Activitv levei2
tt
I
Sleeping
tS u-19.9 Itt -i- 1 7.,
2
Lying alert Sitting Standing Walking (slow pace) Walking
>
-1r t.
6
t
10..1
_r.7-6.
1
-)
4
1
5
2.2 3.6
6
1.-3-2.1 o. ,1-l .2
0..1-0.6
0.2-0.3
Results
of observer effects encountered in ethological research (drawing by Dan
Thonrpson).
Error oJ apprehending is due to the physicar arrangement of the animar and/or the it difficurt to observe the behavior. For example, in a study of mockingbird behaviot Breitwisch (l9gg) stated his avoidance observer making
of error of
hending as follows:
appre-
surements of the location of the bull's-eye. Rifleman lt's measurements (shot holes
I did not record wing-flashing, a dispray sometimes given in the of predators and other contexts . . . because
in target) were both reliable (precise) and valid (accurate). Rifleman B's measurements were reliable. but inaccurate. Several potential observer erro[s, which may have severe effects on the results
ethological studies, have been discussed by Rosenthal (1976) and are depicted in Figure 8.5. Observer ef/bct is due to the visual presence of the observer, or other
stimuli (e.g. odor) lrom the observeq and results in a change in the animal's behavior. In psychology and sociology, this change in the subject's behavior is known as the How,thorn e.f.fect (Martin and Bateson, 1986). An indirect observer eflect was measured by Gotmark and Ahlund (1984). They determined wlrether observers attracted predators (crows and gulls) to the nests of common eic'lers (a result that would affect both the reproduction and behavior of the eiders); they tbund that human disturbance did increase loraging ef-fort and suscess lor gr-rlls, but n
*x '-.i ^,X
(,
',4
a
E
I
ll-NJ =i
1tr'(})l.Jo v\J:=1 Jn)7^aq)-=
z, p
oo PP IH
^l
5 If
3
:h-![
recording social interactions of
,V 4
(\
a
\\:
r\
oa
6,c 36' .Do x+ ()
.i OC
aai ='tl aD !.
a
'r
a
focal individual tested in a four-nonkey group
Digit position
r-
,-;:
1
I
2
-1
4
Role of focal individual
Focal individual behavior
Interactor behavior
S
\
rrnsocial
Initiate with physical contact Inttiate - no physical contact Reciprocate - with physical contact Reciprocate - no physical contact Ignore - with physical contact Ignore - no contact
ID-direction
Passive
Passive
Nonsocial
Explore
Explore
Monkey
Withdraw
Withdraw
Monkey 2
Disturbance-fear
Disturbance-fear
Ro
ck-hu ddle
-
sel
f-c
la sp
Ro
c
k-huddl
e - se
I
I
Monkey 3 f-cl a sp
Monkey 4
Stereotypy
Stereotypy
Self
Play
Play
Toy
Sex
Sex
Ladder-shelf
Threat-aggression
Threat-aggression
Window
No response2
The four monkeys in the group are arbitmrily labeled from I to 4 as subject ID (SID) codes. : The no response category for the third-digit interactor behavior Code 9 rcfers to social interactions initiated by the focal subject that produce no change in behavior of the potential interactor from that occurring befor€ the initiation. S,x?,ce. Frcm Sacketr et al.(1973).
C 16
z -l
oa
o ,1
ETggIc'€Esi po-br=-r :.D; :cV, U
e
A)
\Ooo-IO\Un5'+lt9O
-J
? w
(D
+ v
-+
q
(D
H)
F
I
);-
i.
a
rllJ
=
o
z
X!D ()(D
(9
--rn
rL)
H/-
ll
3,
system
a
G
e? illl a3 h-'; €e Il EE ? 3 llll pi a
: EL
v.
qIt.,
\
;:En illl -o
i ii
Bql ?.
nn of number of
bccn trsctl
lol;rltlttlrtton lirt t';rt lt lrt'lt;rriot lirr lltc strtttlllc l-rcriorl . In arlclition Io lltt't lrtt Ls rttt.l r'.rttttlt'tr lltt', t'rltttl)nr('ttl n('( ('\\rl:tlt's;t )l'i Voll l)('1r0wct'srrltply
()('( un('n('('s lrrrtl
1
BABY
Whiskv DATE
6l5nl
NO.
I
TEllP.
ll'C lvlND
15-20 CLOUD ZUS9_
On Mother
On Mother
Missed Total on & offRI (mothermoves)R2 (mother - rejects passively) ,i13 (mother pushes, etc,) F.i
9.1
I
Total )60 cms only
Total types
ol commercially available detectors which have been used to of animals (Fenton et al., 1973). These devices have
cletcct thc ultrasonic sounds
bccn uscrl in rcscarch on bats (Fenton, 1970; Kunzand Brock, 1975) and insects Scc Sirlcs antl Pye (19741ftir a review
(Klcirr. l()55).
( )ttc rtllr':rsor)ie tlctcclor is rrurrrtrlhe ttrrctl by [{olgates of Totton, Southampton, I lrrilr'tl Kinl'1l1v1,t ll trst's lr t'lr1l;rt'tlln('('nli('r'o;lltonc cirpirblc rll'rcsponcling to frc-
(lu('n(r('\lrclrvt't'rr l0;rnrl lXOLllz:r',rrt'll;rst'lt't'ltorut'lrrrrirrl', lolrnril lltcirrprrrl ll:utrl
i
ANALYSIS OF ANIMAL SOUNDS
DATA-COLLECTION EQU IPM ENT
width. Another that uses a crystal microphone adjusted for maximum sensitivity at 40kHzis manufactured by Alton Electronics Co., Gainesville, Florida. Paige et al' (1985) provide a schematic diagram for constructing an inexpensive, hand-held
(Figure 9.208). This provides only a relative measurements and says nothing about
ultrasonic detector.
versus frequency is the section display (Figure 9.20C). This display samples the
the actual intensity of the sound.
Another feature of the Sona-Graph that provides a relative measure of intensity recording at six or fewer predetermined points and presents relative amplitude as a
9.IO ANALYSIS OF
ANIMAL SOUNDS
available Several types of sound spectrographs and computer/software systems are below. described briefly are tools for the analysis of animal sounds. Some of these
down to 2.5 ms.
e.lo.l EquiPment e.to.ta Kay Sona-GraPh Model7029A for conThe Kay Sona-Graph Model 1029|(Figure 9.19A) is an electronic device an from input sound records It display. visual a to verting tape recorded sound reproduced is then sound recorded audio-tape recorder onto a metallic drum. The burns a by a stylus which scans the various frequencies across time and electrically on the vertical axis sheet of paper to produce a 'picture'of the sound with frequency
of representing frequency on a on a linear scale, although reproduced is usually
axis. You have the option
and time on the horizontal linear or logarithmic scale;
it
Marshall (1917) argued for use of the log scale. The frequency range and duration spectrogram produced depends on the speed at which you set the
of the sound
a sound metallic recording drum to spin while recording from the tape' For example' of duration have a will axis vertical on the spectrogram which displays 20-2000 Hz several produce It will 9.6 s, and an 80-8000 Hz display will have a duration of 2.4 s. mechanitypes of display, all of which are useful in the analysis of vocalizations or
cal sounds (e.g. grasshopper calls)' 9'204' The norntal clispla-v of the Sona-Graph is the sonagram shown in Figure (pitch) reprcis is represented on the horizontal axis (l s/mark), frequency
Time
sented on the vertical axis
horizontal mark at each frequency, inverted from the normal sonagraph (frequency increasing from the top to the bottom of the paper). Note that the section through the bark shows a much wider range of frequencies than that through the howl. Marler and Isaac (1960) describe a device for modifying the sound spectrograph to make frequency-versus-amplitude sections serially through a syllable at intervals
(1
kHzlmark), and amplitude (intensity)
is represetrtetl hy
l4'5 crtt x the blackness of the mark. Sound spectrograms are produced on paper 9'20' Figure in 32.4 cm.only a portion of which is shown From the sonagrams above we can see that the coyote's howl bcgittl with thrcc bcirtg clcirt'ly bursts of energy over several frequencies, with none ol'the fi'eqtrencics llcgrttt 111 11 1'r-'l:ldefined;these are essentially introductory'barks'. The howl portiotl t'cttt;rirtctl lirt tively low frequeng:y and then rose to approximzrtcly l.(r kllz. wltcl'c it o11'sllrrrPly. tltrrppctl approximately two secgncls, at which point thc l'r'ctltrcrtcy lrlltt'kttt'ss)' tt't' Sincc thc dillcrcnce bctwccn intcrrsitics is one ol'rle1',11'1'(rt'l;rtivc
ci,l .sc lltc r,plrlrrrr t'rli:ylttt'lrl rlt'littt'lrlr';tlt';ts ()l ('(lllill llll('ll\llV lll \('\('ll l'l:l(lillt(tll\
Sections are useful for determining the relative amplitude of frequencies in a particular syllable or sonagram. However, they cannot be used to make absolute measurements (e.g. number of decibels) without considerable difficulty, and they should not be used for comparisons between sonagrams since they are affected by the investigator's choice of settings on the sound spectrograph. Vocalizatktn terntinologv has been rather inconsistently applied, with few authors using similar terms. Kroodsma (1977) used the terms in Figure 9.2lAto
detail song development in the song sparrow. These terms are similar to those used by Rice and Thompson (1968) for indigo bunting vocalizations. Although not
totally satisfactory (Kroodsma, pers. commun.), these terms are applicable to vocalizations of numerous other species and are uselul in sonagram analysis. Temporal patterns are extremely important in insect sounds. Bentley and Hoy (1972) developed the terminology in Figure 9.218 for their study of the genetic control of cricket
(Te
leogryllus gryl lus) song patterns.
The Kay Sona-Graph Model 1029Ahas been used for more than two decades in ethology, but it is no longer being manufactured (although limited parts are available; Kay Elemetrics Corp.
-
address below). Although more sophisticated equip-
ment is now being marketed (see below), used 7029A machines may still be available, and they are satislactory for analyzing sounds in many studies (e.g. Miller l994,Payne and Payne 1993).
e.tl.th Kay
DSP Sona-Graph Model550A
The Kay DSP Sona-Graph (Figure 9.198) is a workstation that combines a real-time souncl spectrograph, a computer-based data-acquisition system and a dual channel
I;li'l'rrnlrlyzcr'. Sounrl input is stored digitally for display and analysis, and it can be tlowrrlo:rtlctl lo lrnollrcr corrrl'rtr(cr lirr storlgc tlr analysis by other computer prof,,liuns (st't'lrt'lorv). l( is l ln('nrr tlrivr'rr svst('nl llrlr( tlisplrrys rlscilltlgrtnrs (wave lirttttr). ('(rnt(!ut l)o\\'('t sPt'r'lttun',,ur(1 .'1x'r'lt{r|riun\ rrrr lltc vitletl lll()l)il()t'llLtt citn
ANALYSIS OF ANIMAL SOUNDS
DATA-COLLECTION EQU IPM ENT
Fig.9.19B Kay Elemetrics DSP Sona-Graph Model 5500 interface with a microcomputer.
then be printed. It has a history of use in ethological studies (e.g. Brenowitz and Rose, 1994) and is available with several hardware and software options. The newer
CFL 4300
Model
is a completely computer-based system
that may replace the DSP 5500 for animal sound analysis (Kay Elemetrics Corp., l2 Maple Ave. Pine Brook, NJ 07058).
9.I0.tc Uniscan II and Ubiquitous Spectrum Analyzer Two sound-analysis machines that are no longer being manufactured, but may still be available for use, are the Uniscan II and the Ubiquitous Spectrum Analyzer. Both can be used for analyzing animal sounds. The Uniscan II (Multigon Industries Inc.) system includes a keyboard, processor, monitor and printer. It produces a real-time display of a sound spectrogram to
the monitor or printer in several selectable frequency ranges. Any 1.6 4(XX) Fig.9.19A Kay Elemetrics 70294 Sona-Graph and LJher
lutlio-trtpc rccoltlcl
second
scglttcttt can be liozen on the display for measurements of frequency and duration. I Ihitltritous is the trade narre for the Federal Scientific Spectrum Analyzer.It is a
t'cltl-lirnc. titnc-corttprcssion scirnrrirrg unalyzer which can be used for analysis of rttrtttutl \'()ellli/iltirtns willr llre ;rtltlrtiorr ol'rr tlispllry syslcll (tlrtpkins rl u1..1974). A tltl'tl;rlsvstt'rtt is rrst'tl lo sPt't'tl rrp tlrc rrl'rrrl. iul(l itrr;rrr;rlop,s\slct)l swccl)s tltc tirttc-
11
DATA-COLT-ECTION EQU IPM ENT
ANALYSIS OF ANIMAL SOUNDS
A
Note comPlex
Trill
Phrase
Phrase
'r_ I x53-
-r+.
t I
IJ
I *{
281
Trill
Note complex
Phrase
Phrase
'+
{ l'-Syllable
'n'*,fu Ini
tr!!'fufi'
I
l'* Syllable 1.0 s
B
kJc ffi
Et-
F .;.
E iFr
F * r
k
Fig.
-
Pulse
9.21 Terminology used in a study of song development in the song sparrow (from Kroodsma, l9T7): B. Diagram of structural components and terminology of Telegryllu,s songs: upper line: T. ot'eonit'u.y; lower line: t'ommodus.lnterchirp interval : interval between onset of A-pulses. Intrachirp interval : interval between onset of B-pulses. Intertrill interval-interval between onset of last Bpulse in one trill and the first pulse of the next trill (from Bentley and Hoy,1972).
F
I
D
frr-*
l
compressed signal through a filter. Spectrograms can be displayed on a storage
s
oscilloscope or photographed for permanent copy. Narrow and wide bandwidth analyses are possible, and section displays can be made at intervals as short as 3.125 ms.
Fig.
'l display of the same howl; C. Section display abovc thc norttutl tlrspliry. itrte i' marked on the horizontal axis in or-rc-sccoutl itttcrvltls. lrrctlttcttcy is tttlttkctl ol' the vertical irxis ip onc kllZ irrtcrvirls. (Scc tcrl lirt'irrt erlllirlt:tlttttt ol llrt'l!pt's,,1
One advantage of this system is the speed at which spectrograms can be produced. Hopkins et al. (1974) report that a 2.4-second-long spectrogram take approxittrrrtcly 1.3 minutes to analyze on the Kay SonagraphT02gA and only 9.6 sccotttls ott tho I Ibiquitous. Another advantage is the relative ease with which scctiott tlisplays c:rn bc proclucecl. Spectrograms produced by the Ubiquitous are gt;rittir't tluttt lltosc ptrrtltrcctl hy tlre Sonirgnrph: however, this apparently does not
sottttrl s;lccl rrlgt'rtltts.
;rlli't'l utlcr
9.20 A. Normal display sound spectrogram (sonagram) o[ a coyotc howl:
)
lJ.
('otttottt
grtr'l;rl iort ( I loPk ins,'1 ,r/
.
l
()7.1 )
ANALYSIS OF ANIMAL SOUNDS
DATA-COLLECTION EQUIPM ENT
283
redraw the trace and compare lor its accuracy in representing the original spectro-
g.t0.td Desktop computers
gram.
Desktop computers, commonly the IBM-PC (and compatibles) and the Apple Macintosh, are frequently used with specially designed software to store and
Duration measurements are generally made from wide band pass spectrograms. However, the mark intensity can affect the measurements if they are either under- or
analyze animal sounds digitally.
over-burned.
Digitizing tape recordings of animal sounds is accomplished with an analog-todigital (A/D) converter, often a circuit board inserted into the computer where the
All the measurements described above (and many more) can be made more quickly and accurately with a desktop computer and special software.
sound will be stored. For example, Drosophila courtship songs were digitized using
a Campbridge Electronics Design 1401 (Ritchie and Kyriacou,
1994) and a
Canopus Sound Master (Tomaru and Oguma, 1994), and Gerhardt et al. (1994) used a Soundfx interface board to digitize tree frog calls.
9.10.3 Computer analysis
of recorded sounds
A specialized field of data analysis
has developed around the use
ers in bioacoustics. Software currently exists to permit 9.10.2 Analysis
vals between them; and 3. relative intensities
of portions of the sound. They
can
also be used to compare components of the sounds between and within individual animals. Hall-Craggs (1979) provides several useful suggestions for basic sound spectrogram analysis, and Thompson (1979) offers suggestions for preparing sound spectrograms for publication. The techniques described below involve using hand-
operated mechanical devices (e.g. rulers, calipers, computer stylus) and human judgement. Although time consuming and generally less accurate than computer analysis, they may be suitable for some studies. Frequencies are measured from either a narrow band.filter display on a normal spectrogram or from a section display. Transparent overlay grids are useful in the
making of these measurements. Frequency measurement is more accurate when lower-frequency spreads are used for display (e.9.20-2000 versus. 160-16000 Hz). Contour displays are often useful to determine more accurately the dominant frequencies when large areas are burned. Horii (1914) described a method for producing digital sound spectrograms with simultaneous plotting of intensity and
fundamental frequency. A digitizer system (X. Y cursor, Teletype and computer) was used by Field (1976) to analyze sound spectrograms of wolf vocalizations. Thc cursor is moved along a selected frequency band (e.g., dominant frequency). Thc X. axes
of the cursor's plane of movement
input of animal sounds from it is digitized and stored,
a tape recorder or microphone to a microcomputer where
of sound spectrograms
Sound spectrograms (hardcopy) are generally used to measure: 1. frequencies (Hz), both dominant frequencies and harmonics; 2. durations of sounds and time inter-
Y
of microcomput-
represent time and treclucncy. rcspcc-
tively. The operator depresses a button at predetermined points along tltc tt'ltcc, ittttl the X, Y coordinates are transmitted to the compute r lilr storugc ittttl pt'itllctl rtttl otl
using hardware such as the Unisonic (for IBM-PC and compatibiles), MacRecorder digitizer, or various analog-to-digital interface boards (see section 9.10.1d); the maximum length of sound that can be digitized and stored at one time is limited to
the computer's available random access memory (RAM). Once the sound is digitally stored, the soltware can quickly produce spectrographs of frequency, time and intensity, and oscillographs of time, amplitude and frequency. These spectrographs and oscillographs can then be printed out or analyzed further. Some software can make matches between sound segments (e.g. MATCH; Payne and Payne, 1993) and produce three-dimensional visualizations of sound measurements.
Another important aspect of this bioacoustical software is the ability to manipulate sounds which have been digitized and stored in the microcomputer. Portions of the sound can be deleted, duplicated, moved, reversed or frequency-altered at the touch of a key. The modified sound can then be played directly to an animal or fed back into a tape recorder with a built-in (or peripheral) digital-to-analog converter (e.g.
Allan and Simmons, 1994; Randall, 1994). Sounds can be created from scratch
using these programs, or even more easily and inexpensively, using any of the large number of music programs on the market. Davis ( 1986) describes the Personal Acoustics Lab (PAL), which is a microcomputer based system lor digital signal acquisition, analysis and synthesis. Some of the commercially available sound analysis soltware packages are listed below:
'
('unur1, (Apple Macintosh)
Ilioacoustics Research Program
('orncll Labr>ratory of Ornithology
the teletype. Field (1976) usctl an ovcrlayirrg gritl to locitlc coot'tlitutlc s:tttlplt' poitlls
l5() Sirpsrrckcr Woocls I{rl.
evcry 0.0-5 sccrlntl. Thc grcirlcr tlrc vlrt'ilrliott itt l'tetlttcttt'y ol lltt'sotttltl, lltt'tttolt' ol'lcrt llrc r'orlrrlirurlt's slrorrltl llt'slrrrrlllt'rl I ltt't'.r.rttltnltlt'st'lr t'ltll lltt'tt lrt'ttst'rl l,r
Itlurcrr. NY l-1ti50
' i'lttt
,\1tt'r't
lt I
rtlt
(A;rplt' M;lt tttlpslt
)
DATA-COLLECTION EQUI
PM
PHOTOGRAPHY
ENT
GW Instruments P.O. Box 2145 264 Msgr
record environmentalconditions, lens used, film type, shutter speed, lens opening
(/
stop), filters, and any exposure compensations made. This log may be kept as part
of
your field notebook.
O'Brien HwY #8
The techniques of good photography are beyond the scope of this book; com-
Cambridge, MA 02141
plete and useful discussions can be found in Blaker (1976) and Anonymous (1970).
STGNAL (IBM-PC)
Engineering Design
e.ll.l
43 Newton St.
Belmont. MA 02178 SountlEdit v.2.0.3 (Apple Macintosh; can edit frequencies only from 0-11
KHz; not designed for computer analyses) Farallon ComPuting
The most useful still camera for the ethologist is the 35-mm single lens reflex (SLR)
camera. A distinct advantage of the SLR camera is that the image you see in the viewfinder is the same (93-100'2, accuracy) as the image that is recorded on the film. The SLR camera is also compact, lightweight and versatile. It accepts
2150 Kittredge St.
film types
Berkeley, CA94704 SoundEdit Pro (APPle Macintosh)
(see
a
large variety
of
following sections), although the most commonly used is color-slide
film. Ideally, an ethologist entering the field in an unfamiliar area should be prepared with two camerasloadedwithdifferent types of filmdependingupon the proposed use
MacroMind ParacomP, Inc.
of the photos or slides. Typically, a black-and-white negative film for black-and-white prints is kept in one camera and a color slide film for presentations is loaded in the
600 Townsend St. San Francisco,
Still photography
CA 94103
Although most sound-analysis programs will perform the functions described in greater previous paragraphs, some are easier to use, have faster sampling rates and dynamic ranges, and have additional graphic and analysis capabilities;for example, more Weary and Weisman (1993) state that MacSpeech Lab and SIGNAL are ,sophisticated'software packages than SoundEdit v.2.0.3. You should obtain additional information from researchers who have used the software, as well as the dis-
tributors of the software. before choosing a software package to use or purchase, available sometimes demonstration programs (shortened. simplified versions) are
for you to trY.
other. Color prints can also be made from the slides, if necessary. It is helpful if both
cameraswill accept the same lenses
so
that theycan be easilychanged orinterchanged.
There are a large number of makes and models of 35-mm SLR on the market today, most of which have their own group of ardent supporters. Nikon and Leica
quality and versatility; however, Leica Minolta, Canon, Pentax and Olympus are other camera manufacturers to consider seriously, each having within their systems the necessary equipment lor simple to complex photography. These camera brands will have a complete line of accessories to cover your photographic needs, including a wide variety of lenses, motor drives (automatic film advance), and flashes. The following features are excellent camera systems known for their
is very expensive.
should be considered necessities in any camera you use or purchase: 9.I
I
PHOTOGRAPHY research. Pictures shotrlcl
Ethologists should make a photographic record of their are ncccsbe sharp, well composed and suitable for reproduction if needed. Prints orttl prclirr useful (slides) very are sary for publication, while color transparencies sentations.
photos should depict the: l. study site; 2. animals studied; 3. cqtriprttctrt ittttl methodolo gyl4.results of data analysis (tables and figures). ancl 5. yottr itttct'Pt'ctrthe tion of the results (e.g. models; Chapter l8). As photcls arc takctt it log sltottltl lrtkt'tt wrts thc wlry Plloto kept of photo number, {ate. time. location. suhicct nr:tttcr. (i.e. what yoll were trying trl tlcpict)irntl whirl itt pitt'licttlltl yott sllottlrl ttttlt'u'ltt'tl lllilV you sec t5c tprnsl-lltrcllcv tlr'pl'irrl. ln;rtltlitiott. lo itttPtovt'l'ttltttt'Pltolos, \'()ll
t Maximum z Automatic
shutter speed of at least l/l000th second. and manualexposure settings with a maximum lens opening
of
at least./ 1.9; that is, the/'stops to go as low asfl.9.
r Through-the-lens light meter. .t Black camera body to reduce reflections
and glare directed to the animal.
Tlrc lirllowing are optional features to be considered: Irlcctnrnic cable release - enables remote firing of the camera (connects to I
ltc rttrtl ot' tl rivc).
l)epllr-ol-licltl prcvicw Ptr.'r it'rv
grcrrrrits y()u
lo stop thc lcns down rnanually to
tlt'Ptlr-rrl lit'ltl rvtllt lt I'tvt'tt / slolt.
286
PHOTOGRAPHY
DATA-COLLECTION EQUIPM ENT
Interchangeable finder screens - allows replacement of a split- image focusing Screen with a clear matt Screen for easier focusing. is, Data back - provides on frame information when photo is taken; that provided (information settings exposure number, date, time, and
frame
depends upon capability of different backs)' rubber Water resistence or waterproof - some cameras are sealed with
for a gaskets that resist leakage to moderate depths underwater. A rating camera if the and rain heavy in depth of only 3 meters will be worthwhile is dropped in a stream.
l5 years ago' Computer Today, cameras have advanced far beyond the cameras of functions includchips, instead of manual mechanisms, control most of the camera's
OM-3, Canon Fing fbcus, exposure and flash photography. However, the Olympus therefore l. and the pentax K- 1000, have mechanically controlled shutter speeds and does however K-1000, Pentax The rely on batteries only to run the exposure meter. design years of not accept a motor drive or data back. Electronic cameras, through Their ability to and testing, have reached a high level of reliability and performance. auto-focus, and self-adjust the exposure in difhcult and contrasty lighting conditions, However, since research. in tool imprint data on a photo make them a very valuable that recommended is it electronic (automatic) cameras rely on batteries to function, batteries be replaced yearly and spare batteries be kept on hand.
All of these exposure metering systems will, under normal lighting conditions,
give
you the correct exposure. However, when selecting a camera for use or purchase determine whether that camera has the system that best meets your needs. Lenses play an
important role in the quality of your photographs. The quality of
a lens varies optically and in durability. A very expensive camera will take poorquality photos if a poor-quality lens is used. The 'standard' Iens that is most often supplied with a 35 mm camera is a 50 mm focal length lens. It is considered to have a normal perspective (angle-of-view). Any lens that has a focal length longer than 50 mm is a'telephoto'lens, while a lens with a shorter focal length is a'wide angle'lens.
Wide angle lenses are used where a wider perspective is desired (e.g. habitat photos, photos in tight quarters), and telephoto lens are used to magnify subjects that are
far away. The magnification of an object is directionally proportional to the focal length of the lens; that is, a lens with twice the focal length will double the magnification (e.9. a 100 mm lens produces twice the magnification of a 50 mm lens). Zoom lenses have variable focal lengths built into them, such as a 28 80mm zoom lens. Their advantage is that you can carry one or two zoom lenses rather than several fixed focal length lenses. The disadvantage of zoom lenses is that their quality varies greatly and is often not as good as a fixed focal length lens. Commonly used zoom lenses are 28-80 mm and 80-200 mm.
An accessory which many ethologists find useful is a motor-drive unit which automatically advances the film after each shot is taken. A motor drive comes builtin to some cameras. The speed of film advancement ranges from 1.5 to 5 frames-per-
in The majority of auto-focus cameras have the ability to self-focus accurately available focusing manual standard near dark conditions; they will usually have the choices liom fgll also. Exposure metering systems in cameras give you a variety of electronmanual exposures to fully automatic exposures controlled by the camera's and systems metering exposure of types several ics. The following list describes
to maintain continual observation of the animal(s) through the viewfinder without moving it to advance the fllm manually. You can then concentrate on the animal's behavior and photograph carefully selected behavior units, especially sequences, for
defines their [unction:
later analysis or presentations.
Standard program - the camera sets both shutter speed and lens aperture with a bias of hand-held shutter speed of l/125 s or above. Z Wide program - the camera sets both shutter speed and lens aperture with a bias of smaller aperture over shutter speed lor greater depth-ol'-
I
:
field. Tele program
-
the camera sets both shutter speed and lens apcrturc
witlt
bias towards liigher shutter speeds to freeze action' tllc Shutter-priority auto - you set the shutter speed and the calllcril scts lens aperture.
Aperature-priority
auto
you set thc lcns rlpcrttll'c (/ slpp) lirl. tlcpllt-ol'
second. Besides allowing you to take photos very rapidly, a motor drive allows you
Automatic film advances are necessary when cameras are left set up in the field and are triggered by an animal's activity. For example, Savidge and Seibert (1988) used an infrared device to trigger a camera that photographed predators when they visited artifical nests. Electronic.flashes are helpful when additional illumination is necessary and the subject is within range of the flash output. For nocturnal animals, this may be the
only means to photograph them in their natural habitat; however, flashes are likely to alter their behavior. Photographing small animals (e.g. field mice) by natural light otiert procluccs unsatisfactory photos. The combination of a slower film for quality rrnrl rt snritll apcrturc lor clepth-of-field lorces you to use a slow shutter speed and a
field ancl the cuptcrit sclccts tltc c6r't'cct slttttlct'spcctl. Mtrlrrirlcxp()sr1'c y6rr scl lrollt llte sltttllt't slleetl ;ttttl lltt'l('lls;llx'lllll('
tripotl lirt'strppot'l. A llirsh will allow thc usc ol'a small aperture lorgreaterdepthol-licltl. lrctlt't tlclrril on low lilllrt srrlrjet'ts. rrtrtl tlrc rrbility to ll'cczc nrovcn-rcnt with a
witlr tlte p'ttttl;tllt'e ol tlrt'lrrrrll ttt lt1'ltl lll('l('l
Ittl'11r.',
slttttlct sPt't'tl.'l
tV
lr, lr\('il ll;r'-lr llt;rl
ts
lltt's;rntt'lllrrttl
lrs
llrt'(',lln)cl:t so lllrl il
i.J
oo oo
-.1
?1?#g
o
1
g
i EEi Ei EE1BE1E EE1FEi ?11z1it?i11*i g
iI i;
g
E
EE
i
E EEEEE
g
i I E+e Er I E
zt;iica+ +il
--
g
sI
q
EEE
rn
o .l
i
EE
a
i
i
'
Table 9.8. Selccred,(o d1k black-and+,hitu rtlm for use in 35 mm still camerus Definition Speed
ISO
r-\t
\x 100
T:.-\ Pan
T-\IAX
400
of
Sharpness
enlargement
Very high
Very high
Very high
High
For prints. A good all-around film that combines reasonable speed with high definition qualites
Extremely
Extremely high
125
Very fine
High Very
32
Degree
power
fine
P.::t.tt trnr iC-X
P.-:-\ Pan
Resolving
Graininess
allowed Suggested uses
Very high
High
Sharper than PLUS-X
Medium
Very high
Moderate
For prints. Its major quality is high speed which can be pushed to ASA 800 in some cameras. It can be used in very low light (e.g. forest) or to stop
Fine
Medium
Very high
Moderate
Sharper than TRI-X Can be pushed up ro 1600 ISO
Very fine
400
motion (e.g. running antelope)
i-l15 Recording 1000 tEstar-Ah Base)
Contrast
Coarse
Low
Very high
Low
For prints. This is a poor-quality film which has only its speed to recommend it. It should be used only when very low light or high speed call lor it
Fine
High
Very high
Very high
For copying printed materials (e.g. photos, charts, tables, drawings, etc.). Useful in preparing visual aids for presentations and field trips
3200
Medium
Medium
High
Low to Moderate
multi-
coarse
Has an ISO Range of 1000 6400. Allows photography in very low light at high speeds with good results
64
Copy Film 5069
T-MAX
P3200
it will
fine Medium
100
to high
Hi_eh
For prints. With a special reversing process produce slides. It shoudl be used when rhe emphasis is on very-high-quality prints for publication or enlargement
400
speed
rn ^.)
=,=liTEE,txlEi'gE66EEEIliil1tllatzitz
F:.:l
z = z rn
z -t
Table 9.9. Selected color-reversalfilmfor use in 35 mm still cameras Definition
Daylight ISO
Graininess
power
Sharpness
of enlargementallowed Suggesteduses
25
Extremely
High
High
Slides2
Resolving
speed
Film Kt-rdachrome
25
Type of picture and degree
Has high color quality and a wide exposure
latitude. It should be used under most daylight
fine
conditions when sufficient light is available and fast
High
motion does not need to be stopped Kodiichrome
64
Extremely
64
High
High
Slides2
Combines good definition with relatively high speed.
It
does not have the color
High
so
it should be
fine
F..,:-ichrome
100
Extremely
100
Medium
Medium
Slides
Should be used as a substitute for Kodachrome-64
Moderate
when you expect to do your own processing
fine
! r::.-hrtrme 200
Extremely
200r
Medium
Medium
For use in dim light, shade, or to stop rapid
Slides
movement; also with telephotos lacking large lens
fine
I - :chr..nre -i0
Extremely
50
High
High
fine
Extremely
High
High
fine
l-..
;;:
ISO
be pushed to -100
1 _-:-:-=,.:.. ::-r:.;:n
openings, in order to increase depth
Slides2
Same as Kodachrome 25.
High
Excellent color rendition
Slides2
Good definition with higher speeds. Excellent blues
High
and greens. Can be pushed to ISO 200 with good results
uith special processing.
11s.. be t-rbtained throu-sh an
tili g
additional process.
{Ei q 3q= E€ g;rE x; a1"; ?
5
+
. Ft (D
=
€
*'= E Z3Ei i c-g g
?
f 3{
= 1q3
=
?'
a:
3
c
E ='4 e';as.*1 rESg
(J
e,J
X
+
a
(D
o-
E
::r-lidId
3 si
+fi:io A; :i q5;
=E
x=xYa;:l=. =rD=x(v):fro 3?ii6=)6'=^
rDQ-O-O_-i^a:
E- &
aD!,i.aXA:-!
NJ
UJ
x € 3P+E L-H
-IJ()^;^L
{z9ItsE = gE-=) = xx=ocb_]J
3 # ,v 3
:-+(,
qiFi* 1;= t.a i? EsZlirsErir ii i;it8-ii'ei=q iii7 5a [=E ;iig ei 11 iliEi Ell ig i6EEg ;tg sg *iei:+Es ia;x }f ;iii: ie {;Ef tsi=35:; is ril;i Egf Ei
Y
E
o(D
H
="
=i
of field
Moderate
E; [=sg liEgE lisF i =t s 7 tiz,Z|:i : i 1 ! aii 9 i I r =: r i. I i Ei l I 11 i e?iii5g ii*iig sEcr: x is #: + iia sE r J ''. i.;;I ). a a = j o = '==-z jE a=''' 7 i t':; oe, i V {= af -#iE fi: i:fII = .iE i-!r6I;a;i=1 riE+ii!3;i i: i 13 2i5 i 6t rE ZlfrE 1(??ti+: :;? I 7i:= EE = i = r sElEi+ryE i1fli; e1 iaE =:? +;FE;F,aqsEB? =fi?iE5ie= E* gEg5 ,..?iE:Ii=B ;i'gET;i itr i_._E[;,*g: iEZ i-1ZLE??=€ +;
?=i
quality of Kodachrome 25,
used only when extra speed is necessary
lr
V-L-J
cv--;\:f:.O-O aD--=
il
a
\3
o F,t (D
@
x a F-t CD
NJ il
? a
vAAA
6-
6 x_M
) 5 0 a 3 5)-{ xFSd; .LLlXcT 58 r.-B:H x==? Z +
i-P56S.
3i.g
d, x f B'
=-rdJ='.D oj 3aI H o :E +2. *' ? = ?
=3 -
B-$
il
9.5-
,, + :7
i
PHOTOGRAPHY
DATA-COLLECTION EQUIPMENT
Static electricity caused by rewinding film too rapidly in cold weather will cause Table 9.lO Reciprocity ancl rec'ontmenclecl f stop correc'tirttts.for 35 rum color Jilms
streaks or dots on the film. Also, X-ray inspection units in airports, despite their claims, may damage film whether it is new or exposed. X-ray damage is cumulative
Film type
1s
l0s
Kodachrome 25
*% stop
*2
Kodachrome 64
* % stop * 1 stop * % stop
N/A
Ektachrome 400
*% stop
* lrl
Fujichrome 50
No change No change No chage
*% stop *% stop
viewing. A cataloging system will organize your photos and may be based on: l. separate research projects; 2. behavior types; 3. species; or 4. field seasons.
* 1 stop
Computer programs are available that will catalog by numbers and captions, search fbr specific slides, and print out labels lor slides. Ethologists must develop a system
Ektachrome 100 Ektachrome 200
Fujichrome
100
Fujichrome 400
*
and may not show up until after additional exposure. Check film through by hand
or protect it in special, lead-lined bags available at camera stores.
stops
Prints, negatives and slides should be kept in a cool, dry area where they will be sale from damage, but where they can be easily retrieved. Store negatives and slides
1rl stops
in archival. plastic pages which can be put into three-ring binders or stored in a file cabinet. This will protect your original photos and provide for easy access and
N/A stops
Note: One stop:doubling the exposure
Reciprocityfailure is the loss of a film's light sensitivity during long exposures, normally longer than one second. This can be corrected by doubling the exposure (differing time for shots of one second. or more. Color films may show a color-shift sensitivity to different wavelengths of light) during exposures over two seconds. Table 9.10 lists the reciprocity and recommended J' stop corrections for several common films: Most negative films require an increase of one stop with exposures longer than one second. In additio n, inJiarefl films are available for special uses. Kodak High Speed Infrared film is available in 36-exposure rolls for 35-mm cameras. It is fine-grained with moderately high contrast, and medium resolving power and sharpness' It can be used to photograph through haze or to record behavior of nocturnal animals lighted by infrared bulbs. The speed of the film is highly variable, depending on the ratio of visible to infrared light available. Storageis an important consideration lor all types of film. All films are damagcd packby high temperature and high humidity. Films can be obtained in vapor-tight extctttls filnr Refrigerating aging if you anticipate working in areas of high humidity. its useful life well beyond the expiration date printed on the box. Betol'e using lilrn that has been refrigerated, allow 2-3 hours for the film to reach anrbicnt tcrllpcrrtKotlir k ture in it's plastic container (condensation may form on the film il' retllovccl). lilnr: and whitc makes the following storage recommendations for black
For storage periods of uP Keep lilm
below:
ttl:
2
7'5
l;
6
I?
60 Ir
50 l"
tttottllts
Kccp lilrrr lrwlry l'nlnt irrtlrrstri;rl llrtst's. nt()l()t t'rlt;tttsl.;tttrl tltl,rlr ol lllotlrlr;rllr lirr nt;rlrlt.lrl,rlt', solrt'ttlr, t lt'ltll('l\. ilttrl ttttltlt'tt ()l llllll'll" l)l('\('lll'tlt\t'''
which they find most useful. In addition, attempt to reduce possible losses or damage in the mail by sending photos properly packaged in separate packages: if possible. send duplicates instead of original slides and prints instead of negatives. Another storage medium is provided by digital photograph'!-. As examples, Kodak's models DCS 420 and DCS 460 (high resolution) combine digital imaging with a Nikon N90 SLR camera body. They are available in monochrome, color and infrared models which store the images on removable 170 MB RAM cards. One card will store from 30 high-resolution images (6 million pixels; DCS 460) to 100 images (DCS 420): by changing storage cards, 300 images can be captured on a single I hour battery charge. Using appropriate interfaces, the images can be downloaded to Apple Macintosh il, Powerbook. Quadra and IBM-PC (and compatibles) computers. They can then be used in computer displays, made into prints or slides, or stored in portfolio CDs. Additional information can be obtained frorn: Digital & Applied Imaging, L&MS. MC 00532, Eastman Kodak Co., PO Box 92894, Rochester,
NY
14692-9939.
e.tr.2 Motion-picture photography The obvious advantage of both motion pictures and videotape is that they allow you to record a two-dimensional visual representation of entire behavior patterns. The two-dimensional restriction can be overcome, in part. by the use of two or more
stratcgically located cameras. In addition, it provides the capacity synchronously to rccortl souncl (produced by the animals or the environs, or dictated by the observer).
llult
rrntl
llult
(1974) list fivc situatit'rr.rs in which motion pictures and videotape
;rrc especilrll-y rrsel'rrl: l. swil.l :tcliorr: 2. conrplcx actiorr; 3. strbtle bchavioral t'll;ttt1'1'r''.1. t'otultlt'r lrr'lrrrviot;tl s('(lu('n('('\. rtnrl 5. lltc rtcctl lilt'grt'ccisc nrcitsurcntt'trls rll P:rr ittil('l('t\ I lft'lfr',1 r ltort ('\(ril lt,t\t'lrr nr,rLr'r', llr, lrltrr ',r.', l,rr trr,tl \rllt \\,lltl trl tt\(' l ltr'trr.r
PHOTOGRAPHY
DATA-COLLECTION EQUI PMENT
Table
9.ll
Relative advantage of Super 8 mm and I6 mmfilming
Super 8 mm
l6 mm
1. Cheaper cameras, film and processing
l.
Pictures with greater sharpness,
2. Lighter equiPment
2.
resolution, and definition Pictures brighter when projected
to same
3.
29s
Convenience of cartridge film
size
3. Cameras often more durable 4. Film often easier to handle for editing and analYsis
5. Larger film caPacitY 6. Better for sync. sound
film has essentially disapbasic choices are 16 mm and Super 8 mm; standard 8-mm 9' 11; however, it is basiTable in listed are each of peared. The relative advantages
(16 mm)' If you cally a choice between lower cost (Super 8 mm) and higher quality If you intend don't intend to do much filming, borrow or rent a Super 8 mm camera' 16 mm (Figure to make filming an integral part of your studies and can afford it, use
e.22). and In selecting a camera you will be confronted with a trade-off between cost etc')' durability' certain features (e.g. lenses, built-in exposure meter, filming speeds,
what you need, these features are discussed below Remember to purchase but not more than you need. Also, if possible, try before you buy. for your equipLensesshould be selected with an eye toward the uses you intend such as 10 lenses, the camera has a lens turret, then you might select three Some
of
ment. If necessary to mm (wide-angle), 26mm(standard) ,andJ5 mm (telephoto). It may be Kloot 1964) or 1000 mm' use a telephoto lens as large as 600 mm (Dan and van der (26 mm to 75 or 100 mm) is zoom a If the camera will handle only one lens, then tubes are very useful. If you are working with insects, a close-up lens and extension g.fl'l ' often desirable. Select high-quality lenses with large apertures approachin thc confuse as to great so is movie-making for available of
The diversity
neophyte. Selection
filnts
of the proper film
is generally a trade-otT between film speetl
(amount of light necessary for proper exposure) and picture cluality' Illirck-antlwhile color lilltt white film is cheaper to purchase but more expensive ttl proccss. bttl ol'tctt provides an additional dimension which is not only csthctictlly Plcirsittg lilrrrs tlrrrl Kotl;tk ol' rr lisl provitlcs 9.12 necessary in some ethological studics. Tahlc t'rrtt lrt' lilttts slrr't'irtlizctl nr()t't. are uselirl lirr lilnting llinrll bclrirvior'. Atltlrtiorrrrl. (ittttlr" trtl: lllttlt't lirtrrrrl irr Ii.irstrrr,rr K,tlitk's lttr[lit'lrtigtt /( i I , hrtrhtl, 1t1,,'1,11',l.rtltlttt r''ttl lrt'tl"t'tl ltl "'lt Its u't'llits llolll olltt't ttt;tttttl;tr'ltll('l\ l(rl ('\:ltttl,11" tttlt;ttt'rlltltrr
Fig.9.22 Bolex H-16 l6 rnm movie camera with 75 mm telephoto lens.
junction with infrared lighting (e.g photofloods and infrared filters) to obtain motion pictures under nocturnal conditions. (Delgado and Delgado 1964). The.filnring speetlyou choose will depend on the purpose of the filming. Normal projection speeds for Super 8 mm and l6 mm are l8 and 24 frames/second, respectively. The ellect of accelerated motion is produced by filming a slower speeds (e.g. 2 l0 frames/second), and slow motion is produced with greater filming speeds (e.g. 32-64 frames/second). If you are interested in frame-by-frame analysis (see below) then the faster you film. the smaller the change in the animal's position from frame to frame. Faster filming speeds also allow for unsteadiness by the cameraman; but it means changing film more olten and increased costs in purchasing and processing the larger amount of film.
Various filming speeds and the authors'rationale for their use can be found in
of frames per second. 2-7 or 48 (EiblEibesfeldt, 1972), l6 (Clayton, 1976. Havkin and Fentress, 1985), l8 (Fleishman, 1988), 22 (Diakow, 1975), 24 (Kruijt, 1964; Dane and Van der Kloot" 196{), 32 (Duncan ancl Wood-Gush, 1gl2),64 (Bekoff, 1917a),128 (Hildebrand, 1965), and
the literature. For example, in terms
800 ancl 1000 (Grobecker and Pietsch . 1979) have all been used. Time-lapse photog-
nrphy can be used to clbtain instantaneous samples of behavior over extended pcriorls ol' linrc (ulso sce scction 9.11.3. on lilr-r-r analysis). For example, Capen (l()7ti) ttserl rrtt i"i ttrlrr rrrovic cluncl'irs scl lo llrkc lr ll'lrnc lrl citlrcr rlrtc-. l.-5 or twtltttittrrtt'nrl('rvill', trr ltrs slrrrlv ol rrt'stllrl' l',r''1r,tr',.rr irr rrltilt'rllisr.'s. Wlrerr llrr't';rntcr';rs \\('1r".('l,tllt\l tttttlttlt'tttlt'tt.rl', lrr,',1,t\'.,t1 l,lt,rlr,'.tr,rtlrllrr'olrl;11111'llllotttottr'ltlttt
PHOTOGRAPHY
DATA-COLLECTION EQU IPM ENT
Table 9.12. Selected Kodak rever,vul motion-picture.films Daylight
Speed (lSO)
Film
16 mm/
Super
8
Characteristics
Suggested uses
High degree of sharp-
General outdoor
Black-and-rt:hite
Plus-X
I
6/8
ness. good contrast.
2.5.
Fig.
of the bower to a reflector. When the beam is interrupted, a super-8 motion-picture camera exposes one lranre every two seconds. Birds were also observed from blinds. The system enabled the researcher to monitor the behavior and identity of bower owners and visitors at 33 bowers for the -50 day mating season. The researchers are now using a more sophisticated system based on videocameras. From Borgia ( 1986). Copyright O 1986 by Scientific American, Inc. All rights reserved.
gradation 200
l618
Excellent tonal gradation
9.23 The monitoring system used in a study of the satin bowerbird. An inlrared beam, invisible to the bowerbird, is projected through the avenue
photography
and excellent tonal
Tri-X
3 METERS
Under adverse lighting conditions
Color Kodachrome
Good color rendition
40
Ceneral outdoor photography
40
[]ktachrome
Higher speed
Adverse lighting
Good color rendition
General outdoor
160
l6
E,ktachrome
and sharp images
1239
Ektachrome
400
High speed
photography Adverse lighting
high speed
and high-speed
daylight
photography
cartridge. Borgia ( 1986) used an infrared system to trigger a super 8 motion picturc camera (Figure 9.23) in his study of bowerbird behavior' Both Super 8 mm and l6 mm films and cameras are available tbr simultitneotts recording on a sound truc'k. The sound reproduction is generally not of high quality.
but can be useful for recording the observer's commentary durillg lilnling. (iootlquality sound recordings are best made with l6 mm cameras (e.9.. Bolcx Il-16' Figure 9.22) that will synchronize with a high-quality tape recordcr. sttclt rts tltc Nagra IV-L.
(Milinski,
individual movements (Hailnnn,l96J', Havkin and Fentress, (Hildebrand, 1965; see also Chapter l0) and social displays (Barlow, 1977: Bekoff, l9l7 a,b); c) intra-individual sequences (Tinbergen, 1960a); Balgooyen,l9l6); (d) inter-individual sequences (Diakow 1975): and (e) 1984); (b)
1985), including locomotion
spatial relationships (Dane and Van der Kloot, 1964). Analysis of film is conducted either frame-by-frame or by sampling frames at regular intervals, e.g. every 24th frame (Golani, 1973).If frames are to be selected at intervals for analysis, an intervalometer can be coupled with the camera to expose frames at set intervals (Figure 9.24). This provides a more efficient use of film. Steele and Partridge (1988) projected Super-8
film of courting male Drosophila
onto the underside of a glass table and copied the males'movements onto tracing paper;from these tracings they measured each r.nale's maximum angular lag and top speed during their courtship dance. Analyses are generally conducted with either film editors that have a built-in projection screen (Hutt and Hutt, 1914). an optical data analyzer (e.g. LW International; Milinski, 1984;Havkin and Fentress, 1985), or an analyzer-projector (e.g. Lafayette Analyzer; Lafayette Instrument Co., Lafayette, Indiana). The latter projects the film onto a large screen (Dane and Van cler
Kloot.
1964). Whichever system is used,
cor,rrtter. Thc fiame counter, coupled with the
it should
have a reverse and a frame
filming speed, provides
a time base
lor
nreasuring the Iatencies, durations and inter-act periods of behaviors.
A rligitizing tablet can be used to record directly into a computer, data on the e.t
sprttirtl positiort ol'un irninral
1.3 Film analysis
Ethologists take motittn pictLrrcs lor bitsicrrllV two l)ttl'l)()ses: l. lo ltltvt'rt vtstt;tl recrtrcl o1'thc hclrirvior lirr illrrslt'trlivc l)lll'l)()ses (ptt'sr'ttltlti()lls;tttrl pttlrlit';tllolls, t'1' .1.M. l)lryis lr)75): lrrrtl/pr
.) lirr lulrlv:ts ol (lrl spr't tltr' ttt,ltvtrltt;tl
lrt'ltrtt'tot'.
r>r part of its body. For example, Fleishman ( 1988) digitizerl llre ltc;rtl rrrrtl tlcwlrrl'r 1'rositiort ol-tlispllying /troli,t' lizards lrom Super-8 movie llttttt's. ( l ltt'sr'siun('l('('llrritlrrt's rrrr'rlt'st'tilretl lirr vitlcotll-lcs in ir lirtcr section.)
It;ttttt'l11'lt;ttttt'lrtt;tlYstslt;tslrr','rtil\('(ll()nt('itsUt('lltt'tttovetttcrtlsrll'lltcl0ngttc
VIDEOTAPE RECORDING AND ANALYSIS
DATA.COLLECTION EQUIPM ENT
299
actively interacting. If this were the case, correlations which actually exist might be overlooked. (4) Finally, though unlikely, a movement which was too subtle to be detected on the film, might be a stimulus for another individual. IDune and Van der Kloot, 1964.285J
A computer system lor frame-by-frame analysis of film, FIDAC (Ledley, 1965), has been described by Watt (1966). The system consists
of
a cathode-ray-tube gen-
erator which projects an ordered array of rows and columns of spots of light through the film frame, where the intensity of the light transmitted is measured by a photocell as one of seven different levels of gray. This information is then transmitted to a digital computer. The computer can be programmed to control the location of the array of spots of light, their density in the array, and the area covered. The system has both high speed and high resolution. This system, or a similar one, may
find useful application in ethological studies of movement where the animal
is
filmed against a light background.
Fig.9.Z4 An 8 mm sequence camera and intervalonteter inside a weatherproof housing.
In summary, I have not mentioned the vast array of additional equipment (e.g. light meters, fllters, tripods) that may be necessary for proper filming. These items should be discussed with your local camera dealer. Likewise, the various techniques which will improve your motion pictures and their analysis can best be gathered through discussions, experience and literature (Dewsbury, 1975; Matzkin, 1975: Wildi,
of boas (Csnstrictt)r constrictor) (Ulinski,1972) and the loot of a mollusc (Cardium echinatum) (Ansell, 1967).Illustrations of the results of their analyses are shown in Figure 9.25. Head movements relative to particular behaviors have been analyzed frame by frame lor the Burmese red jungle fowl (Galltt,; gallus spadic'eous) (Krut1t, 1964),laughing full chick (Laru,s utricillu) (Hailman, 1967). and domestic duck
(Clayton. 1 97 6 ; Figu r e 9 .26). Spatial relationships between courting goldeneyes were Ineasured by Dane ancl Van der Kloot (1964)by projecting film frame by frame onto a screen that they hacl divided with 16 equally spaced vertical lines. Distances perpendicular to the
(
A nas p la r y r hy nc' h o s)
camera's line of sight are relatively easy to lneasllre; but the perspective tlf depth is lost in measurements parallel to the line of sight. Dane and Van cler Kloot list tlthcr' complications and restrictions which are common to sin'rilar types ttl' {rlni atlalvsis:
(l) Birds are olten passing in and out of the field of'vicw ol'thc cttrltcril. When the final analysis is undertaken, there is always thc cltittlcc lltrtl lttt action given by a bir* ,-J
Ia a
i
/ r-'TP
R
FLICK CLUSTER
3
a
t
I
I
7
/''+Pl
/P\ / ---->/ / ,''l_--
rpa
/'-:a>J l' '
/
J:-
/'',-'"\
aV "l ,
'--,,1
7.5 cm.
END FLlCK CLUSTER
7
Fig.9.254 Pattern of boa tongue movements in lateral view. Tracings of each frame in motion pictures of two complete flick clusters are illustrated. Successive pictures are about 42 ms apart in time. The ends of the protrusion phase (P), the oscillation phase (O), and the retraction phase (R) are indicated by vertical lines. The figures should be studied from left to right in each line. (from Ulinski, 1972).
The lightweight, compact, battery-powered VHS-C and 8 mm cassette palnrcorders (Figure 9.27) make videotaping in the field relatively easy. Motor drivcrr
I:ig 9.25B Thc ntovements ol the foot of the bivalve mollusc, Cartliunt e(hinutum,during a singlc lcap. shown with relerence to the shell as a fixed object. The positions were titkctt ll'om motion picture of the movcment, the numbers indicating the number ol' t hc ll'itme corresponding to each position ( I 6 frames/second). Active (frames l2 lo 2.j)rttttl rccovery (lrames 23 to 50) are shown separately (from Ansell,196l\.
zoom lenses allow the researcher to obtain a broad or locused vicw ol' bclurvior'. Built-in microphones record environmental sounds (those from thc uninrirls lrc
svslcttt spccilically clcsigned lbr field use. It is an integrated, all weather, compact, tertl-litttc v'itlco tttortitot', r'cnrotc camera (infrared and visible light sensitive) and
generally not of sufficient quality for analysis) and also allow thc rcsclrrcltct' lo rtt:rke
rr't'ortlirr;:systerrr. llirttcricswillpowertheexternalcamerasystemlorupto20hours
verbal notations while recording. Although these populur canrcortlcrs
;ttltl lltt'r'lttt)t'riirlcl'ltttrl Ittoniltlr lilr up to l2 h; the camcorder will recorcl up to 120 lirlx' S('\'('rrrl lf iclrlclrrtt syslcrrrs irrc lrv;rilablc ll-or-r-r Fuhrman Diversifiecl.
ir
rc rclrrt ive lv
resistant to moisture and light impact. tlicy urc rtot rlcsignctl lirr llrc lt:rtslt t'orttli tionstowhich manyfielcl cthologists nriglrI cxposc lltcrn. As witlt still('iun('r'irs. v()u shoulcl chcck thc clr;l:rhililics lirr l)11)l)e r rrst' ltttl tr'sisllur('(' lo ;rlrrrst' lirr ;rttY t:rtrt cot'tlct' V()u ittt' t'ottsitlt'titt1' ttslt1, l ltt' liit'ltlt lun rs irrr ti rrrrrr t'l,rr..'rl t rrt rrrl vrtlt'rr
llrler tllrl:r rccrlrtling
virlt'ot:tpt'rl ll ll-i lr
VIDEOTAPE RECORDING AND ANALYSIS
DATA-COLLECTION EQU IPM ENT
27
I 18
Table 9.13. Relative advantages and disadvantages of videotape and ntovie .film ethological studies
for
Movie Film
Videotape
17 16
I
Advantages
l.
Immediate playback
1. Better quality
2.
Reusable
2. Easily analyzable frame by frame providing an accurate time base for studies of movements 3. With wind-up cameras, time in the field limited only by the amount of film 4. Equipment generally light
6
5
3. Tape relatively inexpensive
a
3
4. Easily duplicated Disadvantages
l.
I 1
This composite Fis.9.26 The duckling's drinking response illustrating the bill-lift element. line drawing is based on frames liom a motion picture lilm (16 frames/second). The sequence ol numbers corresponds to the frame numbers beginning as the bill leaves the water (from Clayton, 1976)'
videotrials in their study of mating behavior in water striders; they point out that taping 'allows the detection and accurate quantification of short-lived behaviour patterns and continuous monitoring of behaviour of long durations'(p.895)' Data from videotapes can be recorded on check sheets or input directly into a computer
using standard data-collection programs (see section 9'10'1d)' For example'
comRoberts (lgg4), in his research on vigilance sequences in sanderlings, used a puter-based event recording System to record the times of behavior events from videotape. Also. several specialized systems and programs have been designed the specifically for recording data from videotapes. Krauss et ul. (1988) describe
hardware and software
of a
computerized multichannel event rectlrder'
lor analyzingvideotapes. It records a starting and stoppitrg sigtlal on audio track of the videotape to mark the beginrritlg atrtl clltl ol' tltc
Videologger,
the second
its itrtcrrrlrl segment being analyzed. The microcomputer uses thc sigtritls to t'csct store in rnem6ry tlrc tlnscl tirnc ittttl tlttl'ltliott ol'kcyllt'csscs lot ltttv clock ancl
I( wirs tlcsigncrl to ltrrr ()n lttl Apgrte llt'otttllttlr.'t lrttl rVlttt lt c0nvctlerl lql lttl ltlM lirt tlr;tt Ilrt'svslt'tll t'ollslsls ol liVt'solitt;tl('l)l(),'l;tttts ruunrbcr o('hcltlrvi0r.s.
t'ltrt lrt'
1. Time delay lbr developing
Poorer-quality picture with less expensive video recorders
l.
2. Film
Most now analyzable frame by
usable only once
and stop action
l.
3. Film relatively
Equipment run off batteries with
expensive
limited chargeable life
4. Duplicating more
-1. Equipment sometimes heavy
expensive
;rrc available gratis from the authors. The Behavior Chronicles software (see section (). 10.
lcl) includes a videotape analysis mode in which the computer screen clock is
svrrchronized with the VCR and an icon on the computer screen allows the r('scarcher to control the VCR with the computer's mouse. Scvcral programs designed for recording data from videotapes are available comi:rlly. CAME,RA is a system which includes software and a keyboard which the r('scrrchcr intcrlaces with an IBM-PC; each button on the keyboard generates a ',r,untl with rr unicluc pitch providing the researcher with immediate auditory feedrrrr're
lr;rt'k.
('n MlrltA wts
reviewed by van der Vlugt et al. (1992) and is available from
l'ro(iAMMn. l'.(). Ilox 841, 9700 AV Groningen, The Netherlands. PRO( ()l{l)l:lt is:rnollrcrptl)gratrlirrrecorclingbehavioraldatafromvideotape;itwas r,'r'rr'\r't'tl lrv lrrpp rrrrtl Wrlrlcn ( lt)t).1)irrtcl is:rvailable lrom Jon Tapp and Associates ,
/,r hrrr
I;r1rp. l0(r l.ibclty l.trne.
l.rrve
t'grtc.
'l'N
370tJ6. Nolclus Inlormation
l,'. lrtr,r1111'1' lrllcr s lr Vitlt',r'l;ryrt' Atutlvsis Svsl('nt lirt' ttsc lvitlr'l'lrc Ohscrvt:r 3.0 stlli\\,r!(' rl t',;tr;ttl:tltlt'ttt Ilttr'r'rltllt'tt'ttl rrl)ll()ll l,lt, kltl't's
VIDEOTAPE RECORDING AND ANAI-YSIS
DATA-COLLECTION EQU I PM ENT
Angle of attack
palmcorder, model PV-S62' to Fig.9.21 Stephanie Bestelmeyer using a Panasonic VHS-C behavior. record waterfowl researcher's Videotape can be reviewed at slower or faster Speeds to enhance the her study ( in ability to observe and measure behavior. For example, Grandin 1989)' of pigs, found that high-speed reviewing of videotape recorded at 0'9 frames/s
I
rr ().lll Schematic representation ol a spoonbill's sweeping, showing the various geometric parameters. Also shown are the simulated prey items placed on the bottom to test displacement (undisturbed pattern on the left) and the bill tip vortcx streamlines, indicating shell motion on the right. U: Sweeping velocity; L: lili; A A: cross section of the bill; D: distance of the tip of the bill to bottom; VTX: induced vortex; SH: empty snail shells ('prey') (from Weihs and Katzir, l99zl). Copyrighted by Academic Press.
of the revealed subtle nosing and rooting movements as easily seen vibrations (1994) to Katzir and Weihs snout. Frame-by-frame analysis of videotape allowed (Figure demonstrate the hydrodynamic function of bill sweeping in the spoonbill 9.28).
samples of Time-lapse vicleotaping is often useful to obtain instantaneous/scan time-lapse (1987) used Grant over long periods of time. For example,
behavior
a beagle bitch video-recording to provide a 'continuous' record of the behavior of has also been used and her pups over a three-week period. Time-lapse vicleotaping red jr-rnglein studies of the behavior of calves (Dellmeier et crl..l985), and Burmese
fbwl (Hogan and Boxel, 1993). trsitrg Movements ancl spatial relationships of animals are frequetrtly tlleasttrccl lttl 9tt itttitltitl videotapes. Earlier, researchers often trace
q N
co
o o .f
c{
o q (o
!,7
il l, ti
o -Q
jI,il
So '6 I
ilt/
C)
o.
fl
i(r
t
(\,
)'"
o lrl/
ol ol
lul
T1
lt
EI (ol
,,J
!\*
eJ
sl Fig.
10.8
I
The Eskol Wachmann coordiuate system superimposed on a owl's head in order to denote the movement. In this example the head has moved 90" to the lelt from point; to point i. (S"e text for explanation.)
t0
24.00
l()
-16.00
-8.00 0.00
8.00
16.00
24.00 32.00
width
Front-vicw ol' Cal-comp graph of body position for two human subjects (fiom
Trochim. 1976).
o
2
0
--t
t0.9 Eshkol-Wachmann notation lor
o
6 the owl's head movement in Figure 10.8.
Measurements of dominant subordinant relationships are gerierally conducted an using one, or both, of the following methods (contexts): l. the resettrchcr stages is. clrch equal number of dyadic interactions between all individuals in a grotrp: that
individual is matched with every other individual in the group att ctluitl trttlttbct'ol' times (e.g. Smith and Hale, 1969); or 2. the researcher rectlrds trittttrltlly occttrittg interactions in wild or captive groLlps (c.g. ('hcncy irnrl Scyllrrlh. l()()0). soltrt'litttcs manipulating thc cpvironnrcn( lo cncoul'lp,c crtttllict (e.g. irllt'otlttt'ilt1', rr lirrrilt'tl 'llris scr',rrtl rrrt'llrorl l'('n('rilllv tt'srrlts itt tltllt'tt'tll rrr.rtlrr,t rll' prcle'r.rctl lilotl). Itrt,tltr,ts ()l i.l(,1.(.li.rrs llt.lryt,t'rr rltllr.tt.ll ,lt;r,lrt r ontlrnlttltottr ol ttttltVttltt;tl', {t'1'
llr,rnrcks ancl Hunte. 1983). The two methods can provide conflicting data. For in two of six llocks of chickens studied by Guhl ( 1953), he found correlaI rr,rrs bctwccn numbcr of contests won (method I ) and number of individuals domrrr,rlcrl rn tlrc l)ock (n-rethod 2) were less than 0.50. Data collection in the second rrirturirl. gnrrrp) contcrt is nrore tinre consuming. but it will generally allow you to
, r.rrrrplc.
,
(,n.,lrucl lr nr()l'c virlrtl hicrarchy. llrc helurvior':rl trnits sclected fi>r measurement (e.g. Hausfater, 1975), the
r .1111'ql(s) rrr
u'lriclr irrlcrircliorrs urc obsel'ved (described above), and the criteria of
,l,,lllllf :rrrt'r' lrrvr' vrrrrctl rvitlcly (llckoll. lt)llb, Kaufhrann, 1983). Ivan Chase (pers. r r,nunun ) lr;rs srrl'1'1'slerl tlurl lltcrc ltt'c lrt lclst tltt'cc tnethttcls of deciding when one
.rrur,rl
lr.r'. tlorrrrrr;tlt'rl iur()lll('r'rlrrtittp,;ul irpprcssivc inlct':tctirltt. Irirst. yoLl cein use
l)rlr,u \ \('l urlrrrlrrr'. t rrlt'r lotr lr;rsr'rl rr;lorr ollsr'tt;tliotts ol'tltc itttitttitls'behitvIot ('\,unIl1'. ('lr;rst'(l')S ))rrst'rl llrt'l,rll,r\\nll'( lrl('ulr in ltts sttttly ol'lricnuclty
,rrr .rr
I',t l,
r1 111,11
lr
)ll ltl ltt'tt r lttt l.t'lt',
SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
EXAMPLESoFDATACoLLECTIONANDDESCRIPTION
One animal was considered to dominate another if she: (l) delivered any jump ons, combination of three strong aggressive contact actions (pecks, and claws) to the other and (2) there was a 30 minute period following
Table 10.1. An example of a dyadic interac'tion ntutrix (see text.for explanation) Loser
the third action during which the receiver of the actions did not attack IChase, l9B2:220] the initiator. an Secondly, you can use a binomial approach; that is, in each aggressive interaction e.g' fleeing; or submission (based on individual is scored as either a winner or loser it wins 1963). An animal is considered dominant over another individual if
Brown,
significantly more (e.g. binomial test; see chapter l4) than it loses in encounters with the other individual. Thirdly. Chase has suggested that you could use a combination of the first two methods. priAnother common criterion for the expression of a dominance relationship is ority of access to a limited resource (e.g. lbod, shelter, space, estrus female, etc')' demonstrated through the supplanting of one individual by another without overt aggression being displayed (see Richards' l0 measures and below). Dominance does not always provide priority of access to all valued resources; hence, there may be dilferent dominant-subordinant relation-
Priority of
access can be
D D Winner
10. I ).
C 24
E
0
A
2t
11
C
t2
B
5t
t6 3l
a
J
0
0
13
0
0
0
t4
The dyadic interaction matrix that results provides the basis for generating a
tIominance hierarchy.
Brown (197 5) provided the following list of steps to lollow in the construction of rr clyadic interaction matrix (dominance matrix):
limited
Observation.r:
BlD. C>A, B>A, C>B, B>D, etc.: BID
(Huntinglord and Turneq 1987). When individuals are ranked by different criteria, the rankings are often not comparable (Bekoff, 1977b; Bernstein, 1970; Syme, 1974). However, Richards of (1974) used the ten factors listed below to assess dominance rank in six groups
an encounter
with D. In most
captive rhesus monkeys and lound that they produced comparable results'
matrix. as illustrated in Table 10.1.
ships for different resources
t
Starting matrix'. Enter the number of wins and loses observed in the Treutment of'reversals: A win by one individual over another that has won the majority of encounters with the first is termed a reversal. Rearrange the order so that only reversals fall below the diagonal, so far as possible;
that is, change the above order to CBDAE or CBEDA or CBAED. Trt'utntent of intransivity: An order in which an individual dominates another (wins the majority of encounters) that dominates the first is tcnncd intransitive or circular. Rearrange to minimize the inevitable
encounters
rrnrbiguity. F'rom the circular relationship shown in Figure
+ Gestures for fear-submission
l0.l I there are
llrrcc nurin irlte rnatives, as shown. In the three alternatives not shown. the
a Yielding ground/avoidances b Cautious aPProaches c Nonsexual presentations/mountings d Fear-grins lo.2.2b Dominance hierarchies tlctcttrtirttrtp, The data collectecl on aggrcssive intcrirclions. blsctl ort lltc ct'itcrilr lirr (lrr'[',1()tll) is listt'tl ilr itrtlivitlrr;rl l'lrt'lt nr:rlrix. lr irrlo crrlcrctl winncr-s rrntl loscr.s. lrc
Irl.tt',t.:rt'ltltxisol'tlrt.nt:trti\.()n(.;t\lslsllrllr'lr'rl
forms of
Starting order: Choose an arbitrary order, e.g. DEACB.
Order to approach experimenter during food offers
z Agonistic : DisPlaYs
means B won
cases, these encounters take the
supplanting rather than fighting.
PrioritY to food incentives a Order to dailY food ration b Order to milk bottle c Interactions at milk bottle
d
0
t7 4t
\\nllr('r'.. lltt'otltt't lost'ts(st't'l;tlrlt'
I
) ---=---=. [i 24
lr,'lttll
I )t.tl,t,tttt,,1 llr,' rttlt,tn',tlttr' lroll llrr' trr,rlrrr ttr l,rlrlr' lll
rrrtlivirlrrlrls
A. I) lrrrtl l:
SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
ITXAMPLE,S OF DATA COLLECTION AND DESCRIPTION
332
o
t33
one' Place departure from linearity involves two individuals rather than pro(lowest relationships ambiguous the individuals that are in tlie least the minimize to tends procedure portion of reversals) in linear orcler. This
inclividuals except the alpha, the third-ranking individual (gamma) dominates all inclividuals except alpha and beta. and so on down the hierarchy. In nonlinear lricrarchies. there are one or more intransitive (circular) relationships, such as indi-
total number of encounters entered below the diagonal. Final mati.r'.The one order that best reflects the order of dorninance within the gror.rp is then CBADE. A matrix may then be constructed'
vidr-ral
A dominating B, and B dominating C, but C dominating A. Rankings in a lricrarchy and type of hierarchy can change. For example, Murchison (1935) found tlrat the ranking in a group of six domestic fowl roosters changed from a nonlinear Io a linear hierarchy as they matured (Figure 10. l2). Perfectly linear hierarchies are relatively rare, making most hierarchies techni-
Best
r'llly nonlinear. Perfectly linear hierarchies are unidirectional. They can contain
DE
rcvcrsals (i.e., a subordinate wins an occasional encounter with a dominant individ-
D
A D
A
E
E
D
A
rutl), but they cannot contain any individuals of equal status or have any circularity ''trclt as: A---B---,C. The nonlinearity is, however, of varying degrees, and some so r
losel! approximate perfectly linear hierarchies that they should be considered
Irrrcitt'.
r
BADEWinsLosses
I-utrclau's index oJ' linearity has been discussed by Bekoff (1977b) and Chase l')71\. The index (ft) is calculated according to the following formula:
,r:(fr;)
59
C
109
t4
32
14
D
2l
70
l',
E
l3
t25
f
B
)[,
,-@-2lttlL
rr lrcl'C:
A
an ordinal scale The dominance hierarchies described above rank individuals on Boyd and Silk ( 1983) (see chapter 8);that is. C ranks above B, and B ranks above A' cardinal index of domdescribe a more complex, statisticalmethod lbr generating a
paired comparisons' It inance rank (versus the ordinal heirarchy above) based upon information on interactions that result in wins. losses ar-rd ties' They
incorporates
.then -r1.96, the kurtosis is not significantly rliflerent from zero at the 0.05 level; that is, this distribution of song durations is neither significantly leptokurtic nor platykurtic. Since the calculated
Z of -0.891
is
4.7000
0.6400
0.s 120
0.4096
5.1000
1.4400
1.7280
2.0736
3.2000
0.4900
-0.3430
0.2401
4.2000
0.0900
0.0270
0.0081
3.7000
0.0400
-0.0080
0.0016
0.0400
0.0080
0.0016
I-his test determines whether the variances
4.1000
0.3600
0.2160
rrre
4.5000
0.t296
3.6000
0.0900
-0.0270
0.0081
2.9000
1.0000
- 1.0000
1.0000
3.0000
0.8100
-0.1290
0.6561
39.0000
5.0000
0.3840
4.5284
If Z>t
t2.t.tb F-max
variance of Pop.
z
0'0384 :o.lo86 M1 on"o: o.50(o.7o7l) MrlM, __ 3)
:
:
.7
421:9.55
largest variance smallest
_0.68_ l '23 variance 0.55 ,
Obtain the tabular value of Flrom Table A4. Two diflerent degrees of
the smaller variance. In our case both degrees of freedom are 9
(cll'-
,_ SK - 0.1086 -0.1086-0.158 --Vvst 10 use: '',,':V-*,, If an1'measurement (10 use: *,,':f {t,,+0'5) normal distribution (e'g' Mendl' This transformation has also been used to create a 1988 ).
A nonparametric statistical test is a test whose modeldoes not specily conditions about the parameters of the population from which the sample was
drawn.
tsiegal, t9-t6..1l
I
Since there are lewer constraints on nonparametric tests, they are usually lcss powerful when used to analyze data where parametric tests are applicable (howcvcr
Blair and Higgins, 1985). Therefore, researchers often proceed with paranrctrie without having necessarily satisfied the four criteria listed above (p. 373). This is supportable, in part. by the fact that some parametric tests are robust; that is, they can be used with reasonable validity even when some ol the parametric model assumptions are violated. For example, Student's /-test can be used even when there is considerable deviation from normality and/or homogeneity of varianoe, except in an independent-samples design with unequal numbers of scores; however, analysis of variance is highly sensitive to the kurtosis of a population ( Govindarajulu, 197 6). Overall, there are several factors which should be considered when selecting tretween parametric and nonparametric tests. Gibbons (1993) has compiled a list which serves as the basis for a safe (yet sometimes conservative) guideline. According to Gibbons (1993) (Jse a nonparafiTetric statistic'al test then uny o./'tha see
tests
Iollow'ing zre true:
t2.l.2h Logarithmic ffanqformation
t
create a norntal distribution in The logarithmic transformation is generally used to used when the measurethe measurements (e.g. Lawrence 1985): it is commonly
categories: nominal scale of measurement).
z r
is given by: ments are skewed to the right. The transformed measurement
some
of
the measurements are zero. or very small" use:
rtnk rttlwrt.
n)()l'c rcprcscntutive than tlre mean.
irrrtl pl'()P()l'lirrtts (o ctclttc lt The arcsine transformatior-r is used with percentagcs
tlistrr' norrrrnldislrihuti,, (e.g.Shcrryt'ltrl..l98l:Mc.tll. lgltl{)'cs1'rcciirllVtvltctrtlre
\,r
.r;tlt'slttt'\
shapes of'the distributions from which the samples are drawn are
: TItc sarnple size is small. r' 'f ltc tncasurentents are imprecise. u l'hcrc ltrc outliers and/or extreme values in the data, making the median
2.1.2c At'csine transformation
blrti.n al-,-rcirsrrr.cnrurts
The assumptions required for the validity of the corresponding paramet-
+ The
,r,,':log,,,(x,rf l)
t
The data are measured on an ordinal scale.
ric procedure are not met or cannot be verified.
r -':log,u('t,r) If
The data are counts or frequencies of dilferent types of outcomes (i.e.
ts is birrorrriirl.'l'lrc lnrnslirt'tttctl tttt'ltsttte tttt'ttt
r,,
I'irt'tt lt\
ll
tlrt'tllrlrt rnccl llrc rrsstrnrpliorrs lirr prrrrrurctric tcsts then parametric
tests
will
lrr'tttr)r('l)()\\'('rlrtl. lr,ru't'r,r't. llrt'nrotr.'lltc rlltl:t viol:rlr'(ltc lrssrrrttl'rtions litr paramet-
ttt'lr'sls.lltt'tttt)t('l)()\\'('tlttllr,,tr;)iu;rttl('lttt (/;rr. l')S.l)
lt'rl',ltt'r'otttt'tt'l;tlirt'lrlllltt':ttttctt'ictcsts
SELEC.TION OF A STATISTICAL TT]ST
Powar-e/ficiency is a measure of the amount of increase in sample size necessary
to make test B as powerful as test tests which can be
I
t3 Parametric statistical tests
(Siegel, 1956). It can be used to compare any two
validly applied to the data, such as comparable parametric and
nonparametric tests (Welkowitz et al., 1916). Given that the data meet the criteria fbr use of parametric test, then fbr a given difference between population rleans, a given alpha level and a specified power, power-eflficiency is a ratio expressed as a percent as lollows: Power-efficiency of nonparametric Where:
{:sample P
t.rt:9x
size for the parametric test.
t/ilp :sample size necessary for the nonparametric test to make it as the
Since much of the data gathered in ethology do not meet the assumptions necessary to use parametric statistical tests, only a few of the more commonly applied para-
100"1,
N,,t,
as
metric tests are describecl below. Also, nonparametric statisticaltests can be applied to data which meet the assumptions for parametric tests; however, in those cases the parametric tests will be more powerful.
powerful
parametric test.
For example. if the parametric test requires a sample size of 80 and the nonparametric test requires a sample size of 100 to make it as powerful as the parametric test, then the power-efficiency is: Power-efficiency of nonparametric
In this
test:
case, the nonparametric test is 80'2, as
Jqx 100
I3.I CoMPLETELY RANDOM TZED DESIGN 100'Zr:80'Zr
13.r.l Two independent samples
powerlul as the parametric test.
The power-efficiency is provided for several of the nonparametric tests discussed in
tJ.l.te Standard ewor of
the
the foll owing ch apters.
dffirence betneen means
we can compare means from two samples ancl determine if they are significantly tlifrerent, that is. whether they came frorn significantly different populations or
rvhether there was a significant treatment eflect. The standard error of the difference means is computed according to the foilowing rormura:
rl'the
sEo-rri:-:
l, 1 //'t'-*t:-\ -
{ \1,r,
1\
l,r,
I
/
The symbols.r,z, s,2 and Np N, represent the variances and sample sizes of s:tttrplcs I and 2" respectively. If the difl-erence between the two means is larger than tNo titres the standard error of the difference. they are significantly different. Iirr exitmple. assume we want to test the research hypothesis
that the mean dura-
lr.tl ol'sottg bottts in Population A of a bird species is significantly different than it
rs
itt l)opttlittiort
IJ.
we randomly sample l0 males lrom each population and recorcl
llrt'tlttt':ttiorl ol'ottc ranclomly selected song bout from each male rr.trkl rr.r'rr:rlly rirkc, rnuch larger sample than this).
t
(see below; we
( 'rtlt'trlrrtc lllc lotitl rtttrl tttclrr song bout duration (in seconds) lor each poprrl;rtiorr
382
COM PLETELY RAN DOM
PARA M ETRIC STATISTICAL TESTS
Sample Ponulation
b
SamPle
A
PoPulatiort B
0.8
4.8
5.1
1.2
1.44
6.4
-1./
-0.1
0.49
5.3
4.2
0.3
0.09
3'l
3.1
-0.2
0.04
5.0
4.1
0.2
0.04
4.4
4.5
0.6
0.36
5.2
3.6
-0.3
0.09
4.9
2.9
-
4.7
3.0
-0.9
Total:49.0
39.0 3.9
sr:
(-t,-X)' 0.09
0.81
Total:ffi: f(.r,- X )r 9
J
We can then calculate the standard
0.01 2.25
sE--: /tn'*tu': 'A'B NB
0.16 3.24
error of the diflerence of the means:
floos.oi'ru): Vo. 124:0. r24:0.35
Vt
V/Vo
tO
The diflerence between the means (4.9-3.9:
0.01 0.25
sr-\A rR - :0.35x2:0.10;
0.09
significant.
1.0)
is larger than twice the
therefbre, the difference between the means is statistically
0.00 0.04
Total= 6.14-X
\,-.\'r)
!1"I {)':94:o.u, 9 N-l
,^:,/[
1.00
1.0
N-t
Sample: PoPulation A
(,.,-X) 0.3 -0.1 1.5 0.4 - 1.8 0.1 -0.5 0.3 0.0 -0.2
0.64
Ir.r,-xrr ' !_ : 5.00 :0.55
Calculate the stanclctrtl tleviution for both samples'
a
$,-*)
(x,- x )'
4.1
4.9
3ul
Sample: Population B:
5.2
Mean:X:
lZEl) I)l:S l( ; N
!''i
l'l:vo
t.1.t.lh Student's t-test Student's /-test is also used to test for significant difl-erences between two sets of data
comparison of means. We will use the same data on song duration ll'orn the two populations that we used in the previous examples. In those examples wc testecl lirr the assumptions of most parametric tests and found: 1. there is homo-
rund is based on a
oa:0 8]
gcrrcity ol' variirnr.:c bctween Populations A and B, and 2. the data from Population
Ii irrc rrcithcr siunilicantly skewed nor leptokurtic or platykurtic.
We should check
tlrc norrrr:rlitv ol'llrc riatit in Population A belore we proceed, but we also krtow that tlre l-test is srrllicicrrlly nrbrrst so llrat lhcsc r.rssunrptions can be violated to a reason;rlrlr.'t'xlr'rrl u'illrorrt ;rllt'c'tirrp'llrc vrrlitlity ol'lhc tcst. Also. all the lirctors in the l( )r
lulrl;r lt:rr t' lr l t t';trlV l)('('n
(';r
lt ul;rlt'rl ( rtlro1
1'
1.
(x^-x,)l( /t
COMPLETELY RAN DOM IZED I)trs t( ; N
ETRIC STATISTICAL TESTS
PA RA M
,ffi)
luo- I xsA:)+( Nu-
Table 13.1. Song bout tluration
| x,sB2)
(s)
Population samples
D
// ro, rot 4.9-3.9 | ! \ l0+ l0/
Row totals
(r)
I
/r
lo-
I
V
xo.7l )+( ro+
lo-
ro, -
4.7
3.9
5.1
18.9
5.1
4.2
5.9
20.0
6.4
3.2
3.9
4.8
18.3
5.3
4.2
3.1
'oV(^ l
5.2
17.8
3.1
_t.
I
3.6
4.9
15.3
5.0
4.1
4.1
5.3
18.5
4.4
4.5
3.2
5.4
t7.5 16.6
VL r8
|
l
(1.0\(2.24t 2.24i-:2-84 2.24 i-- ; v0.62 0.]e l(6.12+s.4 \
V\ r8 )
difference between means (above).
13.1.2 Three or more independent samples
3.6
3.0
4.8
2.9
2.7
5.2
15.7
4.7
3.0
2.9
5.5
l6.l
39.0
34.6
52.1
Table 13.2 Sum Source of variation
df
Mean square
lletween samples (columns)
BSSS
BSMS
Within samples
WSSS
WSMS
lirtal
TSS
tests fbr significant diflerences between
applied to a wide variety of experimental designs; see Meddis (1973) for a clear and
calculate the correction Term:
concise overview. The one-way ANOVA described in the example below is fbr thc
completely randomized design. We will once again use the hypothetical clata orr song duration that we used in the examples above; however, wc will expantl our hypothesis and samples from Populations A and B to include two nlorc popr.rlirtions C and D (Table 13.1).
z
of
squares
three or more independent samples of measurements. Variations of this test can be
t
114.7:GT
(rows)
l3.I.2a One-way analysis of variance
of variance (ANOVA)
5.2 4.9
Column totals(r):{9.9
We then obtain the tabular value for I from Table A,5 for l8 degrees of freedom (dl) and a significance level of P:0.05. Since our calculated I of 2.84 is larger than the tabular r value of 2.101, we conclude that the data are from two distinctly different populations. That is, song duration in Population A is statistically greater than it is in Population B; this agrees with our comparison using the standard error of the
One-way analysis
5.2 4.8
/r roo i /f rqrto.os r +r2y1o.s6)
l:
xo.s6)
I
Cirlctrlitlc lhc lrlt:rls lirr c:rclr slrrrrlllt'(t'olrrnrll.
tr
I
t
//:total
5.lr t4.lJr1(r.4r...
-5.5r
ll.0-l
.
17 0-l I
'/()li
.)
1
.10.()(r
.
10.25
I
( 'ttlt ttl;tlt'lltr'lol.rl,,urrt ol ,.rltr.rrt.., ( ISS)
,,,,
I iI I
number of measurements:40
('alculutc thc sum of squares of the measurements (),r,r2); that is, square circh ol'thc inclividual measurements and sum them.
)'\,,
Complete the analysis ol variance (Tahlc 13.2) hclow by nrlkirrg llrc r':rlculations in Steps 2 13. I{csults irrc lorrtttl irr 'ljrble I l.l
t ('ltlt'ttlrttt'lltt'tol;tls lirt t'rtt'lt r()\\' , | I ('rrlr rrlrrlt'llrt'1,1.ur(llol,rl(( i1 I t iI
whcre:
174'12 :763 CT:GT': N 40
ISS )r
( I
1,)li'l
'(,
I
r'',1
PARA M ETRIC STATISTICAL TESTS
RANDOMIZED BLOCK. MAT('ttt:t) t,n tRs
Calculate the between sample sum of squares (BSSS): L2+ t.2+ r,2+ r *^2
BSSS:''
'
n(,
both conditions (treatments) or they could be meersurements paired by some characteristic (e.g. litter, time, location).
-CT
t
where: n,,:number of scores in each column (sample) 240 t
+ t s2r + t te7l9
a)!4
_ t 63
Calculate:
: 20.36
It:
- l-o* 7115'', ! V N-I
Calculate the within samples sum of squares (WSSS):
N
WSSS:TSS-BSSS
:35.21-20.36:14.85
Where:
Caiculate the degrees of freedom (d0: Between-samples
df:Number of
samples (columns)-
1:3
l
Within-samples df:(Number of rows - 1)(Number of columns) : ( 1 0- I )(4): 36 Total df:Number of measurements (4,1- l:39
ll Calculate each mean square (MS) by dividing the sum of squares by the
il
D:diflerence between each pair of measurements D:mean of the diflerence between each pair ol measurements ly':number of pairs of measurements
z
Compare the calculated t to the tabular varue (Table A5), where df:N - 1. If the calculated value of r>tabular value. then the null hypothesis of no signiflcant difference between the samples is rejected.
Table 13.3
corresponding df Between samples mean square
Within
20.36
Sum
(BSMS):
-:6.78 y!:O.orrt samples mean square (WSMS): '36
Source of variation
l3
Fof
16.445 is larger than the tabular value
(2.81), we reject the null hypothesis of no significant clifl-crer.rce between the samples.
13.2
RANDOMIZED BLOCK. MATCHED PAIRS OIt I{l:l'>lrA'l'lrl) MEASURES DESIGN
l3.z.t Two rclated or matched samplcs 1.1.2.1t Paircil t-tast
l'lrt.1l:rirt.,l I tr'sl rs rrst'tl lo tt'sl lirr sil,tttltt:rnl rltllt'r('lt(("' lrr'lttt't'tt ltto tr'l;tlt'tl ot ttt;tlt llt'tl ";tttr1,l,"' llr."'t'
r
ilttltl lrt' ttlr',t',tll('lll('lll ' Ill lll'",tlll('
Within-samples
20.36
6.78
36
14.85
0.4t
39
35.21
16.44
(rows)
Total
16.445
Compare the calculated Fvalue to the tabular value (Table A6) using the between-samples df (3), the within-samples df (36)and the appropriate alpha level. The tabular value for P:0.05 is 2.87 (extrapolated lrom 2.92 and 2.84). Since our calculated
Mean square
(columns)
duration between the populations calculate the between-samples Fvalue:
t?TPlgnl4! : r: q:l:ttn Within-samples MS
of
squares
Between-samples
To test the hypothesis that there is a significant dillerence in song bout
Between-samples
dr
lll(ltt t'ltt'tl" rttttlt't
As an example, we will provide hypothetical data on songbird species
r
(Table
l3'4)' similar to that recorded by Reid (1987) lor Ipswich sparrows (pas.serculus princcp.r). our research question is whether time spent singing is greater than time spent fbraging in a habitat with an abundant food supply. We t'itttcloltlly selccted l0 individual males from a population of l8 and took focal,rundwiL'hcn,si,s
lrtlitllitl/itll-occurrences samples, measuring the time spent singing and foraging tlttriitg lltc ltours 06(X) to 0900. Total observation time for each male was l0 hours.
PA RA M
388
Table
RANDOMIZED BLOCK. MAT( llI:l) I)AlRS
ETRIC STATISTICAL TESTS
Table 13.5. Copulutions hy males w,ith eli//ert'nt mutirtg histories to virginJbmulc
hy ten male 13.4. H1-pothetical dota on time ,spent singing ancl.fctroging
monarch butterflies
songbirds
Singing
Individual
Foraging
pz
D
A
105
152
47
2209
B
97
202
r05
11025
C
ll5
117
2
D
95
233
E
120
105
l5
F
87
215
188
G H
103
176
89
260
I
t12
131
l9
J
109
139
30
| 032
| 190
Totals:
I
Time since
Total time 1min.)
138
t-)
t7l
788(rD)
985
1986
last mating
Number
0/
Number
/0
(days)
tested
Mated
tested
Matctl
0{
x)'] [Atrr
j]Y)21
Where: 3.162
_ 78.8 _78.8:3.66 67 .97 21.5 3.162
2'262' Silcc ttttr citlcttThe tabular / value (Table A5) for df:9 and P:0'05 is //,,' irtrtl cottcltrtlc tltit( the reject we lated f (3.66) is larger than the tabular value, singirrg. trrttl there is a significant difference between time spcnt lirraging itt ttl:tlc As another example, oberhauser (1988) sttrtlictl tttittittg slt'ltlcp,it's rtl'tttltlcs tttltlctl itl l')s(t tlt:rtt monarch butterflies ancl lilLrnrl thirt ir lowcr'1'rcrccrrl:rgc () ()ll)' ;l tt.sttll sltr' ;tlttilltttt.tl lrr lttl in 1985 (Trrhlc 13..5; |llrir.ctl l l.l' (ll. l.i. /, lrr, lltt'ttttltllrt't .l ttttttstrtlly t.rl.l srrnunt.r. Nolt. llutl llrr'nr('ir\lnr'rttt'ttl\ itl('l);lttt'rl
tllrVs stltt t' t lrt' llr'.t ttt:tl tttl'
l/: Xf: X: l: .\'r )"'
Number of pairs of scores
of the products of the paired scores of the scores of one varil,, -'(X) sr.rrtr of the scores of the other v. , iable ( Y) surn ol'the squared scores of the Xvariable sr.rnr ()l'thc squared scores of the I'variable sum
sunr
l'lrc nrn1,,t'ol'
r'is
l.(X) to + l.(X). antl thc sign of r denotes whether the correla-
Ironisposilivt'orrrcp,lrlive. l'lrcllrrgcr llrcr vrrlrrc.tltcnrorchighlycorrelatedarethe trro st'ls ol rl;rllr Ilrt'srllnilir'rurt'r'ol r (Irottt rl to llrt'r.rlrrr','l r. ur l;rl,lt','\
/
I lrr., r',.r
l\\,'
r
O)r'rrtt l'rr'tlclct'lrtittctl hy crtrtrpltt'ing
l,nlr'rl tt'sl li,t ;t stl,ttilit';tttl t'ott'cllt(iort.
rt';';rr,llt".', rtl lltr",tf,n ol r ll r t', l,rt1',', llr.rtr r llr,'rr \r)u r('l('( I llrr.'//, ;rtttl tottt'ltttlt' llr,rl llrr'l\\l t,tt t,tlrl,'', ( \ ) l,rt, ,t1'tttlt, ,rrrll\ ,,'t t''l,tl, rl
RAM ETRIC STATISTICAL TESI'S
PA
390
on a songbird species that As an example, we will use the same hypotheticaldata since this is only a hypothetical was used for the paired /-test (above). However, by l0 in order to keep the calculaexample, we will divide each of the measurements to calculate Pearson's coelficient tions more manageable. All the variables necessary assign silging as the X variable and are provicled in Table 13.6. We will arbitrarily
t4 Non par ametric statistical
tests
foraging as the Yvariable' t
:
tI
:Vtt :
-!t!1? [L?2!I - I 0rz'l t t ot :s 3s'94) -
I 0( I 7 87. 60)
V{
0(
r
oio-os)
l7 876.00- l8 472.80 ro zoo'so- ro oioi+L(-rs 3se'40-
va, ,r.*!"9' *o,,:
1
79'021
}
Nonparametric statistical tests are the most commonly used statistical tests in ethological research. They are simple tests that can easily be calculated by hand or with a
-32 041'00)l
hand-held calculator. This means they can be used reliably while you are in the field access to a computer.
without
;t'2#: -o'e8
Since nonparametric tests are so easily conducted, it is tenrpting to apply them to alldata. However, parametric statistical tests should be r-rsed when the data meet the
criteria (see section
Table 13.6 Total time (min.;
Male
Singing
Foragtng
(n
(v)
Y
XY
,n 231.04
r0.5
15.2
159.6t)
9.7
195.94
94.09
408.04
B
20.2
C
I 1.5
tr.1
134.55
t32.2s
136.89
90.25
542.89
l)
9.5
23.3
221.35
E
12.0
10.5
126.00
144.00
21.5
239.2s
G
10.3
11.6
181.28
106.09
309.16
8.9
26.0
231.40
19.21
676.00
H
I
11.2
13. I
146.12
125.44
171 .61
13.9
I 51.51
193.21
J
10.9
I 18.81
03.2
179.0
I 076.08
I535.94
Sums:
()x')
(;n
1
787.60
()xr)
GXN
One variable
l1.l.lq
One sample
7
(f l'l
S,
S,.,
,rl
:
-r,:
Sample of variable rl (or Treatment) Measurement on Inclividual No. I
_Y1
s6.25
8.7
r
l.t.l.l
I 10.25
15.69
F
l. I ), since they are more powerful.
I4,I COMPLETELY RANDOMIZED DESIGN
110.25
A
12.
:
.Y,,
()ttc .sutttpIL' run,t I(,tI 'l'his tcst tlctcrntines whether a sequence of two dillerent items (in time or space) is n()n-ril D(l()ln.
Slrrrplc sc(lr.rctrcc: A A B B B A B B B B A A B A The calculated coefficient
of -0.98
Since the calculated r.or- --0.gg is rargcr
inclicates a vcry strotlg ttcg:ttivc cttt't'clrttiott' tlic tubrrl.r'r', .r'0 (r-rr. u'e rr'rr'('t tlrr' //,,
trrir.
slrcrrr ar- na c.rrcl.titlrr irrtr c.rtclrrrrc thrrt lirrrc clt tlt lv. ttel'ltt ively t'ot.t t'l;ttt'tl'
lir*r,irr,
rrrrtl sirrl'rrrr';rr. sr,'rrili
As cxlrrrrplr.'s. !ltc sc(lucncc irbove cotrkl lrc the:
I '
St'r
1
t
t('tl('(' t tl' or'r'u
Sr't1ttt'nlt;tl,rs1lr,'I It'lt'Pltr)n('\\
t't
r'n('('
(
)l l tlo lrr'lrlrvirlrs
(
A. l|
).
ol ttr;rlt't,\)iut(ll('nlitlt'(ll)st;rrlingsg'lcrclrctl irlrtng:r
il('
1 St'rIl('nlr.rlurrlt'r
,'l ',1r,', 1,1 \ l.urrlr rrrr , tllt lrr,'tl rrl,rl rr lt'r'tl lrtttth
NON PARAM
COMPLETELY RAN DOM lZF.l) I) llS I ( ; N
ETRIC STATISTICA L T.I:STS
or separated in want to determine whether the two items are clustered chance' The two non-random the sequence more than would be expected by extremes,for the example above, would be:
ln any
case we
Table 14.1. Numbers of ./bmale grasshopper,\ thut hucl rutt given response chirps to males that subsequently mated or did not mute Trials which ended with:
BBBB Non-randomly clusten:r/: A A A A A A B B B B
q Non-randomly separared:B A A EA Example: Ho: The sheep
1
q4E4E4B
B
Mating
No mating
ll
23
in2h
Female did not give
bunk' (A) and cows (B) are randomly distributed along the feed
response stridulation Sourc'e: From
Determine the number of runs (r)'
Butlin et al. (1985).
AABBBABBBBAABA,,-,
T 2 3 4 Ti'7'-'
Table 14.2. Calc'ulation of chi-square .from data in Table
14.
I
Determine:
Q-D2
No:number of A items:6 Nu:number of B items:8
Response
o
A82) for No and N'' The Compare r with the tabular values (Tables A8t' (r) is significant at the 0.05 level, if: r is less than or equal
number of runs
Mating No mating
23
t7 t7
totheisvalueinTableA8,orrisgreaterthanorequaltothevaluein
'fotals
34
34
Table A8,.
than
Q-q2
-6
36
2.1
+6
36
2.1 18
l8
12
In our example, 7 is larger than 3 (Table 18,) and smaller (Table,48,);therefore,wecannotrejectthef/othatthesheepandcowsare
Expected
randomlY distributed' One samPle c'hi-stluare te'\t
can make one of only two This test is used to analyze clata in which individuals or right, fly or don,t fly). It should be used responses (e'g. accept or reject, turn left less than 5, use the binontial test when the expected values are >5. If they are only
the chi-squared statistic in (described below). Additional precautions when using (1992)' ethology are given by Kramer and Schmidhammer
(E):total
number of responses equally distributed between
each category
of response:
ll +23:34
3412:17
We would expect 17 to mate and 17 not to mate
if they had
an equal
(50:50) chance of reponding either way.
f :2:4.236 .t ('orrpare the calculated I to the the value in Table ,{9 with a degree of ll'ccclonr ol' I (df:no. categories- I ). If the calculated f is greater than or
:
. 2 \x.,-: -[(observed-exPetttq]-0'5]1 exPected
Calculate chi-square,
ctltral to thc tabular value, then the
F10
that there is no significant differ-
crrcc in thc lcnralcs' responses is rejected.
'l'ltc ('llculatcd
f'enlalc gritsshoppcrs For example, Butlin et al. (1935) measured whether ttl tl]alcs strhsctltrctrtly (Chorthippus brunnneus) that did not give respc-rnse chirps mated (Table 14.1).
lo
H,,: Females that clid not givc rcspottsc cllit'Ps nratc. as ttot tl-tittc.
I ('otttplt'tt'tltt'( l:rlrlt' I I )) ( )lr.'r'l tr',1 ( t )) t'' tltt'tl,tl'l ll(llll l 'rlrlt' I I
ll
(o-D
f
ot-
4.236 is larger than the tabular value of 3.841
10.05 lcvcl ol'sisrrilicirncc); thcrcfirre, we reject the I1,,and conclude that
tlrt' li'turrlcs rttlrlctl lcss ol'lcn lltirrt woultl bc cxpccted by chance.
tttrtlcs wct'c etlttitlly likcly ltr lJtttrtttttrtl lt'.tl I rl.t'lltt't
ltt ',(ltlitt('lt'rl;tlrort'
rtr rr ltt. lr llrr't('t',,r I
lltt'l)lll()lttt,rl
lrrr'rltr lr'rl tlrlr rlrnlr,lr
1,".1 t'.;t ()tt('slttttPlt','rttltltless-rll-lit
l,r'lr\r'r'r
l\\o rlt',t rt'lt'r ,tlr'1,ot tt's ll is ll
lcst l'1r1y1l
394
N
ON PA RAM ETR
IC STATISTICA L I. I,STS
COMPLETELY RANDOM \ZF.D I)t:S l(
replacement fbr the chi-square test when any o1'the expected tiequencies are (5. The binomial test determines the probability of obtaining ,r events (the smallest observed value), or fewer, in one category and l/-"r events in the other category, out
of
a
Table 11.3. Alurnt reac,liotr of lttutl luryut, to 600 s) towards
total of Iy'events.
Experimental half
Calculate the probability,p(x), using the lollowing fbrmula: N!
/(v): ,t t,ny- rttPt 2A
395
(ue,\
total of l/ events. The researcher must specify the expected probabilities.
l.
;N
Control half
N
l1
20
t
Sourt,e: From Hews (I9gg).
where: N! means the factorial of
l/
2' A more appropriate pr-obability to calculate for most ethological experiments probability of obtaining .y, or fewer, events in one category and N-r events in the other category out of a total of Nevents.
P:expected proportion of r
Q:l-
is the
P
__ N!_ : : o,'omial .r! (I/-r)! (T)
coerficient
This part of the formula can be calculated using lactorials (Table
Al) or by
determining the binomial coefficient using Table A3.
As an example, Hews (1988) tested the alarm reaction of toad (Bu.fo boreas) larvae to chemical cues released from predation by a waterbug on conspecific and heterospecific larvae. The data in Table 14.3 below are only fbr the tests with preda-
tion on a conspecific larva in the experimental half of the test tank. The 20 larvae could choose the half of the tank where the predation was occurring (experimental half) or the other half (control hal0. We will use an expected probability of 0.50 in eacl-t half of the tank. That is,
P:0.5
and
To calculate this probability, sum the probability of the observed events r and all the more extreme distribr-rtions. This is accomplishecl by successively reducing,r by I and calculating the probability lor each value including zero. This total probability is the probability of obtaining the observed distribution of events, or more extreme values. For our example, the probability of having 3. 2, I or 0 larvae
choose the experimental lialf of the tank is the sum of each of the individual prob-
abilities.
/ A/\
1(-t):\,
p(3):0.00 I (calculated above)
N:20 -r:smallest number of larvae choosing half of tank:3
,('):(T)Pxe'r-' :(
/,(' I
i()ll
).
+
):(/20\,ir0.s,Xo 5,',):20(0.5X l.9l,,): 1.90-s /
Thereft>re.0.001 is tltc problrbility ol'lrirvirrg exrtcllv lltt'cc lrtt'r'rtc t'lloosc tlte t'rpt'tintcntirl lurll'ol'llrc llrrrk. il'tlte c\[rr'('t!'11 rtrrrnlrt't is l0 (lr;tst'tl ()ll iln t't1tt;tltltsltilrtt I
r,il
l0\
(
1t(0)
140x0.5r)(0.5r7)
I 140X0. 125\(l .63 n):0.(X)
'
t0:(20)(0.5r)(o 5IB): I90(0.2511.1.8t n):l.8l
Q: I -P:0.5
:(l
)r'Q'
.l)
t ,,i"''s",10'5r{);: l(l)(9.53'):9.53
0.(x)l+l.gl
11 1.90 5+9.53 7:0.0012
I lrt'rt'lirrt'. llrt- pr6[lr[ilily 6l' lrrrvirrg lh.cc. .r' rcw'cr, Iarvae choose the experimental lr,rll ol tlrt.t;rrrk rs0 ()()l-)
ir i
I
NONPARAM ETRIC STATISTICA L TI:STS
396
t4.l.tb
COMPLETELY RANDOM IZED I)trs t(
Table l4'4' Hypothetic'al dattt on song tlurution,t fiLtnt ttto populations oJ'bircls ancl rankings used in the Mann-Whitney L) te.sr (,rce te.ut )
Two independent ssmples
Ar
Az
xt
r -trz
xzt
,\,I -1
-Y^. t7
xzz
l,:
Sample or Treatment No. I
Song duration (s)
1,, :Measurement on Individual No. I in Sample No.
I
i: .r^
_Y.
lil
Mann-Whitne1, U test The Mann-Whitney [/ test is the nonparametric counterpart of Student's l-test lor independent samples. Whereas tl-re r-test determines significant differences between means, the Mann-Whitney U test uses the medians to test for a significant difference in the location
;N
of the sample data. It
rs 95"1, as powerf
ul as the Student's /-test
(Mood, 1954). This test can be used when the data are, at least, ordinal. For large samples, the Mann-Whitney U Test is more powerful than the Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test; for
Ranks
Population A
Population B
sample
Population A
Population B
sample
sample
sample
4.7
8.t
l0
5.3
t5
4.2
t2
3.6
7
6.7
J
5.1
9.5
4.0
ll
t3
l6
2.7
5
4.1
2
r.8
6
3.8
I
7.8
4
t4
1.3
8
1.4
9
Sum
of the ranks
: ?": 68
very small samples, the Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test is more powertul (Siegel and
Castellan, 1988). If the samples are correlated (paired or matched), use the Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test. The use of both the Mann-Whitney U test and the Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-rank test on independent and paired
where:
N":number of measures in the smaller sample ly'.:number of measures in the larger sample
data, respectively, is illustrated by Breitwisch's (1988) study of parental defense in
(J
mockingbirds. As an example, we will use samples of song durations from two populations in which the variances are obviously ditferent; that is, an F-max test would be expected
to show a significant difference in the variation of bird-song duration from
these
two populations. Therefore. we will use a nonparametric statisticaltest.
r:
(7
x9 ) +1(7
%:trrN. 4
-_
P
Ur:
-,68
:
63 + 28
1l1p1- 23
:
-
68
:
23
40
Obtain the tabular value lor N,.:7, tabular value: l2
N.:9
in Table
Al0.
Llr:23 Procedure:
t
Ur:40
Rank the data (Table 14.4) using both samples. The smallest measurement gets rank no.1.
Determine the surn of the ranks in the smaller sample
it is Population B).If both samples are the same lirst sample. ?":68 Calculatc tlre {/, irnrl t/r s(:rtistics:
{',
,NrN,
' ,' I ,ry.(,ry.
ll)
I
(f;
in our cxanrplc
size. sLrnr thc ranks
ol'thc
'[.lrcrc is a sigrlilicant difference if either of the observed values (u, or ur) is r'tlttttl ltt ttr lr't't' lltutr lhc tabular vetlue. Hence, in our example the song durations are rr,t 5;ig11ilit'lrrrtly tlillcrent between the two populations. This you would suspect, 'tttt't' lllct'c is cl).t1gl1 v'ariubility in Sample B to overlap the values in Sample A; we ( iur scc tlrrs bcltcr.itr gr.irphic litrm (Figure l4.l). It, t f 1 1
1
1
1.1,,
1
1.,,
t,,\'
t t t i t. t t t t
t. I lt.r t
.t, t t t
t
t
I t I t,
I t,,s,
I
llrts lt'sl ts;t sttlrsltltllt'lirt lltt'M;rrrrr wlrrlrrt'v li lt.sl. csPcci:rlly whcn the sample ''ll('l\ rttt;tll ll rlt'lt.unnt(',, rr lr,.llr..r l\\o,,,trrrplr.r tlrllt.r siprrilit.trrrtly irr cif ltcr. litrrtr rtr Iot;lltoil I lr,rl r', rl 1t..,1,. llr,' //, llr,rl lltr.l\\,r,,.trrr;r11.,,,p(.p1t si,,rrilit.lrrrtlf,tlilli,r.t.rr(.
NON PA RAM ETRIC STATISTI('A t.'l' I,STS
398
COMPLETELY RANDOM IZED I)I:S I(;
10
N
Table 14.5. Hvpotheticuldata on tht, lurtrtiort
399
o.f
/eecling bouts in tottt herds o.f'tlecr
Feeding bout duration (rnin.)
Herd A
Herd B
43
24
62
3l
Song Duration
t
47
IJ
35
ll
Population 14.
l9
69 1-
AB Fig.
8l
29
64 Graph of hypothetical data on song duration from two populations of songbirds.
The scale of measurement must be at least ordinal. Even
if
l8
l8
the data are interval or
21
89
43
67
tl
59
ratio, they are analyzed as an ordinal scale, which means that resolution is lost. Therefore, a parametric statistical test (Student's l-test) wor-rld normally be used if
65
t2
28
the criteria (section l2.l.l) are met; however, when the sample size is small the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test is96"l' as powerful as Student's l-test (Dixon, 1954).
I Small suntples The procedure below is used when the number of measurements is
':25 in both
samples. As a hypothetical erample, we
will determine whether
the
duration of leeding bouts are significantly dilferent between two sn-rall herds of deer lrorn dilferent habitats but with the same sex and age composition (Table 14.5).
lormula would be:
Procedure:
I
1J:rnaximum [q,-^t,,]
Convert ratio or interval data to an ordinal scale. Since we are working
with ratio data. the scale of measurements must be divided into intervals. An interval should be selected such that no single interval contains more than two to three ffreasurements. Each sarriple is then arrangcd in orcler ol'
Tcst Statistic: DXmXn
llrc litrgest
dill'erence in the pretlit'ted direction is at the intervals of 45*-49 and ,{) 5-l rnirrulcs whcre .1,:0.916 and S,,ltt:0.333.
rnagnitude, and the cumulative frequencics of observutions lirr each
l)
sample up to each intervalare determined (Trrble 14.6).
:
Calculate the ratios of the cumulative freqr-rencies til thc lolrrl rrurnbcr ol' measurernents in each sample.
where:/??:no. measLlremcnts in Slnrplc I
/,:
n(). nrcirstn'cnrurts irr Srrnrplc
I
(llcxl A) (I
lctrl lf t
,S,,, t'ltlio tll' r'tttttttllrtivt' lrt'r1rtr'ttt'\, lo lr
,\
Find D, the largest clilference between {,, and S,. For a one-tailed test, D is calculated as the maximurn difference in the pretlic'ted direction. For example, if our research hypothesis was that the feeding durations in Herd A were significantly longer than in Her6 B, the
t;llto ol ( untrrl;rltrt'ltr'tIrt'rrt \ lo
//
I
0 9l
(r
0.333:0.583
cst st:rlisric-0.-5t{3X I 2X
l
2:
g3.95
( onrl)itrc tlrc t':rlt'rrllrlctl lcsl strrlislic lo thc lirbular.vulue (Table Al l) for the appro_ l)rr,rlt'r;tlttt's ttl tn.n lrntl k.r,r.l ol sil,ltilit.;rrrt.c.'l'lrc lirbtrlis.vltltrc fbrlll: 12,n:12at l' O O\ tr /.) Srrrr't.()ut (.;tlcrrl;rlt.rl r,;rlrrc rrl 51 t11 rs llrrllr,r. lllrrt lltc tlrbrtlltr
''
rrr
vitlue of.
\\r'rr';t't l llrr'//,,rlrrr rlrt'lt't'rlrrr1,,lrrr,rrr.r,, r, Ilr.rtl,A lrrt.r.l l1r;lggt rl,ttt llrcvrtt.c
ll,'r,l ll
COMPLETELY RANDOMIZED I)lrsl(i
L TI:STS NON PARAMETRIC STATISTICA
Smirttov fir'o samPle test./br small Table 14.6. Calcttlations .fbr the Kolmogorlt' 14.5 samples using the data.from Table
Interval (no. minutes)
Rearranged
Cumulative
measurements
frequencies
Herd A
Herd B
Herd A
Herd B
N
Table 14.7. The number o.f duys it look rccd v,arhlers to re.ject model cuckoo eggs by either ejection or desertion
Ratios
(s,,,) Herd
A
(s,,)
Number of nests with rejection
Herd B
Rejected during
By ejection
By desertion
0
0
0.000
0.000
0-4
Day I
l0
0
0.000
0.000
5-9
0
Day 2
4
7
2
0
0.166
0.000
Day
3
2t
t4
J
J
0.250
0.250
Day 4
2t
18
0.411
Day
l0-14
I 1,12
15-19
18
20-24 25-29 30-34
17, I 8,
l9
21,24
3
5
0.250
5
25
20
28,29
J
1
0.250
0.583
Day 6
29
23
3l
J
8
0.250
0.667
Day
3l
24
0.250
0.750
_1
9
31
24
t0
0.333
0.833
0.333
0.916
4
il ll
0.333
0.916
5
1l
0.411
0.916
7
ll
0.583
0.916
9
t2
0.750
1.000
l0 l0
l2 l2
0.833
1.000
('ompare the calculated test statistic to the tabular value (Table Al2) for the appro-
0.833
1.000 1.000
lrriate values of m, n and level of significance. The tabular value for nt:12, n:12 at /':0.05 is 72. Since our calculated value of 83.95 is larger than the tabular value of
1.000
72we reject our
35-39
35
40_/.4
43
4
47
4
43
4549 50-54 s5-59
59
60-64
62,64 67,69
65-69 7
65
t-\
70-74
4
5-19
80-84
8l
ll
t2
0.916
85-89
89
l2
l2
1.000
m:12
Totals:
n--12
7
Total rejected:
Source: From Davies and Brooke ( 1988).
D:0.916-0.333:0.583 Test statistic
F1o
llcrd A and Herd l.(tr,q(
:
0.583
x l2x l2:83.95
of no significant difference in duration of feeding bouts between
B.
wntltlas If the number of measurements in either sample is >25, a different
trrblc and procedures are necessary depending on whether you are conducting a one-
the nlaximlm ub,;olutt,differencc For a two-tailed test, D is calculated as between
S,,,
and
trrilcd or two-tailed test.
As an cxample, Davies and Brooke (1988) studied nest parasitism by cuckoos
S,,.
D:maximum 1S,,,-S,, Test statistic:
I
Dxmxn
rr';ts
lirt ottt ottt' l;ttlt'rl lr'sl
( 'trt'tt ltt.s t'rt trur
tt,;)
I
lrcv nrclrstrrcrl
t hc nunrber
on reed warblers (Acroc'ephalus scirpaceus). As part of the research,
of days it took reed warblers to reject model cuckoo eggs by
I \\'( ) r rrclhorls, c' jcct ion of'the egg
from the nest and desertion of the nest (Table 14.1).
'l lrr rrscrrlch tlucstitln wirs whcther the distribution of times to reject the model
AswestatedatthebeginningofthisexampleoLlr(]uCSti()llwllswlrctltcr.tllct.cislr .r'the rlcc'i. thcsc tw. rrc.trs' rlre'clir'e' significant duration in the t-eeding b.uts tlrr' r-rcrwccrr rrrc ri'ctrirrg trrrrrri.rrs irr .ur H,, is that thcrc is .. sig.iricurt triflL.r.c,cc \'l \O lttttl 'lr) 't'.c llr'gcs I ttltsttlttlt' tlil'li'rcrlt'e is ;rt llrt' ittlt'trltls .l 'l\ twtr lrerrls. tttittttlt's (lltt's;ttltt';ts il
I
)
,'l'l's lry
rlr'se
rli()n wlts sigrtilicirrtlly rlill'crcnt than by eiection.
l'tot't'rltltt':
I
I
)t'lt'r trrrrrr'llrr't rrrnrrl,rlrrr' lrr'r1ttt'rrr'r('\ iul(l t;rlios lilt'clrcIt 1'rct'itltl, tts irt
l;rlrlt' l,l
li
NON PARAM ETRIC STATISTICA L
402
.I.I]S
COM PLETELY RANDOM IZED I)
I.S
1,S
I(
403
;N
than the tabular value (5.99), we lail to rcject our hypothesis that the nests Sntirrutv tt:o santple test.fbr large Table 14.8. Calcttlations .fbr the Kolmogttrov 14.7 ,samples using thc data.fiont Table
are rejected by desertion sooner than by ejection. F-or a
two-tailed test, D is calculated as the maximum absolute differ-
ence betweefl S,,,and S,,. Since we are testing whether there is a significant
Ratios of cumulative frequencies to total
Cumulative no. nests with rejection bY: Rejected Desertion
Ejection
within
Ejection
(S,,,)
ence.
Desertion (S,,)
4
0.322
2 days
l0 l4
0.166
ll
0.451
0.458
3 days
2l
l4
0.677
0.583
4 days
ZI
18
0.611
0.750
5 days
25
20
0.806
0.833
6 days
29
23
0.935
0.958
7 days
3l
24
1.000
r.000
I day
difference in either direction. we will use the maximum absolute differ-
D:maximum 1S,,,-S,,
I
The largest absolute dillerence is at Day I where S,,,:0.322 and S,,:0.166.
D:0.322-
0.
166:0.
166
This calculated D is compared to the D value obtained by entering the observed values of lzl and
ru
in the expression given in Table A l3 at the
appropriate P value. For our example, we I
Total
z
critical value of
24:n
3l:tn
rejected:
will
use the expression in Table
A I 3 lor P:0.05 as follows:
D:
I
.-16 /f '"
*t
)
V \mxn/
:trrlt:-,;)
For a one-tailed test' D Find D, the largest difference between S,,, and 'S,,' rnthe pretlit'ted dtrection, just as is calculatecl as the maximum difference if our hypothesis is that the in the small sample case above. For example, by ejection, the formula for D nests are rejected by desertion Sooner than
:(1.36)v0.0739 :0.370
would be:
:
D:maxirnum (S,,-S,,,) is at 4 days where The largest dill'erence in the predicted direction : 0'677' S,, : 0.750 and S,,,
to reject the mocleleggs by clesertion and by ejection.
Wull Wollowit:
D:0.150-0.671:0'073 rForalargesample,one-tailecltest.achi-squarevalueisnowcalculatedas follows: - tttXn
/\i:4D: tltl
tt
:4(0.07_t r.
:,1(o.oo5)
3l x24
,, (I
ttt'tt sttmple rltns test
l.ike the Marnn Whitney U test and the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test this test is used to tcsl thc 11,, that that there is no significant difference between two indepenclent srrrnplcs. It wrll re'ject F1,, if the two samples difler significantly in either lorm or locatirrrt. lt is appttxiniately J5'Y' as powerful as Student's l-test for sample sizes of rr1'rpnrxinrirtcly 20 (Srnith. 1953). Since it is less powerful than the Mann Whitney L/ It'st lrrrtl llrc Kolrnog()rov Smirnov test, the primary advantage of this test is its simlrlrcity.
n2O
3.53) -0.21
of 0.166 is smaller than 0.370 we fail to reject the H,,that there is no significant difference between the distribution of times Since our calculated D
As rrn ('\iu))l)lc. r.rc I
tll ') lltr' .l ('rlnr1'llr|cllte t'ltlt'rtllrtctl 1'totlle lltlrttllrt r';tlttt'(llrlrlt'A())lirr l) t)() Sttlt t'trlll t 'llt rrl'rlt'rl 1'rrl '/ I r'' rttt;tllt't l:rlrrrllrt r;rlrrt.;rl l' O gr ts:
will tlclcrrnirtc rvlrcthcr the lrypothetical frequency of agonis-
ol sorrllrirtls tlilli'rs si1'nilit';rnllv lrelrvccrr birrls trt fcec'lerType A and at It'r'rlt't ltpt' ll Wt'rrtll rrsrttttt,'llr,tl ttt'r oll1'1 | lltt'lryPollrclictrl tlitllr rl rr ring rrinc Irc lrt'lr:rrior
11'1
rrnr'lltt111 ',;1111lrltttl, l)r'ttrrrl'.,rt
t,trltlr'r'rl,t II1rr'II,rlrlt'
1,1
tr,
COMPLETELY RAN DOM IZED I) l:S
NON PARAMETRIC STATISTICA L T IISTS
Feeder Type B
sample
sample
(;N
dent measurements when using chi-square has bcen emphasized by many authors, including Kramer and Schmidhammer (1992). Whether the chi-square is a good-
Table 14.9. Hypothetical data on the.lrequertcy oJ' agonistic hehavior oJ' songbirds at tv'tt types o'f'feeders Feeder TyPe A
I
ness-of-fit test or a test of independence, the measurements that are summed to provide the observed cell frequencies must be independent in order to have a valid test. The assumption of independence may be violated if an individual contributes more than once to a data set.
16
ll
The application of chi-square with two samples is described below; its use with three, or more, samples will be discussed later in this chapter. It is used with nominal
9
6
data and compares observed frequencies with frequencies that would be expected in
l8
J
6
15
t2
t1
8
13
7
10 15
5
t4
5
Procedure:
and cast them into Rank all the measurements in order of increasing size from which each score comes a single order. Then identify the population and rJetermine the number of runs accordingly' populations: Measurements listed in order and their corresponding
355 667 8 910 tll2 131415161718 BBBBBBB AA B B AAAAAA 1234
Runs:
Number of runs
less
rnore often:
' ' ' '
Parental choice:
Maternal, 35 Paternal,12
Total47 Procedure:
Determine the expected by either assuming: l. a random expected distrib-
('):4
2 obtain the tabular value from Table A8 r. If the observed value is equal or
uniform or random distribution. As an example, Vives (1988) studied parent choice by larval cichlids (Cichlasoma nigro.fasciatum) that were reared under two treatments: l. in the presence of predators of fry; and 2. not in the the presence of predators of fry. Later, the free-swimming lry were placed in an aquarium where they could choose to stay in close proximity to their mother, lather or neither (see the analysis of Vives'2x2 tnatrix of data in the discussion of Fischer's exact test later in this chapter). Vives combined the data from the two treatments and tested whether the larval cichlids chose to stay in proximity to either their maternal or paternal parent significantly a
to
0'05 level' thanthetabular value, then the //o is rejected at the
tl,:number of measurements in the first sample:9 nr:number of measurements in the second sample:9 r(4) is stnttlle r In our example the tabular value:5. Since our calculated sigrtilictrrrt tlilno is there that than the tabular value of 5, we reject the 11,, the trequency of agonistic behaviors betwce tt sottghirtls
ution and randomly assigning each of the 47 measurements to one of the two categories (maternal or paternal); or 2. a uniform expected distributiorr and assigning 50'X, of lhe 47 measurements to each category, as has been clone in Table 14. 10. Calcr"rlate the
I
lor each category (as shown in the Table l4.l l):
(o-Ef ti
ference between
ll
at the two feeder tYPes'
t'lri-stluru'c lcsls whct'c lltc rlcgrcc ol'll'ecrlonr is I (e.9. Parker, 1979). This
is ol'tcrr rccor)mlcnclcd
that Yutas' torrct'tion.frtr t'ontinuitybe used in
t'onsislsol'tr'tlrtt'in1, lltt'nulnt't;tlor lry0 5bclirrcitisscluared,asfollows: C
lt
i'.s
t1
tut
rt'
gt tt t tl t t t"s't - r t l - I i
t
t
t"t
t'
I tt' t )'\(
tt)
t
I
)I
t'
\
rlt'tt't rtttrtt'tVltt'lltt'r lrr"o. ()l lll()l(" Thc chi-st1'llt-c g()(xlrtcss-trl-[il tcst clrn bc rrsctl to I lr.'trrrlt(,l l,tllt t'r'l ltltr llll' lll(l('l)('ll irrtlelrt.ntlr.rrt slrrrrPk.s;rrt.si1,rrilit.:rrrllv tlillt'rr'rrt
l{( )lrst'rvr'tl l'rlrt't
tt'tl)
l r pr't lr't l
{l
',1'
406
COMPLETELY RANDOMIZE D l)lrsI(i
NON PARAM ETR lC STATISTICA L'l'l:S'l-S
Table
gootlnessTable 14.10. Caluilation o/'the Ohi-squurc t'ic'hlids lurval clrcice purental b' ol-fit test on clatu.fitr
(o-
(O)
Maternal
35
23.5
5.63
Paternal
t2
23.5
5.63
Total
47
47
Srturce ;
n.26
'n:
i,
N,- Ntlttlbcl-rll'
re
\rl
\'
N,P2 - -: N.
pl1
'ry'
r I
I
il
vt 0.4-s
0.
l-i.s
-0.63s)+0.635(r -0.6.rs)l
I
22
r
l
3' 103
Sinct' \ l0l litllsorrlsitlctlrclirrrilsol. I l.()(rlo -l.96.weconcludethatthetwo sp()tlsc itr Stttuplc No'
i1
22(.41)+22(.86\':0.635 22+22
0.41-0.86
N,
Il'tcitsrtrclllcllls irt Slrrrtlllc
,ryr/'r Lry,/',
l'
+
/l o.o.rs1r
Pr P: nt*rr t -rrrl
Percetltage of
/\y'r
the percent-
rtt -
where:
:
N tPt+
2l- ,, Pt-l): Lll t tl pl p(l ll Nr -t
Calculate:
P,
44
Nr:22 Pr:0.86
Vt
of l\t'| Percentlges
{L
22
(fed intact. live honeybees) and control toads (t-ed dead honeybees, with stinging apparatus removed) ate droneflies (honeybee mimic) in different proportions (Table
I 1.26 is larger
1/, '
22
l6
N,:22 P-0.41
significa.t dirlerence This test is used to determine whether-there is a samples (or treatments)' ages (proportions) of a response in two
fl
(14'Y,,)
For example, Brower and Brower (1962) determined whether experimental toads
parent, and we conclude by chance' significantly more often than would be expected
-
3
(59"1,)
0.05 level.
of than the tabular value (3.84) we reject the H,, paternal and the maternal a unifbrm distribution of choices between parent that the larval cichlids chose the maternal
7,
28
or 20:
assigned X and Y Rank the measurements for each variable, arbitrarily
Convert rho to the't'statistic:
(Table 14.24).
I
ll N-2 \ I:rho ll /v - I
either measurements within a variable are equal, then been have would that ranks assign to each of them the average of the tau (below)' especially assigned had they not been equal, or use Kendall's
If two or more
V\l-rhc,f
Compare the calsulated 'r'with the tabular value (Table .{5), where: df: N -2.|f the calculated r is larger than the tabular /, at the appropriate
if
there are several ties. pair of measureDetermine the difference between the ranks for each (Table 14'25)' (r/r) clifferenoe ments (r/) and calculate the square of that Determine the total for the d2s (as in Table 14'25)'
P value. then the Huof no correlation is rejected. Although some statisticians have recommended always converting rlio to 'r', Siegel and
Castellan
Calculate SPearman's rho:
(
1988) recommend using Table A
l9 whenever
ly' is less
than 50.
('onvcrt rho to the ':'statistic:
6(>d2l
-1r_1U
where:
,d2
significant correlation between activity and song frequency.
Procedure:
t
: 6:
we reject the 1/,,of no correlation and conclude that there is a statistically
determinewhetheritisastatisticallysignificantcorrelation.
,.:
I
Total
z-r'ho V'N-
lirr
N:no. of paired measurements:7
I
ir two-t:rilecl test. rho is significant at the 0.05 level
rrrrc-lrrilctl lcst, rho is signilicant at the 0.05 level if
6(6) :,_19:l_o.l07:0.89 _ _,_ t- l+t-l-'P,: 336
,\' llrr rrl l)lllt('(l lll('il\lllt'tttt'ttls
il :
is > I .96. For a
is >1.64.
lrrrr I rkc SPr'ru lnlul s rlto. Kcrttltrll's tlrrr rlctcrrnines the tendenc:y of two r.utI r,trlt'ts ol rlrrl;t lo llt':rtntl.rt hr'trtllrll's llru rs l)r('lr'rrctl wltctt thcrc ttrc several hr'tttltrll':
When N[x( x-ll1 nl_
Ta-
433
M:a*d:4*3:7 U: b* t: I -11:2
of two, three, etc')
and divide by 2 to obtain )l lor each set of ties together
R,:
1A Calculate
lltS
Frog B
vocalizes'l
vocalizes?
Yes
Yes
No
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
Yes
No No
No
Yes
Ntr
Ntr
Ntr
Yt's
Yt's
Ncr
researcher's concept
each
of how an association should be measured.
Irrtcrpretution o./'an ttssot'iution A significantly large tau or rho indicates a high rlcgree of association whether positive or negative. There are basically three interprctations lor a high degree of association (i.e. high correlation):
t : r
Frog B
Change in variable
I
causes a change in variable B.
Change in variable B causes a change in variable
Neitlier interpretations I or
2 are
l.
correct, but rather a change in some
other variable (C)causes a change in both A and B.
'l'lrc corrclation coefficients will not tell us which of these interpretations is r orrccl. Wc can only judge (with varying degrees of validityl which is correct ,rt
Frog A
t'orrlirrg to our prrior knowledge of the variables and their relationships.
434
NON PARAM ETRIC STATTSTI('A L'l'l:S'l
RANDOMIZED BLOCK" MA-I('ll I:l)
S
"'
435
blocked
intct three time periods
Sumple,s
A1
ltS
tt britl,v./rutm.fbur habitat,y
Table 14.28. Hvpothetic'al duta on song durutiort
t4.2.lb Three or more matched samples
I'>A I
Mean song duration (s)
Au
Habitat A
r:r -Y::
\.
Habitat B
Habitat C
Habitat D
10.2
6.5
Sunrise
6.7
8.6
Noon
3.4
4.3
6.5
2.5
Sunset
6.3
8.3
9.9
7.1
_1t
Frieclnutis two-w'ul' trnulysis of' turiunce is a nonparametric test used to deterTlie Friedman two-way analysis of variance blockecl measurements or repeated mine if several (three or more) samples with significantly diflerent. It requires ordinal data measures on the same inclivi6gals are compared to the paratnetric ANOVA' (at least) and tests fbr differences in location. (van as powerful for ten samples iI is 12,,/uas powerful fbr three samples and 87"/u Elteren and Noether, 1959)' durations. tiom four habitats; but this As an examPle, we will again sample sc'rng time periocJs: sunrise' noon' and sunset time we will block our samPles into three
Table 14.29. Ruks of'eot'h rov'o.f nl(osltcments in Tuble 14.28 /br t'alculution Friedman's lw'o-\ruy unulltsis of variunc'e (,see tert )
o.f'
Habitat A
Habitat B
Habitat C
Habitat D
Sunrise
2
3
4
I
Noon
2
J
4
I
Sunset
I
-)
4
2
-5
R*:9
t2
Rr:4
R^
R.:
(Table 14.28).
r
\ y::( / . ^ll-- x266 I-gtst:53.20-45:8.10 "t \12(5) I
Procedure: (blocks) separately (Table 14'29)' Rank the measurefilents for each row and provides a more powerThis minimizes the between-row differences
6
fultestofthebetween-column(sample;habitat)differences. Calculateeachoftlreranksums(i.e.columntotals;R.,,Table14.29). Calculate each of the R.r}s'
Rn':25 Rot:81
Rr't:144
df:N- l:2 tabular 1, ,,.1:5.99
R,rr: l6
Sirrce our calculate:i ;,
: 26s1263 te t s) :
Itl
1
s
Therefore, the expected frequency of threats in adult males in this changing pop-
ulation is 75 compared with the observed number of 210. Once again we could make
the same calculations for the adult females and immatures, and then use a chisquare test to determine
when the the conditions are the b. The expected frequency of behavior per class composition changes' is calculated same as in (a) (above), except that the population
if
the differences are significant.
Uniform, class-specific rates of behavior are expressed as a mean number of occurrences per individual per unit time, according to the lormula below:
according to the following formula:
s.
ur:*t*'L: u L;21t1t11;1
Eu
:2,2f fl
E
:the hypothetical
ri
expected frequency of behavior a for
members of class x
where:
E,,:expectedfrequencyofbehaviorrllormembersofclassr
N 2/
fr,,
total number of occurrences of behavior
:
all classes in the samPle total sample time for all individuals of class -t in the entire
a
Lambda,:hypothetical mean participation rate per member of class x
for all individuals of
:
study
2Ff ,, :total
(lambda) for adult males by referring to Table 15.1.
Lambda,:
x)
2,2,tyt,,:totalsampletimeforallindividualsofallclassesfortheentire study (time in sample period oneXnumber of individuals in all
classes)*(timeinsampleperiocltwoXnumberofindividualsin + . . (time in final sample period x number of indiall classes)
' viduals in all classes)
Asanillustration,wewillusethehypotheticaldatairtTablel5.2(seeAltmann determine the expected freand Altmann. lgJJ, for another example).we can of changing cort]position as quency of threats in adult males in this population lollows:
N 2l
:260 r,,, :[(5x l0)+( ''- -50 +
in the entire
study
individuals in .y)+ . . . (time in final sample periodXnumber of
:
r
As an illustration we can obtain the hypothetical mean participation rate
=ltimeinsampleperiodoneXnumberofindividualsinclass 't)+(timeinsampleperiodtwoXnumberofindividualsinclass class
sample time for all individuals of class
l(r5 I
IIx
l5)+14x l2)l
4l{ 261
)',)',/,/il,, .)(rl ll(5' l:'i)l(ll
.)o)
l('l " lt'11
2/ lrr,
Eu
:0.80x263:210
Therelore, the hypothetical expected frequency of threats for adult males during the entire sample period (20 hours, Table 15.2) is 210. The total number of males observed was 37, so that the mean rate per individual was 21 0137
:5.7
threats for the
l0-hour sample. This hypothetical mean rate can then be compared to the observed nrcirn rate in this sample or from observations gathered later from the same or a diflcrcnt population.
Altnrirnn and Altmann (1977) proceed to the consideration of interactions bctwccn intlividuals. They provide procedures lor calculating expected rates of lrclurvior lirr- syrnruetric and asymmetric interactions at constant rates and interaclions wrth lrypotlrcticrrl clrrss-spccific ratcs ol- bchavior. Michener (1980) noted that
llrt'
;rssrrrrrptiorr
pr'rrotl
'
80
100:0.80 :(5x l0)+(1 1 x l5)+(4x12):263
ol
in Allnlrrur lrnrl Allrrr:rnn's (l()77) lirrnrulae that lor any given
lrnrt'rrrrv rrrtlrvrrlrurl lrrs llrr'polt'rrlr;rl lo irrlt'nrct wilh trny olhcr intlividual.
rvlttlt'ltttt'lrlt l't;tl,ittlott',',r('(t(", t',ttol lttlr'lot ln,rn\'\Pt't'tr'su,ltett'ccl'lltittifttlivirl
fr-
l
444
RATES OF BEHAVIOR & ANALYSIS
()l
SI:QLIENCES
ANALYSIS OF SEQUENCES
Tlie following discr-rssion of behavior sequence anarysis is cursory and meant only as an overview' More detailed discussions of the general topic of sequence analysis' including in-clepth descriptions of specific methocls not discussed i, this book' can be lound in: Bakernan and Gottnran
Table 15.2. HypothetiL'al cluta on the nutnber of'tltrcut,s itt u lterd of mule tleer
Adult
Adult
males
females
Immatures l3
4l
60
l8 l3
2
t5
Number of individuals
l5
20
l5
50
Observed number of threats
80
l5
5
100
Sumplapariod2
Suntple periocl
Totals
( l 9g6), castellan (lg7g).Fagen and Young (1978), Gottman and Roy (1990). Haccou anS Triad A------'B---'C Tetrad A--'-+B-----)g---+P
What constitutes a sufficient sumple size lor analyzing Markov chains? Fagen ancl Yor"rng ( I978)provided the following'rule-of-thumb'(based on simulations run by I'agcn) Ibr analyses of first-order Markov chains (Table 15.4): I'or': /l:number of individual behaviors for which sequences will be ntclsurecl
Ilrrrr: l/(r:
i. which it c.. bc sh.w, t'irt Markov chains are sequences or behavi.rs (). ().('lrrr.lltct'ltl s,l'tlt' rlI trrc hchirvit)rs:r'c trcPe rrtrr.'rrr sitions betwccn twa ar 'r.rc
lcvcltll.prtrlllrlrilitvptcltlct.tltlttlelt:tttec.ltlsrr.tlrt'lr'rt.|()l|)l(,lr:tlrtlttVisltsstttttt.tlt.'
insLrllicicnt srrnrplc sizc
5/t': lrortlcr lirrc
tlrc t.rrrrI
0/i'
slrrrrPlc sizr'
strllit'tr'nl \iuttPlr'
s171'
ol
RATES OF BEHAVIOR & ANALYSIS
450
Table 15.4. Orders of Markov
Table 15.5. Transition.frequent'ies urnong bchuviot' puttern categories in c'onlests involving .supplemented and unsupplementetl ol'ner.\ o.f poor-quality \grrit,,r'.t
c'hains
(N:25
Definition
Order of Markov chain 'zeroth-ord er'
ANALYSIS OF SEQUENCES
SIQtJENCE'S
The behavioral events are independent
(A.8,()
The probabilitY of occureuce of
first-order (B'--C)
a
particular
behavior is dePendent on onlY the immediatelY preceding behavior
second-ord er (A-'-
B-
The probabilitY ol occurence of a particular behavior is dePendent on the two immediatelY
C)
preceding behaviors
5.2.1
h
Locate
130
80
0
0
0
73
435
l5
l0
30
0
8
30
-1
0
0
-:r
0
l0 l0
3
R
0
0
5
Signal Threat Contact Retreat
25
J
1
0
5
Signal
13
85
20
0
28
Threat
0
8
l8
0
20
Contact
0
0
0
0
0
Retreat
-l
8
0
0
5
included are not indepenclent
ow' ne r s
Source: From Riechert ( 1984). Copyrighted by Bailliere Tindall.
9
l5
that appear to difll-er greatly between the two samples. For example. Reichert (1984) rrieasured transition frequencies of agonistic behaviors in spiders tliat owned poor-
4
t7
30
5
8
32
Observed occrlrrences
Following behavior B
C
I
6
20
B
9 19
behavior
29
Have we collectccl en.ugh tlata'l Accorcling
d
Row totals
Transition matrix
34
te
should be sufficient. In our example, R:3 and 10 Rr:90; therefbre, since our sample size of 97 is larger than I0R2, it should be sufficient. The matrix of observed frequencies can initially be inspected lor cells which show large frequencies. For example, Lemon and Chatfield (1971) contructed a transition matrix of preceding and lollowing song types for cardinals (Richntondena t'urdinuli,s); their initial inspection of the matrix showed that switches between certain song types occurred very frequently. Two matrices, each from a dillerent value of the independent variable, or treatment, can be initially examined for cells
of each other. Below is a hypothetical transition
A
LI n s Lrp p I e me n
of
matrix lor three behaviors'
Column t otals
Retreat
25
based on observeg.49, we infer that the (iom
ln fact, you can see difl.erent trom random chance. value at P:0'001' lated I is larger than the tabular
Table A9. that our calcu-
of occurrence of dilferent
song types between the hrst and second halves of their sample periods. To overcome the stationarity problem, Oden (1917) developed a rnethod for analyzing behavior seqLrences based on fitting ascending-order nonstationary Markov pl'ocesses to tlre data.
ties
ThetabularValueforffor4dfatP=0.05isg.4g.Sinceourcalculatedvalueof observed are significantly
For a more complete discussion of the analysis of sequences using Markov chains and transition matrices see Gottman and Notarius
Cruner"sPhi .:-^r..-. a matrix of any slze ls association between cells in of measure convenient Another 0 to I and is calcuto a coefficient that varies from cramer,s phi. lt converts the I latecl as follows (2ar,1984)"
/. '(: lY
J
E
o
Eo
t,) 3 .9
!o
o
:
Principal component analysis (also called principal-axis lactor analysis) utilizes an orthogonal rotation of the data (e.g. Huntingford, 1982). Factors analyzed using
rotation account for maximum possible variance among the observed behaviors. Cooley and Lohnes ( 1971 ) provide a FORTRAN program listing lor Varimax rotation. Giles and Huntinglord (1984) use principal components analysis to describe the anti-predator responses of sticklebacks to a model heron or a live pike.
'l
o E 2
p^
) io
n
:
o)
:
o- n
I6.3 GROUPING INDIVIDUALS
n
If it is suspected that groups of individuals
are behaving in a similar way, then Qf actor analysis can be applied to the data in Matrix A. The analysis extracts individ-
Fig.l6.lThreetypicalmatricesinwhichethologicaldatacirnbeorganized:A.Different B' Irrteractic'rns between individuals:
ual-related lactors based on their observed behaviors.
inclividutls: behaviors performecl by rJiflerent
sequences' C. Inter- or intra-individual behavior
In contrast to principal component analysis, powered-vector factor analysis It places maximum emphasis on biological rele-
cxtracts factors without rotation.
the objectives of analysis shoul<J be matched to the characteristics of the particular
vancy, whereas the principal-axis method emphasizes parsimony (i.e. maximum variance accounted for by a f-ew f actors) (Aspey and Blankenship, l91l).
the stuclY (see Gottrnan' 1978)' Since one of the advantages
rrrollusc Aplysia bru.silianu. They recorded the occurrence
they allow of these multivariate methorjs is that
selected graphical displays (Rohlf' 1968)' visualization of the variables through examPles will be Provided'
I(.1.2
GROU PIN G
B
E'HAVIOR
S
'factors'basecl on their into a smaller number of Figure factors A. this analysis extracts the behavior-related ties. when applied to Matrix varialnce' fbr a large percentage of the total 16.1)
which account
Forexarnple,Aspey(|971c)recorcledtheoccurrenceof20ditferentbehaviors
in40individualspiders.HeuseclR-factoranalysistoextracttburbehaviorlor 20 different behaviors) which accounted related factors (groups from the after then descriptively labeled the tttctors 14.3,,/uof the total variance. Aspey labelecl
them. consequently, Factor I was exarnining the behaviors comprising .rur.r/retrcat.' liitcttlr lV .approach/signal', Factor II .vigorous pursuit,, Factor III labelecl 'tl.tr-li.ki,g" uninterpretable' but was then since none
of the
linkcd witlt composite behavio.s was significantly
tcrs in 32 individuals (Matrix Type A). Q-factor analysis extracted three factors (groups of individuals) which accounted lor 80.2"/u of the burrowers. Burrowing characteristics, interpreted relative to ef-ficiency, were examined and the three r11)ups were labeled 'inefhcient burrowers'. 'efficient burrowers'and 'intermediate
(e'g' behaviors in Matrix A' large number of variables R-fnctor analysis organizes a underlying similari-
at first seemed biologically
Aspey and Blankenship (1976) studied burrowing behavior in the marine ol l0 burrowing parame-
..y
.tltct'
sl:tlctl
interactitlns. Altlrotrgh it is ctlttlttltlttlY behavior cluring inter.indivicltral t'e cxislcrr.c ,l' . c()rll'r(),',1()livltti.ttltl that trse .r- r.ctar irn.lysis irssrr.rctl [spe v ltlts lttt' l':tt'tot (Sllrtt'r' l() / \) ttolt' 11t"1 strttc' tttttlcrlvirlu cltclt cxtt'ltc(ctl ritlt.tlIlritlr,tlt'st'rrlrtir(..lillltt'tllt:tttltttlt'lttrtt.tll.rl,.'1,'ltltrrtt.'lt;tsl.Wtt'1rk('lllil
irrrrrowers'. The three-dimensionalrepresentation of these 32 individuals relative to
tlrc three extracted factors illustrates their location into three distinct groups t
lrigure 16.?).
Ilicrarchial cluster analysis groups variables (e.g. individuals or behaviors) on tlre birsis ol' similarities (or differences) among oommon characteristics. Simple ,lrstrrncc-l'unctiorl cluster analysis is sensitive to variability in the data and tends to .,1rlit'thc vuriablcs intc> more groups (Aspey, pers. commun.). Cluster analysis olten rrrrkcs ll'wcr assunrptions than other methods and therefore is easier to understand
gln tt ttl.. 1976; Sparling and Williams, 1978). llrcr':rrehrll cltrstcr anulyses begin with a matrix of similarities (or dissimilaritrr'r). rurtl r;rrirrblcs rlc sc(lucntially.itlincd on the basis of their relative similarities rrrlo tlr't)tlol'llll)ls (liigttrc 16.11. lvlticlt ltt'c silttplc. vistrllly iptcrpretable representaI rrlll\ (rl tlrt' rr'srtlls ol' lltt' ;'to1111i;11,1. l\ttrt1'lttr t't rtl (l()/(r) rrst'tl lrtrrrr,ltr,rlt ltt:lr't ;rtt;tlVsis ott rl;tllt l'l'rrttt lt ttutlt'ix tll' llrr'tttt'r'lt;rttir.'sol'r'otttlttr'titttl lrlr'll;ln(l l)torrtlt'rl;r',lr,rr1'lrllor\\,rrrlrlr",tttllrr)nr)l ,r'.rrr1'11' lrrrl' , lrr..lr'r ,rrr,rlr',r', {\l t \ I ,r', rrr'll ,r'.,1r',r rr',',rtr1'llrr'Porllnt':rltrI ttt'1';11i11'
t N'l or
M U LTI VAR IATE, ANALYSES
470
GROUPING INDIVIDUALS
4tt
+
o o
.$ |{
o
87
}\ 9 6
100
e
o
t.z Indlvidual Doe Deer
o
o
/C
u*, on, v"7
nuo'tt
16.2 Factor loadings for Aplt'siu burrowing behavior projected onto coordinate axes corresponding to the three factors extracted by Q-factor analysis. The origin falls in the center of the tliree-dimensional space. Factor I (large circles) represents 'inelficient burrowers'. Factor II (small stippled circles), 'eflicient burrowers'; and Factor III (triangles), 'intermediate burrowers'(lrom Aspey and Blankenship. 1976\. i
l
attributes
of the method. Their paper lormed the basis lor the discussion which
follows.
In conducting a single-level cluster analysis, let us assume that wc warrt lo tlclcr'mine the relative association anlong intlividr.rals in a hcrrl ol'cight utlLrlt rloc tlccr irr rr wildlif-e prcscrvc. Wc rlbscrvc lhcrrt lirt'ir lrlllrl ol'5(X) ltotrt's lrtrrl rccortl (ltc lrtttorut( o(
tirtrc tlrirt irrtlivitlrurls lrre closer tlurrr li)ur nr('l('ts llrrolrl,ll tr.",rr s;unplirty' ('\'r'rv tttittttlt' l';tt'lt,)((tltl('ll(('(rl :ttt;tssot'i;tltt)lt l\,r'.''tlllr('(l l(l lt'Itt'it'tll ()ll('lllllltll('{)l
B'vuEBGE A lrriti:tltlerrtllirl'r.rtttt lirt.;tss.t'i;rti.rs i, rr lrvp.llrcticirl hcrcl of eighr adult cloc tlt'r'r. ll. I irr;rl rlt.rrrlrol,r;rrrr l,rr llrt.lrssot.ilrlrolll i11 ,\.
I
MU
472
Table
16.2.
GROUPING INDIVIDTJALS
LTIVARIATE ANALYSES
Hypotltetic'nl ussocitttion
claru
deer' Dut? Jbr u hertl o/ aight adult doe
rtre the nLtmher of-hours observetl in association
doe der
in Tuble 16.2
A
H
G
F
t)
C
A
Table 16.3. Similarities.lbr the us.sot'iution,; irt tlta hyytthctit'ul herd of'eight udult
D
C
B
G
H
A
A B
2l
C
18
D
30
E
l6
F
38
22
l1
ll
ll
G H
3t
67
48
4l
47
31
21
17
151
348
Total hours
3l
2l l0
48 22
28
3l
30
25
23
34
191
202
203
208
168
B
53
C
45
D
15
5l
t17
E
44
27
59
74
F
109
6l
30
46
34
G
69
122
87
74
9l
61
H
1l
69
56
50
82
61
76
t29
247
observed
The similarity is then multiplied by 1000 for convenience, so that we can deal with whole numbers.
lor a discussion association (see section 8.3 on sampling assumption). The procedure is as follows:
r
z
of the hazards of this
shown in Table 16'2' This The data are flrst organized into the association vertically and horizontally in table. a matrix of Type B, can be read both
ordertodetermineassociations'However,itisstilldifficulttoseemuch Hierarchialcluster pattern of associations from the data in the table' of the associations' presentation visual analysis will provide a better (associations) similarities of table The next step is to generate a triangular of time' periods total each deer was seen for different
similarity
:
0.053 X I 000
:
53
We then complete the table
of similarities (Table 16.3).
Next. we search the table for the range of similarities. The highest is 129 for H*G, and the lowest is 27 for E+B. The vertical axis of our dendrogram should include this range, so for convenience we will set up a vertical scale of from 0 to 150; note that the scale increases from top to bottom
(Figure 16.3A).
of association observed, which they derived from Dice's coelficient coefficient of assoCole's same as ( 1945). Note that this is essentially the
linking individuals on the basis of similarities, starting with the largest similarity and working to the smallest. Individuals H+G provide our first association. The next similarity l22is between G and B. Tlris nreans we link B up with both G and H at the lZ2level. This is an cxanrple of 'chaining', which is an undesirable characteristic of the mcthod, since it links through intermediates (Jardine and Sibson, 1968)
ciation ( 1949) (section 17'4' lb)'
irncl is clifTicult to interpret visually. That is, the apparent association
among deer. Since
wemustfirstnormalizethedatatoadjustforthosedifferences.
Morganetul.(|g16)describeamethodfornormalizingdatafortime
SimilaritY:;:., where:
Xy:totaltime when individuals Xand X:total time Xwas observed )/:total time Ywas observecl
Ywere observctl togctlicr
/l irl'l'rrblc l(r'l Thcrclilrc. thc sirlril:rrity lirr intlivitltrirls "l rttltl Sirrrilrrritv
,l r ll r()l r .)0.) o 0s l
We then begin
hctwcur [] and H in Figure 16.3A is really due to B's association with G. M orurr n c t ul. ( I 976) d iscuss the problems of 'chaining' in more detail. 'l'lrc next irssociution is D*C at thc I l7 level of similarity. We then t'orrlirtrrc thc sirrrillritics ('lhblc 16..1) trntil all the inclividuals have been
l lris lr;rppt'rrs tvltr'rr rt'r'nurkc lltc lrss
the tabular value then the sample directions
are not randomly distributed but are significantly clustered.
s If
0.24
Expected
Observed (O- E)llE
A
(50,'1,) 21.5
t4
2.61
C
(50"1,)
29
2.6t
your data are angular degrees from magnetic north (or time) then also
21.5
43
^):5.22
conduct a Ztest (below) fbr confirmation of significant clustering.
As an example, Sordahl (1986) tested whether the distraction displays
of
14
avocets and 5 stilts were directional, especially whether they would lead an observer
away from the nest. He stood
l0
20 m
Plays in the other two quadrants would be considered misleading,
nrost researchers.
from the nests and recclrded the position of
colored-banded, displaying birds in a circle with him at the center. F-or analysis, he divided the circle into lour equalquadrants with one of them subtended by an angle of 45o on either side of a line from him to the nest (Figure 17.3). He observed 8l clis-
rr
distribution) display distributions
IicirnI Iy
Il you use r.lnly the numbcr ol clisplays towru'tl
(tlrrirtlrirrrl A)irtttl rtwiry llont lltt'
('). tltcchi-srltrirrc lcsl sltorvs llrrl lltost'tlisplrrvs u.'r'tr'rlisltilrtttt'tl si1' trilierrrrllv rlillr'rt'ttl llt;ttt t:tnrloltt (st't' l';rlrlt' I / \) llo\\r'\r't ,lt"t,'.1';ltrlttt;' lltr tltr
rlilll'l'cnt.
I lris rr
lcsl ts trsctl lo 1lc1gl'111iltc whetlrer the sample of directions (or tirne) diflers sigilit'rrrr l lv ll ottt t'rttttlolt.t. -f hc tlatu nrust be in angular degrees from north (0.).
plays were in the quadrant away from the nest. the direction of'all dislnrciiorr tlisplays was not significantly different from random.
by
t"t.t.2 Raylcigh test
is given in Table 17.2.
The calculated clii-square value of 5.94 is smaller than the tabulitr vulLtc (3 dl. alpha level:0.05) of 7.81. This means that although most of the clistrlctiott dis-
if not invalid,
Tlre calculated chi-square ol 5.22is larger than the tabular value of 3.g4 (l di lPIla level :0'05 ); therefbre, the number of clisplays in these two quaclrants was sig-
rr i
traction displays from the l9 birds. The observed and expectecl (based ort a uniflorn.t
ncst (cluaclrant
l
Ey:
+ Compare the calculated If
3.78
18
Table 11.3. The c,hi-square gooclness-o/-fit tubte using only the duta.fiont Sec,tor,; A ( tot;-arcl the nest ) ancl C
Calculate the Chi-square value (see Table 17.1):
,.r:r(oALE
29
-
The expected number/sector must >4.
:
489
Table 17.2. The c,hi-scluare gorrlnt,.s,.s-o/-lit table /or
.1. Layout./br c'hi-squure goodness-o/-fit tesl of' data.fiom a c'ircular
Sector
ot, t)llrrr(,.t.tONS
Ittol'1'111111''
t
( 'trlt'rrl;rl('llt(.\lltn ,'\
\
'I t:ur )',rrrr.
1r1. 11.,,.,1
ol
llrr, \///r,\ iuttl r.r,r///r,r ttl.tltc slrtttltlc tlil.cctiOrts. T0ble
l,,t ,.tllrr.t ,rtr1,rtl,1
rlt.1,1t.r.r
pl
li1lr.,.
I
TESTS FOR RANDOMNESS
CIRCU LAR STATISTICS
490
northeast o/' c'ampus while blindfolcled
Alpha level N
0.0s
0.01
0.001
30
2.91
4.50
6.62
50
2.98
4.54
6.14
100
2.99
4.57
6.82
200
2.99
4.59
6.81
500 (or larger)
2.99
4.60
6.89
r
c2)
Calculate the test statistic Z'
Z:RzlN +
Directions pointed while blindfolded (angular degrees from magnetic north)
R'
R:V(sr+
where: N:number of sample directions
Clompare the calculated
Zto
the tabular Z"inTable
17
'4'
+0.2156
-0.9613
88
+0.9994
+0.0349
t44
+ t).5875
-0.8090
328
+0.8480
290
-0.5299 -0.9391
-0.4067
tt4
+0.9135
180
0.0000
128
+0.7880
t52
+0.4695
108
+0.9511
178
+0.0349
-0.469s Totals:
R:V(s2+ c\:t Z
Asanexample'Icollecteddataonthedirectionalorientationabilitiesofl3stu-
miles northeast of the
+0.0698
+0.9976
When the sample directiops are nonrancantly clustered about the mean directiorr' mean direction and cletermine whether dom, you can go on to calculate the sample direction (e'g'home)' it is significantly rJifferent from the predicted
10
Cosine
86
208
cues when they were deprived of normal visual dents in my ethological methods class to a van in a postion). They were blindfolded ancl transported
Sine
164
the directions are nonIf the calculated Z is greater than the tabular Zu,then directions appear to be unimodal' then random. If the distribution of the sample proves that the sample directions are signifisignificance in the Rayleigh test also
(landmarks and sun secludecl location opf .o*i,rutely
491
Table 17.5. Direc'tions l3 blind/bldecl stutlcnl,s poitttctl to intlit'ate the direction to the Colorudo State Univer,rity camplts a.f'ter huvittg bacn driyen approximately l0 niles
Table 17.4. Critical values o.f Zu
z Calculate
Ol l)llLl:("1'IONS
:
R2 I
N
:
S: +4.078
(16.630+3 1.047):\/ 4i .677
47 .67 7 I 13
:
:
+0.3420
-
L0000
-0.6157 -0.8829 -0.3090 -0.9994 -0.8829
C: -
5.512
6.904
3.661
Since the calculated Z of 3.661 is larger than 2,,,,(2.97) we reject the I/n that the
directions pointed by the students are random.
Colora{o State University
and to provicle data lor the students to analyze campus. The exercise was conducted use to ability their the methods' It was not a valid test of
to allow them to critique
Able' field (see Baker, 1980' 1987; Gould ancl other cues, such as the earth's magnetic stttclctlts were not coverecl' and some tll'tlic 1981); for example, the van windows rt cttc ttr its srlll afterngotr the the heat from sitting next to the windows reported using still w'ile v.rr trrc inclivicrually red fr..r westerly direction. The stude'ts were the
'l hcv
the canrpus fi-..r thcir r,r'sc.l blindtblded anci askecl the direction t. ''rrsiti,' (). i'r(l tlle. rir'st wrrrr rrrc hrirrtrirltl still were then askecr t. p.i.t t.war.trs c.,rrr:i. rlrr.strrrrt'rrrs p.irrt'rr lvrrirc lrrirrtil'rrrlt'rl. with trrc bli.tilirrtr r.e.r.vctr..r'rrc trir.ceti.rs
rlt'1't('("':ll('l'l\t'll lll l'rlrlt'l / \ Ittttllltt.siltt.:l:tntlt'ositlt'srrl llto\('illll'tll;lt
t7.1.3 V test Whctr tlte rc is art expected direction, the Ztest is prelerable to the Rayleigh test since
it
is rttorc powcr'f'ul. For example, when homing pigeons are clock-shifted 6 hours
lrrtc. llteir crpcctcrl tlisappcirrance direction upon release is 90" clockwise lrom the
'l'lrc I 'tcst lvill lcrrrl to sigtrilicance only if there is sufficient clustering lrl,,tttttl lltr' ptr'rlir'lr'tl tlitt't'ltott Ilr)\\'('vcr'. tlrc l'1cst shoulcl t>nly be ttsed to test Ior r;rtttl.rnrnt'ss. ll rl,'t's rrol lt",l rrlrt'llrcr llrt's;rrrrplt'rrrclrrrtlircctirlrt rlevitttcssignifi(:tttll\ ltotttlltr'Ptr',1t,lt',1rlttr'rltrrtt lltt'rlttttrlt'ttr't'tttlt'trltlsltottltlltettsctllirrlltlrt 1' )l l)llll)(r"t'(ll,rl"t ltt'lt'l lt)lil,('{",(r ltr,tt | Itorrrc tlirect rort.
492
SAMPLE MEAN AND SPE('ll
CIRCI]LAR STATISTICS
We determine the sinc arrd cosinc
Procedure:
I
R
cos(O-
r:
0,,)
of the mean angle:ffi/r': *0.3l4l5.3l l: +0.5912 Cosine of the mean angle:c-os lr: -0.429rc.531 l: -0.8017
R was calculated previously (see Rayleigh Test)
the test statistic
Using Table A23 we find that the angle whose sine approximates +0.5912 and whose cosine approximates -0.8077 is 143.5". This concurs with our
rz.
previous calculation.
Calculate test statistic lr.
':J(i)(')
@:sample mean angle: 143.5"
where: N:number of sample directions
0,,: predicted direction (campus): I 96'
Compare the calculated a to the tabular value lr,, (Table A24). tf the calcuand laled uzu,, then the sample directions are not randomly distributed
Vt:
R cos(@-
are clustered significantly about the expected direction.
143.5"- I 96o:
Irtrr t ": J(;)(
(unt of tirle they spent in each part
of the moving troop (front,
side, rear, middle or clusters; see Collins, 1984, for a
diagram). When associations between individuals are based on distances, the researcher must decide on a criterion distance between individuals in which the probability of their interacting greatly increases and beyond which they commonly approach one
another in order to re-establish the association; this decision is based on the researcher's experience. For example, Grant (1913) used as his 'measure of association'between individual grey kangaroos (Mocropu,s gigunteus) the number of times each animal occurred within 120 cm of another at set l5-minute intervals (instantaneous samples). With this procedure, the accuracy of observers in determining the distances between individual animals can be a problem, especially in field studies involving distant observations. For example, Morton (1993) measured the accuracy of observers in determining the locations of individual elk in small herds. The distance discrepancy between observed animal locations (from observer diagrams) and actual animal locations (from aerial photographs taken simultaneously with ground observations) averaged 5.6 body lengths. Sullivan and Morton
(
1994) measured the
ability of ground observers to judge inter-animal distances in groups of life-size 11.4.1
Animal-animal spatial relationships
Animal-animalspatialrelationshipscaninvolveintra-orinterspecificassociationsthe the research question can require between individuals or groups. Additionally,
or between known individuals over time' measurement of distanoes or associations simultanesampled being of the group spatial relationships among all members
artifical deer. They took photographs from different observer viewing angles of dif'f-erent herd sizes with the animals oriented in different directions (facing away or perpendicular to the line of sight); then observers diagrammed the animals' locations from the photographs. Larger herds, lower viewing angles, and perpendicular orientation of the animals produced greater discrepancies between observer perccived and actual animal locations.
ously.
your ability to accurately determine their The observability of the animals aud to each other will influence the type positions, either in the environment or relative
ofsamplingmethodemployed.Themethodsbasicallyinvolvetwodifl-erentproce-two recording when (frequency and/or duratiou) >- Boboon trocK
It
lir| gRrttP S lrrltl Fig. 17.6 Baboon group home ranges and core areas. A. Tcn-clay ritngcs llrc itltlicittcs linc sltitrp Tltc Rcscrvc. group Cape C. 2l day 11lges for ('lrrrtl S llv occttllictl At'ctts I|. ntttgc. hotnc gr()up's 9lclch limit lpproximatc irr o'nt'tlltP ol grogps ttrtrl si.;ullrclp lirrrit ol'N grrrtrp's lirtll'('. rrrrlitlrltitl':ttttottttl llrlttlc
t'ltttg.cs
lttttl loclttiolt ol'tolt'lttt':ts
(ltotlt I)t'\'ott
of
observations neces-
to rcitch thc l'Z,level varied with the species and the stage of the nesting cycle. Srurtlcrson ( 19(r(r) suggested that live-trapping mammals would probably provide lnstrllicicnt tlitlit to apply the observation-area curve method, but that radiotelemesru'y
'rrlrl Il;rll
lt)('r)
t
Ptobirhlv worrltl.
'llrc'u'rrlitlitv ol'llrc cirlcrrllrlctl hornc riurgc. or tcrritory. will depend on the techttirlttr'r'lttltlovr'tl. Wltr'tt rlitr't'l olrsr'ttlrlions rrr-c rrrirrlc. lhc irnimal's location can be r'onsirlt'tr'rl ;tlo111';ur ('\:r('nlltllY conlnrrrorrs rlislrilrrrliolr,'l'ltlrt is. it rnity bc corttirtuotl:.lV oltst'tvr'tl llttottl'ltr)ttl. itn(l lounrl,rl ,rtt\ l)('lll \\'tllrrrr. ils lt'ttr. ltolttC r':trtgC.'l-ltis ',,tlttltltlt;, 1,,,',lt,rrlr,ur ltt't otr',trlt'tr'rl trr',l,rnl,ltrr)ll',',,nrrIlnt1'ol ;t lirt':tl;tttinlrl ttsittlt,
\('t\ ',ttt,tll',,rttt1rlr'tttlr'tt,tl', \\,',',1( r ( l')("
I tt'rrr,' I i /)',,rrrrPl1'11 rrstrrl,lot'lrl
Pltrr.
CI RCTILAR STATISTICS
514
Wood
SPATIAL PATTERNS
all-occurrences of visits to dilferent cluarlrirts. Son-rc indirect measures, ssclr ir\ tracking in snow, can also provide continlroLls clata, ancl biotelemetry can plrvirlr.
Pewee
nearly continuous data by rapidly scan sanrpling inc'lividual locations (sec ('lrrrptcr
- 1--o/ /o
/o
e).
Other methods, such as the commonly
Llsed capture-recapture mctlrorl l.r discontinuous spatial distribution of' l()cirlr()n:. restricted to trap sites. Thereflore, the validity of this method is affectctl h.y trrlr spacing (Stickel, 1954), as well as trapping interval (time of trap scl rp tr;r;, check), sample size (number of trapping intervals) and the responses ol'irrrlrrrrl Llal animals to the traps (Balph, 1968; see below). This sampling nrctlrprl t.i1r lrr. considered a one zero sample (Chapter 8) for each trap site. That is. lirr t.;rt.lr trap site each individual is either captured, or not captured, rmtttiotr abottt it bcliitviol'lllsYsl('lrr (('I' rcprocluctivc bclurvior) in an attclrpt to undcrstettttl bcttct'tllcir ciltlscli lttttl lttttt liprrs lrrrtl illustratc thc irrtcrrclltionships bctwectt bcltitvitlrs. Motlcls ttt.e l't'ltt't;tll\ ltypollrcticll irnrl tcutporlry. bcitrg chitngetl as Ilcw t'csttlts c()lllc lirrtlr. l'itt t'rrttttPlt', rrrl('l li:rc;crrtls(l()76)1tnr1'rosctlllttotlcl(l;igtrrclti.l2)tocxplititltltcoccttt't't'll('t'()l tttotlt'l l'rtrrkt' lltt' rrrPti'u'r. bclurvior tlrrrirrg lltc irrcrrbirlion itt lrcrrirtg gtrlls. llitcrctttls rrrlp'svslr.rrrs'.'srrlrsyslclts'. ittttl 'ttcls'wlrrt'lr ltt'tt'l;ttt's to'l irlllctgctr's ( I()\0)t':ttltt't
r.prrt'r'l)lrr;rl tprrtlr.l rll' llrt. lrir.rlrrt'lrit'ltl ()t,'lltttzlrltott
ol ltclltvittl (st't' :tlso l)lttt'kttls.
l()l.i\) lrrrrtirrl'lr)t(l ( l()l.i,l) rllrr'.lt.tlt". :ttttl tlist ltss('s s('\('t:tl ;trltlt l()/(rtr i1t(l Iro11;11 torrtr'Plrlrl rrrorlr'l: r,l lttollt;tllr,tt l\1, l,rrl,rtr,l (l')/l). l\lr Ilrtllrrr.l illl(l llpttrl6rr(l()lil),rrr.l lr,,rlr'.,(l()li(l)tolll,lltt,trlrltll,rll,llr'\,lllr1tlr"',rl '.\'-lt'ttt''ltt,,,lt'l', ( ilrs:..
Sotttr'llltt,',,,,11t,
I
1rrrnr('lrl',.ltttl
ttt,', lt,ttrt.tll.
r,lll
lrt
tt',,'.1 'l',,lll,llo1'lt',
u1
VISUAL REPR ESENTATI()N
INTERPRETATION AND PRESENTNTION OF RESULTS
S
WAVE AND WHIP
STATI C
DISPLAY lrig. I ti.l
I
Kinematic graph of the spermatophore-transler phase of the smooth newt's of arrows is proportional to the lrequency of transition. Arrows pointing to lhe left are returning to retreat display; arrows pointing outwards erre leaving sexual behavior, fbr example, to breathe. Br:brake; C:creep, C.O. :,creep-on; P.B. : push-back, e :quiver; R. D. - retreat display; S - spermatophore depositi on ; T.T. : touch -tail ( from sexual behavior sequence (Figure 18.10). Width
Halliday, 1975).
RETREAT D IS
PLAY rttctaphors to help visualize, and often better understand, behavioral processes. For ('xample, Lorenz's original (1950), and revised (1981), psycho-hydraulic model ol' rrrotivation has appeared to some to be analogous to a flush toilet (e.g. Goodenough
t't u\.,1993); however, it served as the basis for much early theorizing about innate ;rrtitrral behavior. For example, discussing Lorenz's early models, Thorpe (1919)
te EE SPERMATOPHORE TRAN
SF
ER
+
+\rFF ie'6
CREEP & FOLLOW
s
Some of his models were obviously analogous only - but the very of 'analogy'is its imperfections which challenges rethinking. One did not suppose them to be'true'but they were valuable in being
OUIVER
essence TOUCH TAIL
highly suggestive. DEPOSITION
Fig.
18.10
Kinematic graph of the sexual behavior sccprcncc is in black (from Halliday. I97-5;.
It:t'u'c I
BRAKE &
TOUCH TAIL
r()nr iln cvoltrtionat'y perspcctive (e.g. Maynard Smith, 1982). I
PUSH BACK
ol'thc snrootlt trcwt. lhc
IThorpe, I979:I03J
Irkcwisc. giultc theory models, such as Prisoner's Dilernma (e.g. Axelrod, 1984), sct'vetl its a usclirl rnetaphors (Sigmund. 1993) for envisioning animal conflict
+\ETrTi3il,
fl q
tlrtecl:
trurlc
rt
lt't ltct' 1'rct's1-rccl
ivc is provirlccl by generalized conceptual models which help the
,'tltologisl vistutlizc lltc cottrplcx ol'vuriahlcs wlrich impinge on behavior (Chapter ') Sottlc tttotlcls rtitl rcsclrrclrcrs rrr rccogrrizing how thcir rcscarch fits within the'big Irtt ltttr"rttttlltssisls itr itlcrttil'yirrg, irtr1rorllrrrI vtrritrblcs Io invcsligtrtc in lirture str-rrlies. \ tt'tV l'.t'ttr't;tl tttotlt'l ol' tltis lyllt'rr';rs tlt'st'rrllt'tl ;urtl tlist'rrssctl rrr tlcllril by ('rook cl ,tl (lt)l(t) I lrc lrr,rlrrl irst. ;rllorvt.rl lr1, llrt.rr rrrorlt.l rs illrrslrlrlt'tl rrr I,igrrr.c Ili.lj.
( oll':trt(l()/li )l)l()\'t(l(':.;r 1'oorl()\'('rirllrlrrrrri:.rortol llrt'r,,lr'ol
rrrlrlt.llrr,,, ll(.tltrll69-
r{,tll('\(';il( lt ( rrg1t1'1rl tt,tlttt,,,l,'l',,,11,'nlr',t,11,,7,1 ,',lt, lrtt'nrttrlr'/r rrlttrlt,tt,.,.tlrt(.,.,1(.(ltttttt;tlltr.
VISUAL REPRESENTATIoN
INTERPRE,TATION AND PRESENTNTION OF RESULTS
S
I
External Environmental Variables
m
(EEV)
i I I
Fig. 18.13
i
A conceptual model showing how externalenvironmental variables (EEV) are expected to interact with species parameters (SP, for example, morphological and physiological characteristics) to determine social structure (measured as the
I I
principal social system variables (PSSV) and social dynamics (changes in PSSV over time)). The dotted arrow takes note of the lact that EEVs also affect SP. but
a
c t
on a slower (evolutionary) time scale than the effects on PSSVs, which may of an individual through learning (from Crook et al..
o
I
change within the lifespan 1916).
matical terms to enable tests of their validity; that is, they should result in lalsifiable lrypotheses (e.g. Drickamer and Vessey, 1982). Predictive models are built from data sets. Generally, the larger and more accurate the data set, the more accurate the
rnodel; however, Gauoh (1993) has argued that a model can be more accurate than the tlata used to build it since the model amplifles hiddern patterns and discards noise.
Predictive models can be rather general, such as Regelmann's (1984) model for Itow competing individuals should distribute themselves between food resource pittches, or they can be more specific such as Altmann's (1980) mathematical model cxpressing the relationship
of
a baboon mother's feeding time requirement to her inlitnt's age. Tliesc models are beyond the scope of this book, but good discussions crtrt be lirLrncl in Colgan (1978), Hazlett and Bach (1977) and Mangel and Clark
(letili). corollirry on lhc input lor incubation. This input is fed llrrottlllt rt tutil (/). rtcccssrrly to crplain tlrc inhibition o['settling and building rvlrt'rt li'erlllttk ttutlcltcs c\l)c('tiur('v. Ilrc cllt'ct ol'll'ctlhlck tliscrcpancy on Iy' (:rrrtl /). /:,:rrtrl /'. trtn lrt'tr';rrl llorrr llrc iur()\\'s.'l lrc rrlrin syslcnls rnutually \tllrPll'1''()ll(';lll()llrt't. /'rrlltrlttl'ltl lrr01q111:tstttlt'tIrrPliVr'llr'ltlt'"'i0ttt tlurlttglt rlt.'tttlrtlrtll()r(|l \';tttrl / /'trrtr lrr':rtlrr'rrlr'rlrlrrt'rllYlr\t'rlt'ltrlrlslirrrttll likcrltrsl l.llll ol lr,ll,t"llr"' / (.ttl.tl',,,1,1'',lttttttl,tlt'rl lrr rlr',ltttl',ur,t", ollrt'l tlr;rtt rlr.litit.ttl l,',',11,,r, l. lr,'ur llr,', lrrlr lr (lrntrr ll,t, r, n,l,, l,lit') lrrr clle'r'cncc copy or
Fig. 18.12 Model for the explanation
of the occurrence ol'intcrruptivc hclutviot'tltu'irtg tltc
incubation ol a herring gull. The {ixccl actiort pitttcrns ut'c itt tltc t'igltt coltrltttt and superimposed control systcnrs tll'first lrrrtl sccorttl otrlct'ltt'c t'elltesr'ttlerl icll ol thcnr (IV=inctrbtrtiort syslctu. /i cscrrllt'\y\l('nr. /' ptt't'ttttt1, s\sl('ltt I I ltt' l;rr1't' vcrticttl ilt't()ws trl)t'esct)l olit'nl;tlion t otnltottr'ttl', tr tllt tr'1':tttl lo lltr' ttr'sl Itrt'rrlrlrlirrl,is llrt't'orrsrlrrrrlrlorv rrr'l I t't'rll,,r, l. ',lrnrrtl,rltotl ltr,ttt llt,'t lttlt lt.,tllt't lrt'ttt1'ptor't'rsr'tl rrt //'. llorrs l() it uilll (( / I ult,'t,'tl t. r,rtlrl),ttt'rl trtllt ('\lx'( l,tlt( \.
tii
INTERPRETATION AND PRESENTATI0N OF RESULTS
VISUAL REPRESENTATION
S
RANK
18.2.s Other illustrations
1970 The type of visual representation employed and its value in interpreting results are limited only by the ingenuity of the researcher. Simplicity in illustrations is generally
1971
420
022
a virtue worth pursuing. For example, Bercovitch (1988) used a pie-chart to illus-
06o
trate the percentages of the different types of consort change-overs (e.g. feed, fight) in adult male baboons. Patterson (1917) used a simple diagram which clearly
o2o 530
demonstrates the rank-order changes of male shelducks (Tadornu tadorna) over a
two-year observation period (Figure 18.14). The positional and relative extent of the changes in rank order are obvious and conducive to further interpretation. Hutt and Hutt (1910) followed up on a suggestion by Altmann (1965) and
012
of a phase structure grammar model to the analysis of
013
described the application
behavioral sequences. The model was first developed by Chomsky (1957) for the study of psycholinguistics. The model consists of the sequential partitioning of a sentence into its constituent parts based on its explicit meaning. The result is a tree
-
diagram of sequentially smaller clusters of words that together carry the meaning of the sentence. This hierarchical model, discussed by R.Dawkins (1976a) and
of syntax in the repro-
023
ductive behavior of the pigeon (Figure 18.15). The 'Catch-22' ol this method lor the ethologist is that to apply the model to gain understanding of the message in communication, we must first understand the
432
message.
This difficulty is illustrated by applying the analysis to the sentence'We fed her dog bones', which can have two meanings; hence it can be diagrammed in two ways (Figure 18. 16).
530 383 013
/ /.. ,//
015
010
572 613 380 019
321 330 007
624 017
523 /
this level of resolution in analyzing sequences of animal behavior'/ Altmann ( 1965a)
_y
with sufficient experience it can be done.
If one's
'lit stttttttt:tt'ize, rrll ol' tltr'sr' lt't'ltttirlttr.'s ol \ lsurrl tt'1111",1'111,r1ro11 (lrrrrl ollrt'rs rrol tlist'ttsst'tl)t':ttt;ttrltttlltt'tttlt'tPtt'l;rll()n ()l r(",ull'. llr,'\ ',lr,,rrl,ll,, , \,unnl('(ltrol orrl\
-
017
As Dale (1976) states, the ambiguity does not arise from a difference in words or
goal is to draw up an exclusive and exhaustive classification of the animals' repertoire of socially significant behaviour patterns, tl-ren these units of behaviour are not arbitrarily chosen. On the contrary,thcy can be empirically determined. One divides up the continuum ol'actiorr wherever the animals do. If the resulting recombination units lu'c themselves communicative, that is, if they affect the behaviour ol'othcr' members of the social group, then they arc social messagcs. J'htrs. tlrc splitting and lumping that onc clocs is. itlcrrlly. ir rcllcctiorr ol'thc splitting und lumping tlrirt lhc rrnirrrrls tlo f lltttrrttttt. l()lt:,tt .lt).' f
440
015
in their ordet but rather from a dillerence in their constituent structure. Do we have suggests that
563
/
005 531
Westman (L917), was used by Marshall (1965) in his study
546
408
395 053
il1
518
023
425
399 018 057
I rr ll'i l.l
('lr;rrry'r'r rrr r;rrrk ()t(l('t
ol slrt.lrlrrr k., lrt.lrr(.(.n \(.:rrs I lrt.lil,rrlt.s:rr.e llrc scrill ttt,ttkcrl trr,rl,",,rrr,rrr1,r'tl rrr r.rrrl. ortlr.t llttrls rrltitlt wet.c t,tttk,rl ttt lrollr \(',u.,,1t(.lotn(.(l lr\,uto\\, Ilr,.lrrl,lrr,r r.rrrl.rrr1,lrrrrls ttr l()/0 lr'ttrlr'rl lo tr'1',,,,,, lrrl,lr rrr l,l/l (lr,,rrtr I .,rrltil.utr,\\ ,) 1,s11 llr,.trrrtlrllt.lrrtrl., trr l()/0 tttttttlrt'ts
ol
11111;1t,ltt,tl
(lltttttr, t',,'lt,l.ilrrr\\',1 lr'ttrlr,l l,r 1,,,, r,rtrl r, l.rlrr, 1,,11r,,,, ,lllil\\',)(lt"lll
l'.tllr
t ,iltt
l'r'r)
1,,11 1,,
lrr/{l(rl,r.,lrr.rl
REVISING AND RESTARTIN(; ll\ l'()llll:SlrS
INTERPRETATION AND PRESENTNTION OF RESULTS
model'l Does your interpretation help you to tlcvclopr nr:w models and generate new hypotheses? At this point it is again importunt to consider what other research has
SSSeq
,/l\ /t\ /\ t
\
Prep
lnt
w
Wa
ASS
/\
Dr
lnt
D
Bi
lnt
Bw
Aoo Bw
/
Pre
/
M
\
shown.
Co
\
Co
I8.3 COMPARTSONS WITH PREVIOUS RESULTS How do your results compare with those of other researchers? Discuss your results with the same researchers you consulted before beginning your study (Chapter 4). Even though you reviewed the literature belbre beginning your study, it is wise to again search for relevant material in the light of your results. You may want to know rnore about similar behavior in other species or different behavior in the same
Wa
,/\ gE_iw
I
A
species. You may discover that your results have a bearing on a general concept or current theoretical issues. The importance of results are often unforeseen when a
study begins, but become apparent as the study proceeds and finally come to light as
\
the results are carefully interpreted.
qi
P.
of generative grammar in its recursive form to reproductive behavior of the male pigeon. SBSeq:sexual behavior sequence; Prep : preparatory behavior, Con : consummatory behavior; Int : introduce; Wa:warm up; Agg:aggressive behavior; Bw: bowing; Dr:driving; A:attacking; D:displacement preening; Bi:billing; M:mounting: Co:copulation. The underlining represents the final behavior that results from the previous steps. The dots indicate where the pigeon can backtrack in the
Fig. 18.15 Tree diagram showing application
sequence
(from Hutt and Hutt, 1970).
We
fod hor dog
-.Aed
know where you began, and you think you know what your results mean. Even lhough your results were seemingly conclusive, your study could have been better. I{e-evaluate the economics, efficiency and validity of your methods. Did you
plctccl.
fed her dog bones fod
hor
dog boner
-4r
her dog
-,A't.
her
Yrru've now reached the point where you can re-evaluate the entire study. You
sclect the proper species, study area, behavioral units, data-collection method, rrnalytical tests, etc.'J Re-evaluate your study at each phase of the ethological lpproach (Chapter l). You should improve your methods with each study, but tltis can only come through a critical re-evaluation of each study as it is com-
We fed her dog bones
fed her dog bones
I8.4 RE-EVALUATION
dog
III
dog
Fig. 18.16 Two-phase-structure grammar models of a single sentence to illustratc thc different meanings (adapted from Dale, 1976).
to understand better the particular bchavior stutliccl. but itlso to pttt it irt pcrspcctive relative to the various lcvcls ot'bchuvior (('ht1'rte r' | ).
Art'lltt'
5 REVISING AND RESTATING HYPOTHESES
bones
t'csttlls sirrrillrl'lrt lltost'
secl l(lr tltltcr hchitviors. itrrrl spccics illr(l ttnrlt't olltt't t'ttt it()lllllr'lllill t'olltlilitrtts'.' Arc tlre l'csrtltst'otlsistt'lll witlr;r ('()ll('('l)lttltlttt"rlt'l .'t r'tlrt;ll'lt'l.t ttrt'lll il l)lt'rltt ltr''t"
\irrr rtt:ry wunt to rcvisc or restatc your hypotheses whether your results were posilivc or ncglrtivc. Tcsting rcvisccl hypotheses can help reinlorce positive results and rsolrrlc llrc sotrrcc ol'ncgutivc resulls. Ytru might choose to isolate additional vari,rlrles ol tcsl tlrc cxlcrrurl virlirlity ol'yotrr rcsults on othcr spccies. Wlrt'tlteryott terisr'.r'eslltlc.()t !('neI'lltcItcwltypotltc:.ics.y()r.rilrcIl()wbackatthe l,r'l'itutinl'ol lltt'r'lltolo1,i1';11:tllPto;rclt t'vt'lr'(('lr;tPlt't I). r't'rrtly lo bcgitt irgitin. This Ittnt'yt)lt ;tI('ilt()l('('\l)('tl('n(('(1.;rttrl. lr()Pt'lrrlly. trtsr'r Sltr';rklttr ,'' ,rn r'\olttttottltt\ lrtttl,rl,t:.1 . I ( ) Wtl'-,rtt ollr'rr'tl tlrt' lirlltrtvittB ttr',t1'lrl
INTERPRETATION AND PRESENI'ATION OF RE,SULTS
Love the animals for themselves first, thell strain for general explanations, and, with good fortune, discoveries will follow.
APPENDIX A
lf
don't, the love and the pleasure will have been enou gh. Jwilson,
they 1994.
191
Statistical figures and
J
For ethologists, having had the pleasure of observing animals and learnedwhat they do is generally exceeclingly rewarding without having yet fully understood vthv.
tables
Table Al. Factorials. Values ctf n! nl
t7
0l 1l 22 36 424 5 6 7 8 9 l0 Ir 2 rI 14 t.5 r6 ll I ti () l )0 I
120 720
5040
40320 362 880 3 628 800
39916800 479001 600
r
6?.27 020800
87 t78 29t200
I
307 614 368 0(X)
20922 789 tt88 (XX) l-5-5 (,tt7 6
l -1
42fl 096 (XX)
402 17.3 705 72tt 0(x) (xx)
l l 6.t5 t(x)"101{ til2 t
I tlttr
(x
)l{ I 76
6-10 (x x )