Segmental and prosodic issues in Romance phonology
AMSTERDAM STUDIES IN THE THEORY AND HISTORY OF LINGUISTIC SCIENCE ...
41 downloads
1299 Views
3MB Size
Report
This content was uploaded by our users and we assume good faith they have the permission to share this book. If you own the copyright to this book and it is wrongfully on our website, we offer a simple DMCA procedure to remove your content from our site. Start by pressing the button below!
Report copyright / DMCA form
Segmental and prosodic issues in Romance phonology
AMSTERDAM STUDIES IN THE THEORY AND HISTORY OF LINGUISTIC SCIENCE General Editor E.F.K. KOERNER (Zentrum für Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft, Typologie und Universalienforschung, Berlin) Series IV – CURRENT ISSUES IN LINGUISTIC THEORY Advisory Editorial Board Lyle Campbell (Salt Lake City); Sheila Embleton (Toronto) Brian D. Joseph (Columbus, Ohio); John E. Joseph (Edinburgh) Manfred Krifka (Berlin); E. Wyn Roberts (Vancouver, B.C.) Joseph C. Salmons (Madison, Wis.); Hans-Jürgen Sasse (Köln)
Volume 282
Pilar Prieto, Joan Mascaró and Maria-Josep Solé (eds) Segmental and prosodic issues in Romance phonology
Segmental and prosodic issues in Romance phonology
Edited by
Pilar Prieto
ICREA – Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona
Joan Mascaró
Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona
Maria-Josep SolÉ
Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona
JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY AMSTERDAM/PHILADELPHIA
4-
The paper used in this publication meets the minimum requirements of American National Standard for Information Sciences — Permanence of Paper for Printed Library Materials, ANSI Z39.48-1984.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Segmental and prosodic issues in romance phonology / edited by Pilar Prieto, Joan Mascaró and Maria-Josep Solé. p. cm. -- (Amsterdam studies in the theory and history of linguistic science. Series IV, Current issues in linguistic theory, ISSN 0304-0763 ; v. 282) Includes bibliographical references and index. 1. Romance languages--Phonology--Congresses. PC76 .S44 2007 440--dc22 2007060751 ISBN 978 90 272 4797 1 (Hb; alk. paper) © 2007 – John Benjamins B.V. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by print, photoprint, microfilm, or any other means, without written permission from the publisher. John Benjamins Publishing Co. • P.O.Box 36224 • 1020 ME Amsterdam • The Netherlands John Benjamins North America • P.O.Box 27519 • Philadelphia PA 19118-0519 • USA
CONTENTS
Introduction
vii
Part 1: Segments and processes
1
Detection of liaison consonants in speech processing in French: Experimental data and theoretical implications Noël Nguyen, Sophie Wauquier-Gravelines, Leonardo Lancia & Betty Tuller
3
Patterns of VCV coarticulatory direction according to the DAC model Daniel Recasens
25
The stability of phonological features within and across segments: The effect of nasalization on frication Maria-Josep Solé
41
Pre- and postaspirated stops in Andalusian Spanish Francisco Torreira
67
Part 2: Prosodic structure
83
Variation in the intonation of extra-sentential elements Lluïsa Astruc-Aguilera & Francis Nolan
85
Voicing-dependent cluster simplification asymmetries in Spanish and French Laura Colantoni & Jeffrey Steele
109
The phonetics and phonology of intonational phrasing in Romance Sónia Frota, Mariapaola D’Imperio, Gorka Elordieta, Pilar Prieto & Marina Vigário
131
Disentangling stress from accent in Spanish: Production patterns of the stress contrast in deaccented syllables Marta Ortega-Llebaria & Pilar Prieto
155
vi
CONTENTS
Part 3: Acquisition of segmental contrasts and prosody
177
On the effect of (morpho)phonological complexity in the early acquisition of unstressed vowels in European Portuguese Maria João Freitas
179
The perception of lexical stress patterns by Spanish and Catalan infants Ferran Pons & Laura Bosch
199
Logistic regression modelling for first and second language perception data Geoffrey Stewart Morrison
219
Rhythmic typology and variation in first and second languages Laurence White & Sven L. Mattys
237
Subject Index
259
INTRODUCTION
The second Phonetics and Phonology in Iberia (PaPI) conference, hosted by the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona in June 2005, proved a great success in bringing together scholars from around the world, all of them involved in researching contemporary issues in phonetics, phonology, and related areas, including language acquisition, language variation and change, and speech technology. This volume provides a selection of the papers presented at that conference. Most of them are concerned with the relationship between phonetics and phonology, and most of them also use a methodological approach that has come to be known as ‘laboratory phonology’. This approach, whose foundations were first laid about twenty years ago, sets out to answer a wide array of research questions through the use of experimental methods. In other words, experimental methodology previously associated with phonetic studies is applied to the realm of phonology with the goal of exploring the crucial correspondence between empirical data and theoretical claims. Over these last two decades, this experiment-based approach has proved extremely fruitful in its ability to test controversial claims in phonological theory, resolve phonological issues, and discover the principles guiding linguistic mechanisms (for a good overview, see articles by Pierrehumbert et al. 2001 and D’Imperio 2005). The specific focus of the papers in the present collection is descriptive and theoretical issues in the phonology of Romance languages. These papers provide new empirical data on a number of phonetic and phonological phenomena in a variety of Romance languages and their dialectal varieties, including Catalan (Eastern and Western Catalan, Valencian and Majorcan), French (European and Quebec), Italian (Neapolitan), Portuguese (Standard European and Northern European) and Spanish (Andalusian, Argentinian, Central Peninsular and Chilean). Importantly, most of the contributions take a crosslinguistic or crossdialectal perspective, paving the way to a better understanding of linguistic differences in typologically close language varieties. This focus on Romance languages is motivated by our belief that there is a need for multilanguage data to test current theoretical claims and models, which often lack precisely this sort of broad crosslinguistic basis. The virtue of crosslinguistic research is that it constitutes a valuable tool to explore similarities and differences between languages and thus allows us to construct general linguistic theories, while at the same time ensuring that the peculiaritites of individual languages can be characterized within the theory.
viii
INTRODUCTION
An important goal of this volume is to bridge the gap between traditional Romance linguistics—already with a long and rich tradition in data collection, cross-language comparison, and phonetic variation—and laboratory phonology. In our view, subjecting the theoretical claims and data from traditional Romance linguistics to the scrutiny of experimental techniques in the laboratory can only strengthen the scientific basis of this discipline and better integrate its findings in current phonetic and phonological theory. Though in recent years laboratory phonology has proved to be a broad and fertile interdisciplinary approach in Romance linguistics and has grown in popularity among researchers, it is still not a well-known and established approach among Romance scholars. The body of experimental work devoted to Romance languages is still far smaller than the work that has examined, for example, Germanic languages. This volume is thus an attempt to help redress that imbalance: as the reader will see, the studies collected herein present cutting-edge laboratory phonology research as applied to Romance languages. The volume has been organized into three main topic areas, which reflect the main themes of the conference. The first is concerned with segmental processes (coarticulation and assimilation processes, sandhi processes, feature cooccurrence and sequential restrictions), the second with prosodic structure (prosodic characterization of parentheticals, syllable structure, prosodic phrasing, stress and pitch accent prominence), and the third with the acquisition of segmental and prosodic features (the acquisition of vowel reduction, L1 and L2 vowel perception, initial word segmentation, and L2 rhythmic patterns). Thus we begin with the smaller segmental units, move on to larger phonological constituents, and conclude with a look at acquisition on both levels. Section 1, Segments and processes, comprises four papers which address the phonetic properties of segments, their mutual influence, and phonological processes across words. Three of these papers focus on the interaction of production and perception in phonological structure and sound change, mostly within the framework of gestural phonology (Browman & Goldstein 1986), which allows modelling of the biomechanical and aerodynamic constraints of speech gestures in a way that accounts for the actual observed patterns. These papers also serve to illustrate the notion that sound change can be partly explained by universal phonetic factors and has its origins in synchronic variation (Ohala 1989, 1991). Daniel Recasens examines the coarticulation patterns found in different types of VCV sequences in Catalan and Spanish and how they provide support for the ‘Degree of Articulatory Constraint’ (DAC) model of coarticulation. In particular, he argues that the workings of the speech production mechanism, and in particular the DAC model, can explain the directionality and extent of coarticulation found in VCV sequences. Acoustic data on the size of anticipatory and carryover effects between vowels [a] and [i] and a set of consonants clearly show that: (a) some VCV sequences with salient consonant anticipatory effects—like sequences with dark [l]—exhibit more vowel-to-
INTRODUCTION
ix
consonant anticipation effects than carryover effects, while sequences involving the alveolopalatals [] and [] show the reverse pattern; (b) vowel anticipation does not exhibit a comparable size for all consonants but is greater for unconstrained consonants than for more constrained ones. As predicted by the model, the direction and extent of vowel coarticulation varies inversely with the degree of constraint of the intervocalic consonant. Importantly, Recasens provides evidence in support of a close match between the predictions of the model and a number of observed sound changes and assimilatory processes in various Catalan dialects. Maria-Josep Solé argues that the articulatory-acoustic stability of phonological features may be affected not only by concurrent features, but also by features in adjacent segments when they coincide in time due to coarticulatory overlap. Specifically, she addresses the question of whether aerodynamic factors are at the origin of the incompatibility between nasality and frication. She presents the results of a series of experiments designed to explore the effects of velopharyngeal opening (or degrees of nasality) on the stability of segments requiring a high pressure build-up in the oral cavity, such as fricatives. Acoustic and aerodynamic evidence shows that in fricative + nasal sequences anticipatory velum lowering during the acoustic duration of the fricative reduces or even extinguishes the pressure difference required for frication. This is clear evidence that frication is unstable when it comes in contact with nasalization in adjacent segments. She presents important additional evidence that this instability is at the origin of a number of phonological patterns found historically and synchronically in Romance languages by which fricatives tend to lose their friction when they precede nasal consonants. In addition, she argues that the same aerodynamic and acoustic factors responsible for the combination of features within a segment can be used to explain how features interact in contiguous segments. Francisco Torreira deals with the well-known phonological process of /s/ aspiration in coda position present in a large number of Spanish dialects. The author furnishes instrumental data showing that in Andalusian Spanish, a southern variety of Peninsular Spanish, /s/ aspiration before voiceless stops is accompanied by consistent postaspiration of the stop consonant. His analysis of spontaneous speech clearly shows that /s/ preceding a stop, though usually realized as a period of aspiration or breathy voice, may be absent in a considerable number of cases. Crucially, voiceless stops following /s/ show a consistent pattern of postaspiration. This asymmetry suggests that the conditioning factor for postaspiration in Andalusian voiceless stops may be the presence of a preceding underlying laryngeal gesture that is not strictly timed with the supralaryngeal gestures. Torreira argues that a gestural analysis (Browman & Goldstein 1986) offers a plausible account for a phenomenon that is especially difficult to explain in terms of segments. Within this framework, the timing of the starting point of the glottal opening gesture with respect to the supraglottal closure may not be very accurately specified, while the timing of the ending point with respect to the end of the stop closure is more precise.
x
INTRODUCTION
Finally, Torreira reviews diachronic and synchronic examples from other languages which illustrate various paths of change for the same type of sound sequence. He suggests that this pattern in Andalusian Spanish, showing unstable preaspiration and more consistent postaspiration, might eventually lead towards a new category of aspirated voiceless stops, as has occurred in other languages. Noël Nguyen and colleagues address the well-known sandhi phenomenon of French liaison and the question of how liaison consonants are processed in speech perception. Specifically, the study analyses whether during speech comprehension liaison consonants are processed and represented differently from word-initial and word-final consonants. With this object, the authors undertook a series of perception experiments that examined potential differences in the detection rate of liaison consonants vs fixed consonants and attempted to determine whether these differences might be attributable to the phonetic properties of the consonants involved. The results provide evidence that liaison consonants are more difficult to detect than word-initial consonants (detection scores were lower and response times tended to be slower for the former than for the latter) and that these differences are not attributable to potential phonetic differences between the two. Nguyen and colleagues argue that the difficulty in processing liaison consonants seems to provide partial support for the autosegmental representation of liaison consonants as floating segments. Yet as detection accuracy also seemed to vary depending on the degree of lexicalisation of the carrier word sequence, the authors point out that more research is needed to evaluate the potential effects of probability of occurrence and thereby properly evaluate the predictions of exemplar-based models in the perception of liaison consonants. Prosodic structure and intonational phonology have been recurring themes in laboratory phonology work, partly because of the unreliability of introspective work on these issues. The four papers in Section 2, Prosodic structure, address important issues in this area of study, taking advantage of the benefits offered by using a common crosslinguistic experimental approach. Lluïsa Astruc-Aguilera and Francis Nolan examine the prosodic characteristics of extra-sentential elements (dislocated phrases, vocatives, adverbials, etc.) in Catalan and English. The authors present the results of three experiments set up to study phrasing and accentuation patterns in these constructions. Though phonological studies have traditionally considered that extra-sentential elements form prosodically independent units and are unaccented, results from these experiments show that they do not always form independent tonal units, nor are they always deaccented. Rather, they show variation in their phrasing and intonation, revealing a trade-off between prosodic independence and tonal subordination: deaccentuation only seems to be compulsory in those cases in which the extrasentential elements and the main phrase belong to the same prosodic domain. Also, the experiments reveal that accentual cues seem to be stronger and more consistent cues than phrasing cues and the degree of inter-speaker variation is lower in the former than in the
INTRODUCTION
xi
latter. In their third experiment, using both a database in which three levels of stress were specified and a masking noise technique for recording, the authors found that right-dislocated phrases were totally deaccented. Astruc and Nolan conclude that prosody signals the pheripheral status of extrasentential elements by general deaccentuation or compression of the pitch range, independent phrasing being a more optional cue. Laura Colantoni and Jeffrey Steele provide a detailed account of the behavior of stop-liquid clusters in two varieties of Spanish (Chilean and Argentinian) and two varieties of French (Quebec and European). They show that choice of cluster simplification, whether by assimilation or dissimilation, is correlated with the voicing properties of the stop and the manner properties of the liquid (tap, fricative, approximant). The results of the production experiment in the four varieties under investigation show that: a) similarity in manner and voicing between the two members of the cluster determines the degree of cluster simplification; b) in the case of stop-rhotic clusters, the phonetic characteristics of the rhotic determines the strategy used; in the case of Spanish, with the tap being highly similar to the stop, dissimilation via vowel epenthesis is the preferred outcome; and c) stop voicing plays a role in determining the degree of assimilation and dissimilation: given that voiceless stops are longer than their voiced counterparts, a compensatory lengthening effect is observed, and thus shorter epenthetic vowels are found. The effects that trigger synchronic variation can also account for the evolution of stopliquid clusters from Latin to Romance. Finally, the authors provide an optimality theory-based analysis of their experimental results. The chapter by Sónia Frota and colleagues explores the phonetic realization of intonational phrasing in five Romance language varieties: Catalan, two varieties of European Portuguese, Neapolitan Italian, and Spanish. Data from a common experimental database (the ‘Romance Languages Database’) is used to analyze the phonetic realization of phrasing in the five varieties under examination. First, the authors provide a typology of combinations of nuclear pitch accents plus boundary tones used across languages, as well as their relative frequency. The dominant boundary tone used in the five varieties is the high (H) boundary tone, in the form of either a continuation rise or sustained pitch. Second, they offer a detailed analysis of the phonetics of the H boundary tone across languages. Specifically, the data reveal that nuclear pitch accent choice affects the scaling of the H boundary tone in a similar and consistent way, namely, the tone is higher after High nuclear accents than after Low nuclear accents. They interpret this as resulting from the upstep of the H boundary tone after an accentual H. Also, the data reveal mixed effects of constituent length on the scaling of H boundary tones, with the languages observed clustering in two main groups: the CatalanSpanish group (with almost no length effects) and the Italian-European Portuguese group (with clear length effects). Marta Ortega-Llebaria and Pilar Prieto’s paper examines the acoustic correlates of stress prominence in Spanish in both accented and unaccented
xii
INTRODUCTION
environments (e.g. parentheticals). Traditional studies typically describe the correlates of stress in accented environments, thus suffering from covariation between stress and accent. This paper goes beyond traditional accounts in that the pitch accent factor is controlled for. The results of the production experiments described reveal that the stress contrast is maintained in deaccented contexts and that syllable duration and spectral tilt (intensity at high frequencies of the spectrum) are reliable acoustic correlates of this contrast in Spanish. These results contribute to the discussion about the nature of stress across languages, advocating for the view that stress prominence has its own phonetic cues, and against other views which claim that stress cues are parasitic on vowel reduction cues. Thus while American English, Dutch and Spanish differ in the degree of vowel reduction involved in marking stressed positions, they do not differ greatly in the way they use other acoustic correlates (i.e. duration and intensity) to signal the presence of stress. Section 3, Acquisition of segmental and prosodic structure, comprises four papers which address the acquisition of segmental and prosodic contrasts by infants and second language learners. The papers in this section illustrate how laboratory phonology is in fact starting to bridge the gap between psycholinguistics and phonology. Maria João Freitas focuses on the early production of vowels in unstressed position by European Portuguese-speaking children, and specifically, on how these children start acquiring two specific phonological processes of vowel reduction, namely, that of /, e/ turning into [] and that of /a/ turning into [] in unstressed positions. Since the acquisition of vowel reduction processes has not received much attention in the acquisition literature, this paper provides new empirical data from European Portuguese, a Romance variety which presents a number of reduced vowels in unstressed position deriving from the productivity of the vowel reduction process. On the basis of longitudinal data collected for four children aged 0;10 to 2;8, it is observed that Portuguese children acquire vowel reduction relatively early in the path of development, and that syllable deletion is one of the common strategies found in children. Freitas claims that the complexity of the target vowel system increases children’s early sensitivity to vowel differences and promotes the speed of phonological development. Interestingly, the results show that vowel reduction emerges either simultaneously in word-medial and word-final position or possibly earlier word-finally. Freitas suggests that the presence of morphological content in the word-final vowel might be promoting phonological development in this position. Geoffrey Stewart Morrison introduces logistic regression analysis as applied to L1 and L2 speech perception data involving Spanish vowels. The chapter is intended as a tutorial for L2-speech-perception students and researchers who are not familiar with the technique. Using data taken from previous identification experiments on L1 Spanish vowel perception and on L1 and L2 English vowel perception, the author applies logistic regression model fitting techniques to determine which acoustic cues are attended to by listeners
INTRODUCTION
xiii
when identifying stimuli. He shows that logistic regression coefficients can be successfully used to produce intuitive representations and quantify how listeners use those acoustic cues, as well as to model sequential stages in L2 learners’ perception. At the same time, these statistics can also be used to determine whether there are significant differences in the perception of stimuli by L1 vs L2 groups of listeners. In sum, Morrison shows how the logisticregression technique can be successfully used in L2 speech perception research. Ferran Pons and Laura Bosch focus on how infants under one year of age deal with word segmentation and which prosodic features they pay attention to in order to perform this task. Sensitivity to prosodic information has been observed very early in development in studies with English and Dutch children. For example, at nine months, American English infants show a trochaic bias, meaning that they prefer to listen to lists of strong-weak disyllabic words (trochaic), as opposed to lists of weak-strong disyllabic words (iambic), a stress pattern which is atypical of English. Using a slightly modified version of the Head-Turn Preference Procedure, a paradigm that has been used successfully in infant speech perception research over the past twenty years, Pons and Bosch set out to explore the metrical preferences of sixmonth-old Spanish- and Catalan-learning infants. The data revealed no pattern of preference for trochees. An additional experiment with nine-month-old infants revealed that, unexpectedly, even at this age they do not show a pattern of preference for trochaic or iambic stress. The authors partly attribute this crosslinguistic difference to a weaker predominance of the bisyllabic trochaic pattern in Catalan and Spanish relative to English. Yet the results cast some doubt on the usefulness of this prosodic cue (i.e. stress pattern) alone to help early word segmentation of fluent speech. The authors suggest that phonotactic information—the fact that heavy CVC syllables appear generally in stressed positions—might be combined with stress cues at a very early age in order to predict the patterns of preference. Finally, Laurence White and Sven Mattys set out to test the discriminative performance of different metrics of rhythmic distinctions across languages. One of the novelties of this article is that it collects data from second language rhythm, in the hope that the metrics will prove useful as a tool to identify the rhythmic differences between native English speakers, for example, and Spanish speakers of L2 English. The authors’ first production experiment is designed to test how well different metrics support the distinction between the rhythm of ‘syllable-timed’ French and Spanish and that of ‘stress-timed’ Dutch and English, with the effect of L1 on L2 rhythm also considered. The results show that rate-normalised metrics of variation in vocalic interval duration clearly and effectively (a) discriminate between the classic distinction between stress-timed and syllable-timed languages; and (b) are informative about the adaptation of speakers to rhythmically-similar (Dutch and English) or rhythmically-distinct (Spanish and English) second languages. Their second production experiment examines the rhythmic contrasts between
xiv
INTRODUCTION
different accents of British English, with results showing evidence of rhythmic gradience between them. Finally, results from a perceptual test find a normalised metric of vocalic interval variation to be the strongest predictor of the rating of the second language speaker’s accent as native or non-native. As a final word, we would like to thank the scholars who agreed to review the contributions included in this volume. We are greatly indebted to the anonymous reviewers at the John Benjamins office as well as the external reviewers who have participated in the assessment of articles: Laura Colantoni, Néstor Cuartero, Eva Estebas, Paula Fikkert, Chip Gerfen, Barbara Gili-Fivela, José Ignacio Hualde, Conxita Lleó, Francis Nolan, Hugo Quené, Daniel Recasens, Marija Tabain, and Laurence White. We owe the smooth progress in the production of this book to Anke de Looper of Benjamins and E.F.K. Koerner, the series editor, who we would like to thank for their active and continuing support from the start of the project. Many thanks are also due to Michael Kennedy-Scanlon, Marianna Nadeu and Maria del Mar Vanrell for their help in textual editing and proofreading. This work was partially supported by grants BFF2003-08364-C02, HUM200420318-E, and HUM2006-01758/FILO awarded by the Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnologia and FEDER, and by grant 2005 ARCS1 00174 awarded by the Agència de Gestió d’Ajuts Universitaris i de Recerca (AGAUR, Generalitat de Catalunya). We believe this volume will constitute a very useful companion for phoneticians, phonologists, and researchers investigating sound structure in Romance languages. It is our desire that it will spark further interest in laboratory phonology and will contribute to enlarging the body of research focusing on these languages. Barcelona, November 2006 Pilar Prieto Joan Mascaró Maria-Josep Solé
References Browman, Catherine P. & Louis Goldstein. 1986. “Towards an articulatory phonology”. Phonology Yearbook 3. 219-252. D’Imperio, Mariapaola. 2005. “La Phonologie de Laboratoire: Finalités et quelques applications”. Phonologie et phonétique: Forme et substance ed. by Noël Nguyen, Sophie Wauquier-Gravelines & Jacques Durand. 241-264. Paris: Hermès. Ohala, John J. 1989. “Sound change is drawn from a pool of synchronic variation”. Language Change: Contributions to the study of its causes ed. by Leiv E. Breivik & Ernst H. Jahr. 173-198. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
INTRODUCTION
xv
----------. 1991. “What's cognitive, what's not, in sound change”. Diachrony within Synchrony: Language History and Cognition ed. by Günter Kellermann & Michael D. Morrissey. 309-355. Duisburg: Peter Lang Verlag. Pierrehumbert, Janet, Mary E. Beckman & D. Robert Ladd. 2001. “Conceptual foundations of phonology as a laboratory science”. Phonological Knowledge: Conceptual and Empirical Issues ed. by Noel BurtonRoberts, Philip Carr & Gerard Docherty. 273-303. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
PART 1 SEGMENTS AND PROCESSES
DETECTION OF LIAISON CONSONANTS IN SPEECH PROCESSING IN FRENCH EXPERIMENTAL DATA AND THEORETICAL IMPLICATIONS *
NOËL NGUYEN1, SOPHIE WAUQUIER-GRAVELINES2, LEONARDO LANCIA1 & BETTY TULLER3 1 Laboratoire Parole et Langage, CNRS & Université de Provence, 2Structures formelles du langage, CNRS et Université de Paris VIII, 3Center for Complex Systems and Brain Sciences, Florida Atlantic University
Abstract The goal of the present study is to better understand the mechanisms involved in the processing of liaison consonants by listeners in French. Previous work (Wauquier-Gravelines 1996) showed that liaison consonants are more difficult to detect than word-initial consonants in a phoneme-detection task. We examined to what extent such differences are attributable to the consonants’ phonetic properties, and we also compared the perception of liaison consonants with that of fixed word-final and word-medial consonants, as well as wordinitial ones. The results suggest that liaison consonants have a specific perceptual status. Implications for both autosegmental and exemplar-based theories of liaison are discussed.
1.
Introduction French liaison is a well-known phenomenon of external sandhi that refers to the appearance of a consonant at the juncture of two words, when the second word begins with a vowel, e.g. un [œ] + enfant [f] → [œnf] “a child”, petit [pti] + ami [ami] → [ptitami] “little friend”. Liaison consonants are usually enchaînées, i.e. realized as syllable-onset consonants, although they can also appear in coda position, compare [p.ti.ta.mi] (with enchaînement) and [p.tit.a.mi] (without enchaînement, Encrevé 1988). In the following, the two words at the juncture of which liaison consonants appear will be referred to as Word 1 and Word 2, respectively.
*
This work was partly supported by the ACI Systèmes complexes en SHS Research Program (CNRS & French Ministry of Research) and by NSF Grant #0414657. We thank Sharon Peperkamp and Stéphanie Ducrot for drawing our attention to the missing-letter effect. We are also grateful to Robert Espesser for sharing his statistical expertise, and to Pierre Encrevé, Zsuzsanna Fagyal, Cécile Fougeron, Mariapaola D'Imperio, Maria-Josep Solé, Marina Vigário, and three anonymous reviewers for useful comments.
4
NGUYEN, WAUQUIER-GRAVELINES, LANCIA & TULLER
Among the many different approaches to French liaison that have been proposed over the last thirty years or so (see Tranel 1995; Côté 2005, for reviews), a major bone of contention relates to whether liaison is a phonological or a lexical phenomenon. The phonological approach dates back to early generative studies on French phonology, in which liaison was seen as an exception to a general process of final consonant deletion, referred to as the ‘French Truncation Rule’ by Schane (1968). By contrast, according to another proposal made later in the same general framework (e.g. Klausenburger 1974, 1977), liaison consonants arose in the course of the derivation owing to an insertion mechanism (views differed as to whether this epenthesis occurred at the end of Word 1 or at the onset of Word 2). More recent treatments of liaison in nonlinear phonology have reconceptualized the deletion/insertion dichotomy, as pointed out by Tranel (1995). Thus, in the autosegmental account proposed by Encrevé (1988) and Encrevé and Scheer (2005), liaison consonants are viewed as floating segments, with respect to both the segmental and syllabic tiers. Such consonants must be associated with both tiers to be phonetically realized, and this association takes place only under certain conditions. In both the linear and nonlinear phonological approaches, liaison is generally portrayed as being subjected to prosodic, morphological, syntactic and stylistic factors. Lexical approaches to liaison can be divided into two main strands. Suppletive analyses as advocated by Klausenburger (1984) among others, assume that words such as petit are associated in the lexicon with two distinct allomorphs, a longer one ending in a liaison consonant (/ptit/) and a shorter one without liaison consonant (/pti/). In contrast, in exemplar-based models, such as the one recently proposed by Bybee (2001, 2005), liaison consonants are said to take place within specific grammatical constructions, e.g. [NOUN z- [vowel]-ADJ]Plural, in enfants intelligents [fzteli] “clever children”. Constructions display different degrees of generality/abstractness, and range on a continuum from very abstract (as in the example given above), to fixed, lexicalized phrases like c’est-à-dire [setadi] “that is to say”. This provides a unified account of both false liaisons, which are attributed to the overgeneralization of a high-frequency construction, as in quatre enfants [katzf] “four children”, and word-specific differences in the realization of liaison. Frequency of use is of central importance, as liaison is assumed to occur more often within a sequence of words characterized by a higher frequency of co-occurrence. This approach is neutral with respect to the issue of whether liaison consonants result from a deletion or insertion process, nor does it make any specific claim as to whether the consonant belongs to Word 1 or 2. As noted above, liaison consonants when realized are usually enchaînées, i.e. syllabified into onset position. This results in a mismatch between word and syllable boundaries. Specifically, the syllable whose onset position the liaison consonant comes to occupy straddles the boundary between Word 1 and Word 2 (e.g. [p.ti.ta.mi], where the word boundary takes place between [t]
DETECTING LIAISON CONSONANTS IN FRENCH
5
and the following [a]). Recent psycholinguistic studies (Wauquier-Gravelines 1996; Gaskell, Spinelli & Meunier 2002; Spinelli, McQueen & Cutler 2003) have shown that this mismatch does not necessarily make it more difficult for listeners to identify the second word, but may in fact facilitate the recognition of that word with respect to a baseline condition. This raises questions for models of speech perception in which the syllable is viewed as a primary unit of segmentation in lexical access in French (see Content, Kearns & Frauenfelder 2001, for a recent discussion in that domain). A related issue concerns the way in which liaison consonants are processed in speech perception. One may ask which perceptual mechanisms allow a liaison enchaînée to be distinguished from word-initial as well as word-final consonants, and to which of the two words the liaison consonant is associated by the listener. More generally, the question arises whether in speech comprehension liaison consonants are processed and represented in a way that is different from fixed consonants. It is this issue that is addressed in the present paper. A series of experiments are reported which together suggest that liaison consonants do have a distinct perceptual status. Implications for current models of liaison in French will be discussed. 2.
Empirical evidence for a specific status of liaison consonants in speech perception Wauquier-Gravelines (1996) examined the speed and accuracy with which listeners can detect the presence of a liaison consonant in the speech chain. Because it has not been published, we present this work here in some detail and in light of more recent findings. Wauquier-Gravelines compared listeners’ responses to liaison consonants and word-initial consonants in a phoneme detection task. Listeners were presented with a series of sentences and were asked to detect a pre-specified phoneme in each sentence. The material contained pairs of sentences that were designed so that the target phoneme appeared as a word-initial consonant in one sentence (e.g. son navire [snavi] “his ship”) and as a liaison consonant (e.g. /n/ in son avion [snavj] “his plane”) in the other. The two sentences in each pair were matched with respect to their syntactic, lexical and phonemic make-up. A number of filler sentences were also used. Both the test and filler sentences were recorded by a native speaker of standard French. Two experiments were conducted. Each experiment was comprised of a training phase and a test phase. The target consonant was /t/ in the first experiment and /n/ in the second one. There were fourteen subjects, all native speakers of standard French, with no known hearing impairment, and naive as to the purpose of the experiment. The data showed that listeners experienced greater difficulties in detecting the liaison than the word-initial consonant. There were significantly fewer correct responses for the liaison than for the word-initial consonant for both /t/ (liaison: 67.8%, word-initial: 92.8%, χ2 = 9.56, p [n] IE snuṣā “daughter-in-law”, OE snoru > Latin nurus, Greek nuós, Armenian nu, Spanish nuera (Watkins 1985). 7. *sn > [n] Burmese *sna > [na] “nose”. (b) Phonological alternations 8. [sn] > [n]
9. [sm] > [m]
IE *dhus-no > Welsh dwn “dull, brown colour”, OE dun(n) “dark brown”; BUT IE *dhus-ko > Latin fuscus, OE dox, English dusk (Watkins 1985). IE *dhs-no > Latin fanum “temple”, English fanatic, (pro)fane; BUT IE *dhes-to > Lat. festus “festive”, German Fest, Spanish fiesta; IE *dhes-ya > Lat. fesiae, feriae, English fair, Spanish feria (Watkins 1985). IE *gras-men > Latin gramen “fodder”, English grama, gramineous; BUT IE *gras-ter > Greek gaster “stomach”, English gastric, epigastrium (Watkins 1985).
(c) Stylistic variation [zn] > [nn], [jn] isn't [nnt], ain’t [ent]; doesn’t [dnnt]; wasn’t [wnnt] (Gimson 1962). [mVn] > [mn], [mn] something [smn], [smn].
n ] Tyneside English (Local 2003:325). [nsVn] > [nVn] Vincent that [vn
[mfr Vn] > [mr Vn] San Francisco [sæmrnssko] American English (N. Hilty, p.c.). [vm] > [mm] [VN] > [VN]
give me, gimme [mm], have mine [ hæmman] (Gimson 1962). like them [lakm], tell them [telm]. BUT like this [lak s], tell this [tel s].
[VN] > [VN]
thank you, thanks [kju], [ks].
[zn] > [nn]
business [bdns], [bnns].
Table 1: Examples of fricative weakening/loss in fricative + nasal sequences.
52
MARIA-JOSEP SOLÉ
4.
Experiment. Variations in articulatory overlap The following experiment was designed to find out whether the tendency for prenasal fricatives to weaken or disappear can be attributed to anticipatory velopharyngeal opening for the nasal overlapping the acoustic duration of the fricative, thus diminishing the oropharyngeal pressure required for frication, as argued for concurrent features. In order to test this hypothesis we examined whether anticipatory velic opening occurred in such sequences and, if so, how it affected the aerodynamics and acoustics of fricatives. 4.1
Experimental procedure Simultaneous oropharyngeal pressure (Po), oral and nasal flow, and audio signal were obtained with PCquirer (Scicon) for five American English speakers producing words containing fricative + nasal sequences at slow and fast speech. The two speaking rates allowed us to observe the effects of increased articulatory overlap on the relative timing of velic and oral gestures, and how such changes in timing affect the pressure build-up for fricatives. The audio signal was digitized and sampled at 12 kHz, and the dc channels were sampled at 1 kHz. Po was obtained by a catheter introduced at the side of the mouth and bent behind the rear molars and connected to a pressure transducer. The volume flow from the mouth and the nose was collected simultaneously with a Rothenberg mask, using one of the outlet holes in the mouth mask for the pressure tube. The pressure and airflow signals were low-pass filtered at 50 Hz. Po and airflow were calibrated as described in Solé (2002a). Words containing fricative + nasal sequences with C1 = [s, z] and C2 = [n, m] in word medial position (e.g. Dessna [desn], Fresno [frezno], Missmer [msm], Mesmer [mezm]) were read in a carrier phrase. Control sequences with voiced and voiceless alveolar fricatives followed by laterals, stops, fricatives and approximants, all dento-alveolar (e.g. Grizzly [zl], Gizder [zd], is the [z], Ezra [z]; Esling [sl], Esda [sd], less the [s], Esra [s]) were also analyzed for comparison. The test and control tokens were randomized in the reading list. The aerodynamic and acoustic data were collected in two different sessions. In the first session, three speakers (JO, MS and DM) read five repetitions of each token at self-selected slow and fast rates. Because the three speakers showed different patterns of velic-oral coordination, two more speakers were recorded in a second session under the same conditions. Speakers JE and RS produced six repetitions of each token at slow and fast rates. 4.2
Measurements and analysis The measurements made on the data are illustrated in Figure 2, which shows the aerodynamic and acoustic data for Say Mesmer again in slow and fast speech for one of the speakers. Measurements were made at the following points in time for the fricative + nasal sequence: onset and offset of friction on the spectrographic records; onset of increased nasal flow (channel 5), indicated
THE STABILITY OF PHONOLOGICAL FEATURES
53
by a vertical line in Figure 2, reflecting velo-pharyngeal port opening for the nasal /m/; onset of oral closure for the nasal, indicated by a drop in oral flow (dotted line in channel 4), and offset of the nasal, indicated by an increase in oral flow (dashed line in channel 4) and an increase in amplitude and formant structure for the following vowel on the spectrogram. Figure 2 illustrates anticipatory velopharyngeal opening: the vertical lines mark the onset of nasal flow (channel 5) due to velum lowering for the nasal. At this point in time oral pressure decreases (channel 2) and the high frequency noise disappears (in slow speech) or is attenuated (in fast speech) (The sliding center frequency noise after the vertical line in fast speech reflects the effect of anticipatory lip movement, which is known to filter out higher frequencies). The increase in nasal flow leads the drop in oral flow (dotted line in channel 4) for the complete oral constriction for the nasal. Thus, for a few tens of ms there is concurrent (increasing) nasal flow and (decreasing) oral flow, characterised acoustically by some low amplitude aperiodic noise around 3 kHz. In other words, the velum starts to lower during the acoustic duration of the fricative, resulting in a sudden drop in amplitude of high frequency noise. SLOW
FAST
[ s e m e z m e n] [ s e m e z m e n] Figure 2: (1) Audio signal, (2) filtered Po, (3) unfiltered Po, (4) oral airflow, (5) nasal airflow and 0-5 kHz spectrogram of Say Mesmer again in slow and fast speech. Speaker JO. See text.
The duration of fricatives in test and control sequences was measured on spectrograms and aerodynamic records. In fast speech a considerable number of cases involved blending of the gestures for the two contiguous consonants generally resulting in deocclusivized following stops (as noted by Honorof 2003) or fricativized following sonorants [l, r, n, m]. The aerodynamic records were of great help in segmenting these blended sequences.
54
MARIA-JOSEP SOLÉ
4.3 Results 4.3.1 Patterns of velic movements. The patterns of velic coordination observed in the data are described in patterns 1 through 4, below, and schematized in Figure 3, which shows the oral gestures for the fricative (C1) + nasal (C2) sequence followed by traces of the observed velic patterns. Time 0 is the onset of the oral constriction for the nasal, indicated by the drop in oral flow (dotted line in Figure 2). A delayed oral gesture for the nasal, resulting in an epenthetic vowel, is represented at the bottom of Figure 3. (1)
If onset of velopharyngeal opening is synchronized with the oral constriction for the nasal, nasal flow starts at 0. We take this pattern to reflect a precise synchronization of the supraglottal and velic gestures for the nasal 5 , which allows sequencing of the fricative and the nasal consonant. The oral and velic gestures for the nasal, however, may not be precisely synchronized.
(2)
Onset of velum opening, i.e. nasal flow, may occur prior to the complete oral constriction for the nasal (time 0). That is, anticipatory velophrayngeal opening overlaps the acoustic duration of the preceding fricative. In this case we find concurrent oral flow (from the fricative slit constriction) and nasal flow before time 0 in the aerodynamic data, and lack of friction or attenuated friction on the spectrogram during this time interval.
(3)
Velopharyngeal opening may be delayed relative to the oral constriction for the nasal (time 0) resulting in a transitional epenthetic stop. In this case the aerodynamic data shows neither nasal flow (velum up) nor oral flow (nasal constriction formed) immediately after 0, and a silent gap on the spectrogram. We found the epenthetic stop to be audible if the oral and velic closure overlapped for over 15 ms. Figure 3 shows that the transitional stops emerging in these sequences are always nasally released, that is, they end when the velum lowers.
(4)
Finally, the oral constriction for the nasal may be delayed with respect to the release of the preceding fricative constriction, resulting in an epenthetic vowel. In this case oral and nasal flow is found immediately before 0. Only one such case was found in the data.
5
Whereas it is clear that the oral target gesture for the nasal is a complete constriction, the velic target gesture may not be opening the velum but rather achieving a certain magnitude of velum opening. If this were the case, onset of the oral closure for the nasal would be coordinated with a lowered velum, and nasal flow would begin before 0, as in the case in VN sequences. However, since most of the observed values cluster around 0, we will assume the suggested target coordination.
THE STABILITY OF PHONOLOGICAL FEATURES
55
It is worth noting that these patterns parallel those found in the historical and synchronic data reviewed in Section 3, in line with Ohala’s (1989, 1993) claim that sound change emerges from synchronic phonetic variation.
1. synchronous, onset of nasal flow at 0 2. bleeding Po, oral & nasal flow before 0 3. epenthetic stop, no nasal or oral flow after 0
4. epenthetic vowel, oral & nasal flow before 0 Figure 3: Diagrammatic representation of patterns of velic coordination in fricative-nasal sequences. Traces for the supraglottal movements for C1 and C2 followed by different traces of velum position (dashed lines). See text.
4.3.2 Coordination and timing of velic movements. The patterns of coordination of oral and velic movements in fricative + N sequences for each token at slow and fast rates are presented in Figure 4. This figure plots the time interval between onset of velopharyngeal opening and onset of the oral constriction for the nasal (time 0) for each token for the five speakers. The production of each individual token has been arranged in decreasing duration of that interval. Bars to the left of 0 are cases of anticipatory velopharyngeal opening (onset of nasal flow precedes onset of oral constriction for the nasal; pattern 2 in Figure 3); tokens at 0 represent synchronous onset of oral and velic gestures for the nasal (onset of nasal flow at 0; pattern 1 in Figure 3) and, consequently, a precise sequencing of the fricative and the nasal segment; bars to the right of 0 represent cases of velopharyngeal opening lagging behind the oral closure for the nasal (transitional stops; pattern 3 in Figure 3). The single case of vowel epenthesis (Fresno pronounced [frezno]), where the oral constriction for the nasal consonant is delayed relative to fricative release and velum lowering, is indicated by a lined bar (speaker DM, slow speech). Since inspection of the patterns of sequences involving voiced (e.g. Fresno) and voiceless (e.g. Dessna) fricatives showed no differences, both types of sequences were pooled in this graph. White bars represent homorganic sequences, [sn, zn], and grey bars represent heterorganic sequences, [sm, zm]. Each bar represents one token and the number of plotted tokens ranges
56
MARIA-JOSEP SOLÉ
between 5-6 for each sequence, [sn, zn, sm, zm], depending on the speaker and session. Figure 4 shows that although all observations cluster around time 0, the five speakers show three distinct patterns of velic-oral coordination. Subject JO exhibits extensive anticipatory velic opening during the acoustic duration of the fricative for homorganic and heterorganic sequences. Such anticipatory velic opening vents the required high oral pressure for turbulence and, thus, frication is attenuated or extinguished (see Figure 2). Speakers JE and MS exhibit a majority of cases of anticipatory velic opening in heterorganic sequences (/sm, zm/), but not in homorganic sequences (/sn, zn/), which exhibit a greater number of transitional stops (i.e. delayed velum lowering)6 . However, both speakers show cases of a precise synchronization (time 0) and of anticipatory velic opening for homorganic as well as heterorganic sequences. Finally, speakers DM and RS show mostly epenthetic stops, with a few cases of anticipatory velopharyngeal opening. The difference between homorganic and heterorganic sequences for these speakers seems to point in a different direction from that observed for speakers JE and MS: the epenthetic stops, resulting from a prolonged velic raising while the oral occlusion for the nasal has been achieved, appear to be more common and longer for /sm, zm/ sequences than for /sn, zn/ sequences. In spite of speaker-dependent differences, all speakers show cases of anticipatory velic opening bleeding the high oral pressure for frication. Overall, anticipation of velar activity during the fricative was found in 40% of the tokens in slow speech and 26% of the tokens in fast speech. We now turn to differences in the timing of gestures in slow and fast speech. We expected to find greater overlap of anticipatory movements of the velum with the fricative in fast vis-à-vis slow speech. Contrary to our expectations, Figure 4 shows similar patterns of coordination of velic and oral gestures at slow and fast rates for each speaker, and no major differences in the absolute values of velic timing across rates. Any differences are in the direction of more cases (i.e. more bars with negative values) and a longer period (i.e. larger negative values) of anticipatory velic lowering in slow than in fast speech. That is, the velum appears to be freer to lower before the oral constriction for the nasal is achieved at slow rates, most likely due to the time pressure in fast speech imposing tighter time constraints. However, since fricatives are slightly shorter at faster rates, the same period of anticipatory velopharyngeal opening has a slightly greater percentage effect in fast than in 6
Assuming that the motor instructions for the oral and velic gestures for the nasal are synchronic, and ignoring differences in velocity of articulators, the difference between homorganic and heterorganic sequences could in part be accounted for in terms of gestures involving independent articulators overlapping in time—anticipatory overlap of the labial and velic gesture for /m/ during the alveolar fricative in /sm, zm/ sequences. In homorganic /sn, zn/ sequences involving the same articulator—the tongue tip—the oral and velic gestures for the nasal would be delayed till the tongue tip was available for repositioning. However, this interpretation does not explain why velic opening lags behind the oral closure for the nasal in homorganic sequences.
57
THE STABILITY OF PHONOLOGICAL FEATURES
slow speech. This is illustrated in Figure 5, which plots the modal 7 duration of anticipatory velopharyngeal opening found for each speaker and rate as a percentage of the average duration of the fricative for that speaker and rate, for voiced and voiceless fricatives separately. Figure 5 shows a slightly larger percentage of overlapping velopharyngeal opening (i.e. longer black bars) in fast than in slow speech for all speakers and sequences. Figure 5 also allows us to observe the larger percentage of anticipatory velopharyngeal opening in voiced as opposed to voiceless fricatives, due to the shorter duration of the former. SLOW JO_sm_slow JO_sn_slow
12 11
JO_sm_fast JO_sn_fast
12 11
10
10
9
9 Token number
Token number
FAST
8 7 6 5 4
8 7 6 5 4
3
3
2
2
1
1 -60
-40
-20
0
20
40
60
-60
-40
-20
Time in ms
JE_sm_slow JE_sn_slow
12
11
10
10
9
9
8 7 6 5 4
40
60
40
60
8 7 6 5 4
3
3
2
2
1
1 -60
-40
-20
0
20
Time in ms
7
20
JE_sm_fast JE_sn_fast
12
Token number
Token number
11
0
Time in ms
40
60
-60
-40
-20
0
20
Time in ms
The mode was plotted rather than the mean because of extreme values in the data.
58
MARIA-JOSEP SOLÉ
MS_sm_slow MS_sn_slow
12
MS_sm_fast MS_sn_fast
12 11
10
10
9
9 Token number
Token number
11
8 7 6 5 4
8 7 6 5 4
3
3
2
2
1
1 -60
-40
-20
0
20
40
60
-60
-40
Time in ms
-20
0
20
40
60
Time in ms
DM_sm_fast DM_sn_fast
12 11 10 Token number
9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 -60
-40
-20
0
20
40
60
Time in ms
RS_sm_slow RS_sn_slow
12
11
10
10
9
9 Token number
Token number
11
RS_sm_fast RS_sn_fast
12
8 7 6 5
8 7 6 5 4
4 3
3
2
2
1
1 -60
-40
-20
0
20
Time in ms
40
60
-60
-40
-20
0
20
40
60
Time in ms
Figure 4: Coordination of oral and velic gestures for each production of Fresno and Dessna (white bars), and Mesmer and Missmer (grey bars) at slow and fast rates for each speaker. Tokens with voiced and voiceless fricatives have been pooled.
59
THE STABILITY OF PHONOLOGICAL FEATURES
fricative duration
overlapped velopharyngeal opening
100% 80% 60% 40% 20%
FAST JO
FAST JE
FAST MS
FAST DM
vd
vd
vless
vless
vd
vd
SLOW
vless
vless
vd
vd
SLOW
vless
vless
vd
vd
SLOW
vless
vless
vd
vd
SLOW
vless
vless
0%
SLOW FAST RS
Figure 5: Modal duration of anticipatory velopharyngeal opening (heterorganic and homorganic sequences pooled) as a percentage of the total duration of the voiced or voiceless fricative in slow and fast speech for the five speakers.
4.3.3 Fricative duration. The hypothesis that nasal leakage due to anticipatory velopharyngeal opening extinguishes or attenuates frication for a few tens of ms predicts that fricatives preceding nasal or nasalized segments should be phonetically shorter than those preceding non-nasal segments. We tested this prediction by measuring fricative duration for test and control tokens in slow and fast speech. The results of the measurements show that, as predicted, fricatives preceding nasals are generally shorter than those preceding non-nasals. (Following fricatives and laterals also tend to result in shorter preceding fricatives for some speakers.) Two-way ANOVAs with fricative voicing (voiced, voiceless) and following consonant (nasal, non-nasal) as independent variables, and duration of the fricative as the dependent variable, were performed for each speaker and speech rate separately. Table 2 shows the results for the ANOVAs. The durational differences between fricatives preceding nasal vs non-nasal consonants reached significance for speakers JO, MS and DM in slow and fast speech. Since the interaction between the two factors was significant for speaker JE at faster rates, one-way ANOVAS were carried out for voiced and voiceless fricatives separately for this speaker. Voiced fricatives were found to be significantly shorter preceding nasal than oral consonants (F(1,33) = 11.629, p