NE U R O M E T H O D S
Series Editor Wolfgang Walz University of Saskatchewan Saskatoon, SK, Canada
For other titles ...
56 downloads
1565 Views
5MB Size
Report
This content was uploaded by our users and we assume good faith they have the permission to share this book. If you own the copyright to this book and it is wrongfully on our website, we offer a simple DMCA procedure to remove your content from our site. Start by pressing the button below!
Report copyright / DMCA form
NE U R O M E T H O D S
Series Editor Wolfgang Walz University of Saskatchewan Saskatoon, SK, Canada
For other titles published in this series, go to www.springer.com/series/7657
Animal Models of Schizophrenia and Related Disorders
Edited by
Patricio O’Donnell Departments of Anatomy & Neurobiology and Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD, USA
Editor Patricio O’Donnell, MD, Ph.D. Departments of Anatomy & Neurobiology and Psychiatry University of Maryland Baltimore, MD 21201, USA
ISSN 0893-2336 e-ISSN 1940-6045 ISBN 978-1-61779-156-7 e-ISBN 978-1-61779-157-4 DOI 10.1007/978-1-61779-157-4 Springer New York Dordrecht Heidelberg London Library of Congress Control Number: 2011928327 © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011 All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Humana Press, c/o Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 Spring Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are subject to proprietary rights. While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of going to press, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omissions that may be made. The publisher makes no warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material contained herein. Printed on acid-free paper Humana Press is part of Springer Science+Business Media (www.springer.com)
Preface to the Series Under the guidance of its founders Alan Boulton and Glen Baker, the Neuromethods series by Humana Press has been very successful since the first volume appeared in 1985. In about 17 years, 37 volumes have been published. In 2006, Springer Science + Business Media made a renewed commitment to this series. The new program will focus on methods that are either unique to the nervous system and excitable cells or need special consideration to be applied to the neurosciences. The program will strike a balance between recent and exciting developments like those concerning new animal models of disease, imaging, in vivo methods, and more established techniques. These include immunocytochemistry and electrophysiological technologies. New trainees in neurosciences still need a sound footing in these older methods in order to apply a critical approach to their results. The careful application of methods is probably the most important step in the process of scientific inquiry. In the past, new methodologies led the way in developing new disciplines in the biological and medical sciences. For example, Physiology emerged out of Anatomy in the 19th century by harnessing new methods based on the newly discovered phenomenon of electricity. Nowadays, the relationships between disciplines and methods are more complex. Methods are now widely shared between disciplines and research areas. New developments in electronic publishing also make it possible for scientists to download chapters or protocols selectively within a very short time of encountering them. This new approach has been taken into account in the design of individual volumes and chapters in this series. Wolfgang Walz
v
Preface Animal models of schizophrenia and major psychiatric disorders have been sought for decades. As it is clear that schizophrenia is a uniquely human disease, animal models are not likely to reproduce all facets of this disorder. However, there has been considerable growth in this field in the past several years with models that test possible pathophysiological scenarios, the role of environmental factors, or contributions of gene variants conferring risk for schizophrenia to abnormal function and behavior. As a result, we are now facing new vistas on pathophysiology that could lead to novel therapeutic approaches and even hint at possible preventive strategies. The animal models that yield this advance will be needed to gain deeper insight into biological processes that can yield to behavioral anomalies and, perhaps more importantly, to explore novel treatments. It is critical in this regard that the manipulations used to model schizophrenia-relevant phenomena are used consistently across laboratories. Hence, this book presents an overview of what information can be obtained with several different models and a detailed account of how to generate such models. As the search for animal models of schizophrenia is a decadeslong endeavor, many models have been proposed. Here we cover only some; it would be unrealistic to describe all models proposed. The sample presented in this book includes pharmacological models such as non-competing NMDA antagonists, emphasizing their use in vitro, neurodevelopmental models such as the neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion and the antimitotic MAM, models that reproduce environmental factors such as neonatal hypoxia, vitamin D deficits, and prenatal immune activation, as well as several different genetic model approaches. Although not all models have been studied with the same tools, a remarkable convergence is observed for most: whenever the impact of these manipulations on cortical inhibitory interneurons was tested, it was found abnormal. Of relevance to the disease, these anomalies emerge during adolescence and are consistent with a wealth of human post-mortem and imaging data. Thus, the use of animal models to gain insight into pathophysiological mechanisms of relevance to major psychiatric disorders is paying off and will certainly be expanded to test targets that could restore or ameliorate function. Patricio O’Donnell
vii
Contents Preface to the Series . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
v
Preface . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
vii
Contributors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
xi
1.
2.
A Method to the Madness: Producing the Neonatal Ventral Hippocampal Lesion Rat Model of Schizophrenia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . R. Andrew Chambers and Barbara K. Lipska Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol) Administration: A Promising Animal Model for Psychosis Onset . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Gwenaëlle Le Pen, Alfredo Bellon, Marie-Odile Krebs, and Thérèse M. Jay
1
25
3.
Prenatal Infection and Immune Models of Schizophrenia . . . . . . . . . . . . . Alan S. Brown
79
4.
The Hypoxic Rat Model for Obstetric Complications in Schizophrenia . . . . . . Andrea Schmitt, Peter Gebicke-Haerter, Ulrich Sommer, Markus Heck, Anja Lex, Mario Herrera-Marschitz, Mathias Zink, Markus Fendt, and Peter Falkai
93
5.
The Developmental Vitamin D (DVD) Model of Schizophrenia . . . . . . . . . . 113 Darryl W. Eyles, Thomas H.J. Burne, Suzy Alexander, Xiaoying Cui, and John J. McGrath
6.
Studying Schizophrenia in a Dish: Use of Primary Neuronal Cultures to Study the Long-Term Effects of NMDA Receptor Antagonists on Parvalbumin-Positive Fast-Spiking Interneurons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 127 M. Margarita Behrens
7.
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 149 Pascal Steullet, Jan-Harry Cabungcal, Anita Kulak, Michel Cuenod, Françoise Schenk, and Kim Q. Do
8.
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models . . . . . . . . 189 P. Alexander Arguello and Joseph A. Gogos
9.
DISC1 Mouse Models . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 211 Yavuz Ayhan, Hanna Jaaro-Peled, Akira Sawa, and Mikhail V. Pletnikov
10. Genetically Engineered Mice for Schizophrenia Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . 231 Juan E. Belforte and Kazu Nakazawa
ix
x
Contents
11. Epigenetic Animal Models of GABAergic Deficit in Mental Disorders . . . . . . . 243 Patricia Tueting, Erminio Costa, and Alessandro Guidotti 12. Modeling Schizophrenia in Neuregulin 1 and ErbB4 Mutant Mice . . . . . . . . 261 Yisheng Lu, Dong-Min Yin, Wen-Cheng Xiong, and Lin Mei Subject Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 279
Contributors SUZY ALEXANDER • Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, Australia P. ALEXANDER ARGUELLO • Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA YAVUZ AYHAN • Department of Psychiatry, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA M. MARGARITA BEHRENS • The Computational Neurobiology Laboratory, Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, CA, USA JUAN E. BELFORTE • Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina ALFREDO BELLON • Centre de Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France ALAN S. BROWN • Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA THOMAS H.J. BURNE • Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, Australia JAN-HARRY CABUNGCAL • Department of Psychiatry, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland R. ANDREW CHAMBERS • Laboratory for Translational Neuroscience of Dual Diagnosis and Development, Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA ERMINIO COSTA • Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA MICHEL CUENOD • Department of Psychiatry, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland XIAOYING CUI • Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, Australia KIM Q. DO • Department of Psychiatry, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland DARRYL W. EYLES • Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, Australia PETER FALKAI • Department of Psychiatry, University of Goettingen, Goettingen, Germany MARKUS FENDT • Faculty of Biology, University of Tubingen, Tubingen, Germany PETER GEBICKE-HAERTER • The Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany JOSEPH A. GOGOS • Department of Neuroscience, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA ALESSANDRO GUIDOTTI • Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA MARKUS HECK • The Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany MARIO HERRERA-MARSCHITZ • Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
xi
xii
Contributors
HANNA JAARO-PELED • Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA THÉRÈSE M. JAY • Centre de Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Universite Paris Descartes, Paris, France MARIE-ODILE KREBS • Centre de Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France ANITA KULAK • Department of Psychiatry, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland GWENAËLLE LE PEN • Centre de Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Université Paris Descartes, Paris, France ANJA LEX • Faculty of Biology, University of Tubingen, Tubingen, Germany BARBARA K. LIPSKA • Clinical Brain Disorders Branch, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA YISHENG LU • Georgia Health Sciences University, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Genetics, Augusta, GA, USA JOHN J. MCGRATH • Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, St. Lucia, Brisbane, QLD, Australia LIN MEI • Georgia Health Sciences University, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Genetics, Augusta, GA, USA KAZU NAKAZAWA • Unit on Genetics of Cognition and Behavior, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA MIKHAIL V. PLETNIKOV • Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA AKIRA SAWA • Department of Psychiatry, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA FRANÇOISE SCHENK • Department of Psychiatry, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland ANDREA SCHMITT • Department of Psychiatry, University of Goettingen, Goettingen, Germany ULRICH SOMMER • The Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany PASCAL STEULLET • Department of Psychiatry, Center for Psychiatric Neuroscience, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois, Prilly-Lausanne, Switzerland PATRICIA TUETING • Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA WEN-CHENG XIONG • Georgia Health Sciences University, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Genetics, Augusta, GA, USA DONG-MIN YIN • Georgia Health Sciences University, Institute of Molecular Medicine and Genetics, Augusta, GA, USA MATHIAS ZINK • The Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany
Chapter 1 A Method to the Madness: Producing the Neonatal Ventral Hippocampal Lesion Rat Model of Schizophrenia R. Andrew Chambers and Barbara K. Lipska Abstract The neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion (NVHL) rat model of schizophrenia has demonstrated broad heuristic utility as an investigative platform encompassing many of the behavioral, neurobiological, and developmental aspects of this devastating neuropsychiatric illness affecting 1% of all human beings. This chapter serves as an essential description of materials and methods for generating and verifying the NVHL model in rats, which continues to hold significant potential in helping us understand schizophrenia, comorbid disorders, and their neurodevelopmental dynamics. Many of the approaches described here can be modified or adapted for producing other types of neurodevelopmental models of behavioral disorders. Key words: Ventral hippocampus, neonatal lesions, ibotenic acid, neurodevelopmental, methods.
1. Introduction The neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion (NVHL) rat model, developed at St. Elizabeth’s Hospital in Washington, DC, was first described in 1993 from the intramural program of the NIMH (1). It has become one of the most intensively studied animal models of schizophrenia ever developed, being the topic of over 120 primary research publications by at least a dozen independent research groups worldwide (2). Aside from representing a major advance in schizophrenia research in terms of providing the first significant non-pharmacological model of schizophrenia (i.e., not produced by exposure to psychostimulant or hallucinogenic drugs), this model includes multiple features that make it an attractive investigative platform. As a model of a complex neuropsychiatric syndrome that encompasses many neurobiological, P. O’Donnell (ed.), Animal Models of Schizophrenia and Related Disorders, Neuromethods 59, DOI 10.1007/978-1-61779-157-4_1, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
1
2
Chambers and Lipska
endophenotypic, and clinical/diagnostic features, which might seem etiologically unrelated, it succeeds in reproducing most of these phenomena in detail, if not in broad strokes, as a result of one experimental intervention: developmental damage of the ventral hippocampus in post-natal rats (see (2) for a comprehensive review). So, for example, instead of representing a paradigm that can test rats for a single schizophrenic trait, such as deficits in pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) of auditory stimuli, the model itself produces deficits in PPI, and many other schizophrenialike features including deficits in latent inhibition (LI), positivelike symptoms (e.g., behavioral hyper-responsiveness to novelty, stress, and psychostimulants that are reducible with neuroleptic treatment), cognitive symptoms (e.g., spatial working memory deficits), and neuroleptic-unresponsive negative symptoms (e.g., abnormalities in grooming and social behavior). Neurobiologically, NVHLs produce a host of structural and functional alterations that are downstream in anatomical space and developmental time from the initial lesion and that also mimic neuroimaging or post-mortem findings in human schizophrenia. Timely to growing interest in the periadolescent neurodevelopmental basis of schizophrenia and many other psychiatric disorders, the positive symptom-like behavioral features of the model show periadolescent emergence, in parallel with progressively abnormal biological or physiological measures of prefrontal cortical dysfunction (3). Moreover, the distributed neurocircuit effects of the lesion also represent mechanisms informative to understanding complex co-morbidities spanning mental illnesses such as addictions (4). Finally, the model itself or many of the techniques for producing it may be used to develop more specific experimental interventions in modeling a wide variety of other mental disorders involving alternative developmental time points, anatomical sites, or bioactive agents of lesion delivery, or combinations of lesions with particular environmental or genetic backgrounds. Successfully producing NVHL rats in sufficient quantities, while similar to approaches commonly used in stereotaxic brain interventions in adult rats, requires more of a nuanced, skilled touch and several special modifications. For the second author, originally developing this model accurately (e.g., achieving >80% lesion accuracy) from scratch required the sacrifice of well over 1000 rat pups, and working though numerous “wrong directions.” Even with the bugs worked out, and as trained directly from the inventor herself, the first author required nearly 4 months of effort and >100 pups to gain >66% proficiency. It is our goal to provide this chapter as a fairly complete and definitive resource for other investigators who are starting out in producing NVHL or similar developmental models so that they may get up and running with sufficient number of rats more quickly. We describe the essential materials and methods for producing the
Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model
3
NVHL lesion and verifying its accuracy in adulthood. Section 4 describes trouble areas, pitfalls, and miscellaneous advice in this experimental approach.
2. Materials 2.1. Rats
Any rat strain of either gender may be used with the NVHL model, but the majority of papers (>90%) have examined Sprague–Dawleys and probably at least three-quarters of papers have focused on males. Thus if an experiment aims to reference or contextualize experimental results to the bulk of the literature, the best bet is to use Sprague–Dawley males. Alternatively, to explore gender or genetic biases with the lesion, both genders or Spragues with another strain are used in single experiments. The lesions are made in (and stereotaxic coordinates are designed for) 7-day-old rat pups (PD-7). This developmental point was chosen to approximate the second to third trimester equivalent of human fetal brain development, when the hippocampus is undergoing a crucially sensitive period of network formation, thought to make it vulnerable to a host of infectious, hormonal, neuroimmunological, or anoxic risk factors implicated in the early pathogenesis of human schizophrenia. Given this background, and the potential effects of other sources of perinatal stress, it is essential that rats are born in the laboratory from pregnant dams, either bred in or arriving in the laboratory housing facility no more than 17 days into gestation (i.e., 4 or more days before parturition on gestational day 21). Orders for pregnant dams at 13–17 days of gestation usually require 2–4 weeks advanced notice. In our experience, a given pregnant dam will typically provide 4–6 male rats/litter of appropriate weight range for the surgery. With one stereotaxic setup, and performing lesions proficiently at full speed, surgeries may proceed at about 4 rats/h. This rate can be increased with more stereotaxic setups (e.g., 6–8 rats/h with two setups). Depending on the expected rate of surgery, pregnant dams should be ordered with gestational due dates 1–2 days apart to avoid traffic jams of pups available for surgery.
2.2. Ibotenic Acid and Artificial CSF
Ibotenic acid (α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-isoxazoleacetic acid) is an NMDA receptor agonist, naturally produced by the Amanita mushroom species. In low doses delivered generally to the CNS, it has psychedelic effects, and interestingly, a portion of the compound is metabolized to muscimol, a potent and selective GABAA receptor agonist. In concentrated doses delivered to specific brain regions, it causes lethal neuronal calcium influx via the NMDA
4
Chambers and Lipska
receptor. However, as an advantage among lesion techniques, it predominantly kills cells with cell bodies in the vicinity of the high dose and leaves axonal projections through the “blast zone” intact. Ibotenic acid (IBO) is available in 5 mg quantities (e.g., from Sigma) (>$350) providing enough toxin, theoretically, to lesion up to 800 rats. Ibotenic acid for neonatal lesioning is dissolved in artificial CSF (aCSF) vehicle, which after mixing and storing in Eppendorf tubes may be kept in –80◦ C freezer for >6 months and possibly up to 1 year without losing effectiveness. At room temperature and subjected to ambient light, IBO will lose potency within 6 h. The aCSF is a solution of electrolytes at physiologic pH that is inert when administered intrathecally to the rat. Beyond serving as the vehicle for IBO delivery, the aCSF is (a) delivered alone to the brains of SHAM-operated controls; (b) the general tubing fluid between the IBO or aCSF/SHAM bolus and the injection syringe; and (c) a rinsing solution for the injection cannula/tubing/syringe system after cleaning and storage in alcohol. A 1 l volume of aCSF is made by the following three-part recipe: (1) Into flask A, place 500 ml of sterile distilled H2 O and add the following: NaCl KCl CaCl2 •2H2 O MgCl2 •6H2 O
8.66 g 0.224 g 0.206 g 0.163 g
(2) Into flask B, place 500 ml of sterile distilled H2 O and add the following: 0.214 g or Na2 HPO4 •H2 O (dibasic) 0.358 g Na2 HPO4 •7H2 O NaH2 PO4 •H2 O (monobasic) 0.027 g (3) Add flask A to flask B. This gives a 1 l solution with 150 μM Na, 3.0 μM K, 1.4 μM Ca, 0.8 μM Mg, 1.0 μM P, and 155 μM Cl. This solution has pH 7.4. Artificial CSF may be kept in a regular refrigerator in a covered flask. The somewhat more challenging work of preparing IBO in the aCSF vehicle is described in the following four steps, which require pH litmus paper, micropipettes, and Eppendorf tubes: (1) In the small vial in which the 5 mg IBO arrives, add 500 μl of Millipore sterilized aCSF. Use a button spinner to briefly agitate the solution in the container at this point and after subsequent additions of solution described next. (2) After adding the aCSF to the original container, the IBO does not completely dissolve. Now add 1 μl of 10 N NaOH (MW 40 g/mol) to help ionize the acid and facilitate
Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model
5
solubility. Check the bottom of the container to see how much IBO remains undissolved; repeat these 1 μl additions of 10 N NaOH until only a few tiny IBO clumps are visible. (3) Now the IBO is nearly, but not totally, in solution, and the pH is still not quite physiologic (e.g., it is 12 rats) will lead to higher frequencies of PD-7 rats below the 15 g weight minimum. To address this possibility, and to anticipate the number of subjects available for surgery, it is recommended that litters be assessed at PD-3 to 5 and gendertyped and culled as needed (more culling will speed growth per animal). Gender-typing pups before PD-3 can be tricky but is quite accurate thereafter with a brief visual exam of pup’s genitalia (Fig. 1.4). Generally, compared to females, males will have a greater distance between anus and phallic prominence, and, no huge surprise, the males have a larger phallic prominence. On the day of surgery, it is important to balance randomized NVHL vs. SHAM assignments across and within available litters. Across litters, this is important for minimizing the effects of genetic heterogeneities between moms. Allowing for expected rates of attrition due to incorrectly placed lesions, this means generating NVHL vs. SHAM ratios (e.g., 5:3 NVHL/SHAM), as consistently as possible across litters. Within litters, even when
female
male
Fig. 1.4. Sketch of gender differences in genitalia in PD-3–7 pups.
Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model
11
keeping within the 15–20 g weight range, it is important to optimize balance of lesion status by weight (e.g., ensure all the SHAM animals do not tend smaller). 3.2. Performing NVHL and SHAM Control Surgeries 3.2.1. Surgical Setup
On the day of surgery, setting up should include the following: Hypothermic anesthesia bucket: Prepare the anesthesia bucket as a rubber ice bucket filled with small ice chips/balls. The researcher’s fist is used to punch a central crater in the ice field 5–10 cm deep for pup placement. Cannula preparation: Tubing and syringes must be clean and patent with easy gliding motion of the syringe plunger. Between surgical sessions, the lines should be flushed with a tuberculin syringe loaded, first with 90% EtOH (4–5 ml squirted vigorously through a 25-gauge needle into the plunger-less Hamilton syringe end of the cannula) and second with aCSF flush (4–5 ml). During these flushes, one wants to see a thin but vigorous stream of fluid emerging from the injector tip. If the flow is slow, repeated flushing may be necessary, possibly with the use of cleaning wires provided with Hamilton syringes for removal of matter from inside the needles. The functioning cannula is then filled completely with aCSF free of air bubbles in the lines (although air bubbles may initially be used to check for good flow in the cannula as described above). As with the cleaning, this insertion of the “hydraulic” aCSF is accomplished by a more gentle injection of aCSF through the plunger-less Hamilton syringe with a tuberculin syringe. Now, for loading the injection bolus (e.g., IBO, aCSF, or dye), one first pulls back on the Hamilton’s plunger to create an air bubble of 0.2–0.3 μl volume as a mobile boundary between the aCSF hydraulic and the injection solution. This bubble not only prevents dilution of the IBO or dye into the hydraulic aCSF but also provides a visual marker to confirm that flow is occurring within surgeries, and for knowing for how much injection solution is left in the cannula over the course of multiple surgeries. Now with the indicator bubble in place, simply suck up IBO (from an Eppendorf tube), aCSF, or dye as needed. For a run of eight or more NVHL surgeries, we recommend filling the line with >7 μl by gentle backward plunging on the Hamilton syringe. If not anticipating a need for IBO for the rest of the session, return it to –80 freezer; if reloading later in the day, it may be stored on ice and under cover for 2–4 h in the surgical room. Now, mounting the cannula needle onto the stereotax, it is key to have the needle secured to the injector arm as close to the tip of the needle as possible for stability (Fig. 1.5), because the needle itself is somewhat unstably flexible. Also, the bevel of the injection needle should be squarely positioned facing the caudal aspect of the rat. After mounting the Hamilton syringe into the infusion pump, go ahead and manually waste some injection fluid out the injector needle tip
12
Chambers and Lipska
no
yes
Fig. 1.5. Proper mounting of the injection needle on the stereotaxic arm.
to again confirm patency and ensure that the infusion pump and the syringe are correctly mechanically interfaced. Finally with no pup on stage, make one or two 0.3 μl infusions to verify proper function and volume delivery, and to generate a consistent balancing of hydraulic tension in the system. Wipe away the small infusate bead on the tip of the needle. Pre-op ready chamber: Properly selected pups (about six at a time, at the start of a run) should be on hand in a rat-housing tub. Each rat should be numbered with a marker on their back, with weight references, semi-random lesion assignments, and possibly gender ID noted on paper. Logistically, it is best to perform a series of rats (six or more) as IBO lesions and another series as SHAMS, to prevent from having to change cannula between surgeries. If two setups are used, they may be segregated for NVHL vs. SHAM surgeries. Post-op chamber: This may be a rat-housing tub, containing soft padding overlying a warming pad. 3.2.2. Inducing Hypothermic Anesthesia
Although unfamiliar to some investigators and animal research review committees, generalized hypothermia is safer, easier, and less expensive than chemical anesthesia for infant rats. The pup is placed in the ice crater for 15–20 min, with the cover placed over the ice bucket. During this induction, the pup should be checked every 5 min or so, as initially it will often succeed in crawling out of the crater to the edge where hypothermia occurs unevenly. In the 15–20 min window, the rat should be checked for complete immobilization and lack or respiration. This is achieved as early as 17 min. This induction should be rigorously timed. Leaving pups on ice for longer than 20 min significantly increases risk of death; taking them off too soon increases the risk of premature return of mobility during surgery, which is a major cause of lesion inaccuracies or unintended mechanical lesions in SHAM animals.
Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model
13
Usually, 17–18 min on ice will provide 12–16 min of immobility, which in experienced hands is a sufficient time to perform the surgery. 3.2.3. Lesioning
The two major issues with the neonatal lesion surgery, as opposed to an adult stereotaxic surgery, are (1) the targets are much smaller and (2) the skull bone is far thinner and highly flexible. The former increases the challenge, while the latter provides both up and downsides. On the upside, there is no need for a bone drilling step; the injection needle tip itself is the drill. On the downside, the pup’s braincase is fragile and flexible, rendering head immobilization virtually impossible with traditional means. Thus, your target is very small, and to some extent, it will be moving, because anything (needles and fingers) that touches the pup will displace it after the stereotaxic coordinates are set. This has two important implications for the attitude of the researcher. First, compared to adult stereotaxic surgery, it has to be accepted that neonatal lesioning is much more of an acquired art or a skill (like bowling, dart throwing, and golf ). Second, like these sports, it takes practice to become reasonably proficient. Next we provide detailed guidelines on how the surgery should be approached, while Section 4.2 outlines approaches in practice lesioning. After induction of anesthesia, lesioning consists of three basic steps: (a) mounting the rat; (b) puncture and infusion; and (c) post-op recovery. Including anesthesia, all of these steps performed for a single rat take 40–55 min. However, when performing serial surgeries in assembly line fashion, one rat is undergoing anesthesia while another is being operated on, while another is in recovery. This shortens the procedure to 15–20 min per rat, or shorter with more than one setup. The remainder of this section assumes only one setup and is performed identically with IBO infusions (NVHL) or aCSF (SHAMs). (1) Mounting the rat: This is a crucial step in the process as mistakes here amplify the chance of inaccurate targeting. After the rat has been adequately anesthetized, place it squarely on its abdomen on the surgical platform with the aid of guide marks drawn on the platform. The rat’s tail should be pulled out straight behind and its four limbs should be spread out to the sides so that they do not sit under the rat’s neck and torso (Fig. 1.6). All this is to ensure that the body and the head sit on the platform in the most stable, central and square position (with respect to the stereotax arm movement). Now, using a scalpel or a sterile razor blade, a single incision is made down the middle of the rat’s head (1.5–2 cm long) so that bregma and the sagittal and coronal cranial sutures are observed. Be careful not to lacerate the veins or the tissue on the exposed skull. Spread out the
14
Chambers and Lipska
Coronal Suture
Lamboidal Suture
Bregma
Saggital Suture
Fig. 1.6. Proper positioning and taping of pups on the stereotaxic stage.
width of the wound with your fingers to ensure adequate lateral room for insertion of the cannula needle into the skull. Now, using strips of freezer tape, secure the rat’s neck and head to the platform crosswise (Fig. 1.6). The caudal strip is placed first to stabilize the neck. Then, one hand is used to manually re-position the head so that it is flat on the platform and square with the stage (e.g., the sagittal suture is made parallel with the long-axis motion of the stereotaxic arm and non-rotated with respect to the vertical), while the other hand places the rostral strip of tape. Note that either or both the caudal and rostral pieces of tape can be placed just a bit over the open wounds to aid in keeping the wound open. Further, quick adjustments should now be made as needed to the tape and/or the rat’s positioning as necessary to maximize alignment of cranial landmarks with the stereotax (you will notice that the rat’s skull slides around very easily within the skin around the rat’s head). In making all these adjustments, it is important to view the animal from directly above, as a viewing angle off the vertical will result in a misaligned pup. (2) Puncture and infusion: Now using the stereotax, position the needle directly over bregma. In these infant rats, bregma may be quite difficult to see. Confirm that you have bregma
Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model
15
by very gently taking the needle down to the skull surface. Carefully applying some pressure with the needle (do not puncture the sagittal vein!) will depress the soft tissue between the cranial plates giving contour to bregma and the cranial sutures. Looking at the skull from a sideways view so that the light is reflected from the skull can help reveal these contours. Once bregma is confirmed, write down the anterior–posterior (AP) and medial–lateral (ML) coordinates as given by the stereotax. Remember, ventral–dorsal (VD) coordinates are not needed because of the standardized depth needle band. The coordinates relative to bregma for the NVHL surgery are AP –3.0 mm and ML ± 3.5 mm (VD –5.0 mm). Move the needle using the stereotactic mechanism posteriorly 3.0 mm from bregma. Observe that as you move it back, it traces the sagittal suture. If it does not appear to follow it in this fashion, then you have got an alignment problem that needs to be fixed as described under the previous step (mounting the rat). It does not matter which side of the rat’s head you lesion first as long as you are consistent. Using the stereotactic mechanism and coordinates, move the needle 3.5 mm from the midline toward the side you choose to do first. Now you are ready to penetrate the skull. First use the needle as a punch to make a hole in the targeted spot on the skull. Holding the rat’s head steady with one hand (gentle pressure with thumb and index finger on either side), make a quick and shallow jab with the needle using the stereotax with the other hand. Now, keeping one hand in support of the rat’s head, slowly re-advance the needle into the hole until the standardized depth needle band touches (and somewhat depresses) the rat’s skull. The most likely complication with needle insertion (and a major source of inaccuracy) is rotation of the rat’s head to one side; gentle head support with the fingers and a properly targeted needle should keep this from happening. Once the needle is in, you should notice that the pressure of the needle band has forced blood out of the venous sinuses of the sagittal and lambdoid sutures. Slowly raise the needle just until the blood refills these veins. Now that the needle is in place, the infusion pump may be started. It should pump in 0.3 μl of fluid over the course of 2 min and 15 s (135 s). Once the infusion is complete, let the needle rest in place in the rat’s head for about 3 min to prevent backflow. For this duration of needle placement, it is recommended that a stopwatch set to 5 min be used to time from the start of the infusion to when the needle should be withdrawn. After 5 min of total needle placement, we now lesion the other side of the brain. In our experience, this second infusion is
16
Chambers and Lipska
more likely to be off target than the first because the effects of pup motion and/or subtle misalignment, if present, are cumulative. Again, gently support the head with thumb and index finger of one hand, as this prevents cranial rotation during withdrawal of the needle. Still holding the head in this way after the needle is out, use the other hand to manipulate the stereotax so that the needle moves over 7.0 mm to the other side of the head (e.g., 3.5 mm to the other side of the sagittal suture). With good head support, the needle is now positioned above a spot that is symmetric with the AP and ML positions of the puncture hole on the contra-lateral side of the head. If when viewing the animal from directly above you do not see this symmetry, you may have to manually adjust the targeting just a bit (i.e., deviate from the exact coordinates) based on your line of sight only. This maneuver can be tricky but is sometimes necessary if inadvertent pup motion has occurred in prior steps, and it is often accurate with the eye of an experienced surgeon. Now, just as for the procedure on the other side of the head, and while providing head support, repeat the steps: punch and needle placement, timed infusion, and 3-min waiting period while the needle rests in the rat’s head. At the end of the second 3-min waiting period, the needle is withdrawn and the surgery is nearly complete. If during the surgery the rat begins to show movement (usually begins with back legs), put more pieces of ice over its back to maintain hypothermia. It is important to maintain immobility as much as possible during all phases of needle placement; movement of the head can make an IBO lesion too large or cause the SHAM surgery to make a mechanical lesion. After withdrawing the needle, carefully pull the tape off the rat. Take advantage of the remaining anesthetized state for using a mouse ear punch as needed to mark animals (one or two punches on either ear as NVHL, SHAM or probable failed attempt). It is best to close the wound after putting holes in the ears to prevent re-opening of the wound. Close the wound with a thin line of veterinary wound closure glue. (3) Post-operative recovery: Pups are placed in the post-operative tub for warming and monitoring. If a post-operative pup has not yet begun to stir, it should within the next 5 min. Animals receiving IBO may be observed to be more mobile compared to SHAM animals within this time frame. Occasionally, an IBO pup will die 5–10 min after becoming mobile, possibly in relation to improper ventricular spread of IBO that may impair respiration after recovery from hypothermia. Animals that die after showing little to no movement most likely had lethal hypothermia (an outcome
Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model
17
that can be 100% avoided with proper timing of anesthesia induction). Proactive attempts to resuscitate pups with diminished or lost respiration are ill advised, since it may indicate a bad lesion, or extra-lesion damage (e.g., cerebral hypoxia). If after 15 min, the pups are respiring normally, motorically responsive to touch, have normal pink coloration, and dry wound closure cement (if used), they are ready for return to their litters. Ordinarily, rats are returned to their original litters. However if culling or other sources of attrition have greatly reduced litter sizes, adoption to other litters is reasonable, as long as litters remain balanced and equal numbers of NVHLs and SHAMs are adopted. 3.3. Lesion Confirmation
Lesion confirmation is a necessary step for all rats in a given experiment (e.g., including both NVHL and SHAM-operated animals). Bilateral success rates for NVHLs may vary (even for the same surgeon) from 50 to 100% between cohorts, and occasionally a SHAM will have enough needle track damage to warrant exclusion. Rats may be sacrificed at any age for this purpose, but consistent with most experimental designs, we describe methods general to adult rats (>PD-56).
3.3.1. Histology
After sacrifice (by deep anesthesia and guillotine) and brain removal, the brain is placed whole into an isopentane-filled cylinder (kept between –30 and –40◦ C on dry ice), for 20–40 s. The rock-hard brain is then removed with forceps and wrapped in an appropriately sized (∼12 cm × 12 cm) and labeled square of aluminum foil. The brains may then be stored in a –80◦ C freezer until cutting. For sectioning, aim to collect 6–8 coronal sections on a cryostat through most of the rostral–caudal extent of the hippocampus. Generally, sections for slides should begin when the dorsal– rostral blade of the hippocampus comes into view (∼–2.0 mm AP bregma (5) as shown in Fig. 1.7). Sections are collected for slides about every 400 μm. We have used 20-μm-thick sections, in which every 18th–20th section is put on slides, or 40-μm sections, with every 9th–10th kept. Four to six sections are mounted on each appropriately labeled slide. Before staining, mounted slides may be stored in a slide rack for several days in a –20◦ C freezer. For staining, allow slides to warm from cold storage at room temperature for about 5 min. Then, process slides through the following 17 solution steps, while allowing the dipping rack to drain off between emersions. Notably, only the steps marked by (∗ ) require an actual solution dish preparation; steps 4 and 6–9 may reuse solutions 1–3.
18
Chambers and Lipska
Fig. 1.7. Appearance of dorsal blade of the hippocampus in coronal section; approximate area where first section for histological processing should be taken.
Dehydration (1) 70% ETOH (3 min)∗ (2) 95% ETOH (3 min)∗ (3) 100% ETOH (3 min)∗ (4) 100% ETOH (3 min) Fixing (5) 1:1 chloroform/100% ETOH (1 h to overnight) ∗ Rehydration (6) 100% ETOH (1–3 min) (7) 95% ETOH (1–3 min) (8) 95% ETOH (1–3 min) (9) 70% ETOH (1–3 min) (10) Calcite water (1–3 min)∗ (11) Calcite water (5 min)∗ Staining (12) Thionin solution (0.05%) (5–7 min)∗ (13) Calcite water∗ (14) 80% ETOH (1 min)∗ (15) 95% ETOH (1 min)∗ (16) 100% ETOH (1 min)∗ (17) Citrosolv (2 min)∗ (18) Coverslip with DPX (or equivalent)
Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model
3.3.2. Grading Lesions
19
Investigators should examine brains from all animals in a given experiment, blind to the results of the completed study. It is recommended that brains be examined under both very low (4×–10×; magnifying glass) and low magnification (40×; microscope) to allow close visualization of the entire brain and a more detailed exam of the cellular architecture of the hippocampus. Both lesioned and SHAM rats should be examined. Many SHAMs will normally show trace evidence of needle track damage – and that is permissible, unless there is pronounced evidence of tissue distortion or destruction, suggesting a mechanical lesion. The coordinates for the NVHL target the area where the CA3 layers of the dorsal and ventral blades merge [in adult sections, at about −5.0 mm from bregma (5)] (Fig. 1.8). Lesioned animals are typically characterized by one or more of the following: (a) generalized enlargement of the lateral ventricle; (b) tissue atrophy and/or distortion of normally curvaceous cell layers in the ventral hippocampus (involving CA1, CA2, CA3, and subiculum) and/or the CA3 of the ventral extent of the dorsal hippocampus; (c) loss and disarray of individual neurons (e.g., decrease in thionin staining density in cell layers) within cell layers. Between subjects, it is to be expected (and is permissible) that lesion damage within the hippocampus is somewhat heterogeneous in size and exact location, much as histopathological studies of brains of schizophrenia subjects show within-group heterogeneity of morphological findings. However, the following parameters should also be used to disqualify rats and limit this heterogeneity. (1) Lesion size: Lesions can be too small or too large, and either extreme may alter the behavioral phenotype (6). Require evidence for IBO damage (not just needle tracks) in at least two serial sections. Conversely, the lesion should not be a total hippocampectomy or involve significant damage to the dorsal blade of the hippocampus as this structure has differential connectivity patterns and functionality compared to the ventral (more limbic) hippocampus (7). This outcome is easily ruled out by ensur-
Sham (normal)
IBO
Fig. 1.8. Primary target areas (cross hairs) of coordinates for NVHLs (left) and typical zone of tissue loss with a “perfect” bilateral hit in an NVHL (right).
20
Chambers and Lipska
ing lack of extension of the lesion into the dorsal blade on the first section in the series. (2) Lesion location: The damage should be to the hippocampus proper; significant damage evident to the overlying cortex, underlying thalamus, or ventrally posited amygdala should be inspected for, and warrants disqualification. The NVHL lesion does cause some degree of generalized atrophy to its efferent/afferent structures (e.g., prefrontal cortex, temporal/entorhinal cortex, amygdala, dorsal hippocampus, and likely elsewhere) and so if noted on exam, such trends are not grounds for disqualification unless there is evidence of significant cell loss and/or gliosis in these structures under 40×. (3) Bi-laterality: Small lesions on one side and large on the other are not infrequent and are permissible as long as the lesions on each side meet the above criteria. However, a lack of bilateral lesions is an exclusion, representing either a lack of enough infusion volume on one side or an extrahippocampal infusion that may be hard to find.
SHAM
NVHL
Fig. 1.9. Example of published demonstration of serial maps showing smallest and largest extent of lesion damage in a study (8) and exemplary micrographs of SHAM vs. NVHL, with permission from Springer.
Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model
21
For purposes of publication, it is typical for researchers to show anatomical maps of largest vs. smallest lesion extents of rats with appropriate lesions (e.g., from among all those not disqualified from a given study) along with a representative micrograph of a SHAM and NVHL brain (e.g., Fig. 1.9).
4. Notes: Problems and Pitfalls 4.1. Making Ibotenic Acid
4.2. Practice Lesioning
This obviously crucial step, if not done carefully and correctly, can cost serious time and money since histological lesion results are typically not assayed until well after rats reach adulthood (PD-56) and experiments are completed. The addition of NaOH for putting IBO into solution is a delicate matter and must proceed in a gradual and deliberate titration with frequent pH checks. The danger is that the pH can suddenly shoot higher than 8.5 from a prior reading of 25%, “bad acid” is a likely culprit, and a new batch should be mixed. To obtain a reasonable level of skill and accuracy, it is recommended that surgeons inexperienced with NVHLs initially practice lesions on 50–100 pups. Learning curves are more rapid if this is done in batches of 1–10 rats, with meticulous intra-surgery notes taken on each rat and batch sacrifices for lesion verification conducted within 24 h, if not immediately after a surgery. Keep a record of improving bilateral success rates vs. unilateral hits and total misses. Two major alterations to the general methods are important for practice lesioning: first, use Chicago Sky Blue (50 mg in 20 ml of aCSF) (or equivalent stain) as the infusate. Second, the histological processing involves sacrifice of rat pups [e.g., with lethal (>25 min) hypothermia, brain removal, isopentane-rapid freezing, and cutting on a cryostat only]. The lesion verification is done by visual inspection (with magnifying glass) of dye delivery while the brain is being sectioned on the cryostat – there is no need for slides or histology. Of note, the PD-7 rat brain and hippocampus do differ architecturally somewhat from the adult. After histology, the adult hippocampus appears to have two independent lobes: one dorsal that comes into view first (coronal sections cut caudally) and then a ventral that appears independently, then merges with the dorsal. In the pup, these parts, appearing darker than overlying cortex, seem more contiguous regardless of which section you are looking at. A good bilateral hit vs. common patterns of missed hits are shown in Fig. 1.10. In general, the key will be achieving consistent bilateral
22
Chambers and Lipska
Bilateral Success
No
No
No
Fig. 1.10. Patterns of success and typical failures in practice lesioning using dye.
symmetry, alignment, and infusion depth. Create small cartoons for each result, compare with written notes, and adjust your technique accordingly. 4.3. SHAM Controls
It is debatable whether SHAM lesions are the best control for all studies. Mechanical damage or other complications of introducing a needle into the brain are likely to have at least some subtle long-term effect consistent with a mild lesion, potentially decreasing effect size in some NVHL vs. SHAM lesion comparisons. Although early piloting studies and some published results have shown little effect of SHAM–NVHL lesions compared to unoperated controls in a limited set of phenotypic measures, SHAM lesions of the neonatal medial prefrontal cortex do show trends approaching significance across several phenotypic measures compared to unoperated controls (9). Therefore, depending on the experiment and experimental justification, one or more alternative controls may also be used (e.g., unoperated pups not removed from litters; pups undergoing hypothermic anesthesia; and superficial wound opening only).
4.4. Cautions About Alternative Approaches
In developing or learning the methods for producing NVHLs, it may be tempting to try alternative modifications to the procedures. Here, we mention a few of these that we would advise caution in pursuing:
Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model
23
Anesthesia: Although alternative forms of anesthesia (e.g., pharmacological) other than hypothermia have been used for NVHL surgery, we generally advise against these approaches. In our experience, hypothermia, when properly done, is safer, more predictable, cheaper, and cleaner. Using it arguably maintains the model as a more purely non-pharmacological model of schizophrenia, while other anesthetics if used this early in the developmental stage of the animal may have longterm effects and have themselves been used to produce models of schizophrenia (e.g., ketamine). Head stabilization: It may be tempting to create an apparatus for allowing greater head stabilization as an attempt to reduce the amount of practice and skill needed to make accurate lesions. In our experience, such attempts have amounted to wild-goose chases. While some groups appear to have successfully made form-fitting molds for stabilizing the rat’s body and head in the operative platform, we recommend against the use of metal probes, clamps, or ear bar-like instruments. The pup’s skull is simply too soft, flexible, and rotatable under the skin. Wound closure: The wound may of course be closed with wound closure glue, clips, or sutures. However, the latter unnecessarily adds time to the surgery and is susceptible to wound re-opening after return to litters. Mothers may also meddle with sutures and especially clips. Placing clips involves additional risk of injuring the pup’s skull, and they need to be removed a week or two later which unnecessarily distresses the animals. The only caution with glue is to make sure it is dry before returning pups to their litters.
Acknowledgments Compilation of this chapter was supported by NIDA K08 DA019850 (R.A.C.). The authors wish to thank Alena Sentir for her effort in rendering the original drawings into digital form. References 1. Lipska BK, Jaskiw GE, Weinberger DR. (1993) Post-pubertal emergence of hyperresponsiveness to stress and to amphetamine after neonatal excitotoxic hippocampal damage: a potential animal model of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 9: 67–75.
2. Tseng KY, Chambers RA, Lipska BK. (2009) The neonatal ventral hippocampal lesion as a heuristic neurodevelopmental animal model of schizophrenia. Behav Brain Res 204: 295–305. 3. O’Donnell P, Lewis BL, Weinberger DR, Lipska B. (2002) Neonatal hippocampal
24
Chambers and Lipska
damage alters electrophysiological properties of prefrontal cortical neurons in adult rats. Cereb Cortex 12:975–982. 4. Chambers RA. (2007) Animal modeling and neurocircuitry of dual diagnosis. J Dual Diagn 3:19–29. 5. Swanson LW. (2004) Brain maps: structure of the rat brain. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Elsevier. 6. Swerdlow NR, Halim N, Hanlon FM, Platten A, Auerbach PP. (2001) Lesion size and amphetamine hyperlocomotion after neonatal ventral hippocampal lesions: more is less. Brain Res Bull 55: 71–77.
7. Moser MB, Moser EI. (1998) Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8:608–619. 8. Chambers RA, Jones RM, Brown S, Taylor JR. (2005) Natural reward related learning in rats with neonatal ventral hippocampal lesions and prior cocaine exposure. Psychopharmacology 179: 470–478. 9. Schneider M, Koch M. (2005) Behavioral and morphological alterations following neonatal excitotoxic lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex in rats. Exp Neurol 195:185–198.
Chapter 2 Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol) Administration: A Promising Animal Model for Psychosis Onset Gwenaëlle Le Pen, Alfredo Bellon, Marie-Odile Krebs, and Thérèse M. Jay Abstract This chapter provides an overview on exposure to methylazoxymethanol (MAM) at embryonic day 17 as a promising animal model for schizophrenia that mimics behavioral abnormalities and deficits in prefrontal cortex networks. This early insult produces in adult offspring from E17 MAM-treated dams the following: (1) behavioral changes including spontaneous hyperactivity and hypersensitivity to psychotomimetic drugs that are reminiscent of positive symptoms of schizophrenia and associated with a temporal pattern of expression; (2) impaired social interaction similar to that observed in schizophrenic patients existing prior to the onset of disease; (3) cognitive deficits in a variety of domain: working and reference memory, behavioral flexibility, attentional functioning, object recognition, and reversal learning; (4) behavioral abnormalities resembling schizophrenia-related endophenotypes like deficient sensorimotor gating and disrupted latent inhibition; (5) anatomical changes with cortical, thalamic, and hippocampal reductions in volume that are associated with an enlargement of the lateral ventricles; and (6) abnormalities in functional connectivity between brain areas that involve deficits in dopamine, glutamate, and GABA systems. The E17 MAM model that incorporates neuropathological and functional manifestations associated with schizophrenia may help forward early preventive interventions that can successfully reduce the risk of developing schizophrenia in exposed individuals. Key words: MAM, animal model, schizophrenia, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, schizophreniarelated endophenotypes, behavioral and cognitive deficits, anatomical and functional abnormalities.
1. Introduction Schizophrenia is one of the most disabling and emotionally devastating illnesses of the brain. It is characterized by a constellation of distinctive symptoms that can be divided into three groups: (1) positive symptoms, such as delusions, hallucinations, paranoia, P. O’Donnell (ed.), Animal Models of Schizophrenia and Related Disorders, Neuromethods 59, DOI 10.1007/978-1-61779-157-4_2, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
25
26
Le Pen et al.
and psychosis, (2) persistent negative symptoms, such as flattened affect, impaired attention, social withdrawal, and (3) cognitive impairments (1). Despite a hundred years’ research, the neuropathology of schizophrenia remains obscure. To better understand its pathogenesis, animal models are important tools in experimental medical science. Schizophrenia can be modeled in animals using standardized procedures that recreate specific pathogenic events and their behavioral outcomes. Availability of suitable animal models could further allow analysis of the mechanisms that underlie the etiology of the disease and may facilitate the development of novel therapeutic strategies. Nevertheless, an animal model that can perfectly replicate all aspects of a human disease such as schizophrenia does not exist. Despite the fact that the etiological origin of schizophrenia is still unclear, there is increasing evidence suggesting that early insults during brain development are associated with an increased risk for schizophrenia (2, 3). In particular, postmortem studies have revealed reduced volume, decreased neuronal size, and loss and/or malpositioning of cells in the hippocampus and prefrontal, entorhinal, and cingulate cortices of schizophrenic patients (2). Epidemiological studies and the observation of minor physical (4) and dermatoglyphic abnormalities (5) also suggest that the second trimester of pregnancy could be a “critical window of vulnerability” during which the fetus may be placed at a higher risk for schizophrenia (4, 5). This period of time in the developing human brain coincides with a massive neuronal migration of cells to the cortex (6). In addition, it has been hypothesized that the interaction between hippocampus and prefrontal cortex might be particularly disturbed in the disorder (7, 8). This so-called disconnection hypothesis (9) is also attractive since the hippocampal formation (HF) is selectively vulnerable to some obstetrical insults (10), and a disturbed interaction with the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex would thus offer an explanation of epidemiological data linking schizophrenia to early neurodevelopmental disturbances (11). Consistent with this notion, several neuroimaging studies have reported altered functional integration between prefrontal and hippocampal regions in schizophrenia (8, 12–15). Hence, an animal model based on neurodevelopmental defects in the hippocampus and the cortex has been proposed as offering some degree of construct validity for schizophrenia. The use of a mitotoxin administered prenatally and the knowledge of a precise neurogenic timetable (16, 17) has allowed targeting neuronal ablations in specific brain areas of the central nervous system when administered at different gestational days. To this end, brain cellular proliferation can be briefly interrupted with the mitotoxin methylazoxymethanol (MAM), administered
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
27
during late gestation at embryonic day 17 (E17), when neurons that will migrate to the hippocampus and the neocortex are undergoing major cell division (18). Because of its unique characteristics, MAM has long been used to induce developmental brain dysfunction in rodents and to study postnatal behavioral effects of a prenatal administration (17, 19). MAM is an antimitotic agent (20–23) that, in vivo, is rapidly converted to methyldiazonium, damaging DNA through the methylation of guanine nucleic acids at O6 or N7 positions (24). In addition, MAM has specificity for actively dividing neurons, leaving other cell types and differentiated neurons relatively unaltered (25, 26). Indeed, actively dividing neuroepithelial cells during the S phase are affected by MAM, whereas post-mitotic neurons or neuroblasts in the G0 phase are spared (27). Furthermore, MAM is able to readily cross the placental barrier (26, 28). The narrow time window of biological activity of MAM in fetal tissue remains for a limited period of 2–24 h, after intraperitoneal administration to the pregnant dam, with a peak activity at 12 h that allows targeting the proliferation of specific neuronal cell populations (24, 29). While the hypotheses taken into account to develop the E17 MAM model seem to endow the model with construct validity for some aspects of schizophrenia, there is now converging evidence supporting that exposure to MAM at E17 has also reached a certain degree of face validity. Indeed, E17 MAM-exposed animals exhibit behavioral changes that mimic some aspects of the positive, negative, and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia (30–37). In addition, the diachronic pattern of the phenotype (i.e., peripubertal pattern of behavioral abnormalities), shown in MAMexposed animals, is strikingly similar to that seen in schizophrenia (34–36). Finally, concerning the face validity of the model, E17 MAM-exposed animals also exhibit neurochemical and neurophysiological abnormalities reminiscent of those observed in schizophrenic patients (32, 37–42).
2. Behavioral Changes Observed in E17 MAM-Exposed Rats 2.1. Behavioral Abnormalities Reminiscent of the Positive Symptoms of Schizophrenia
In rodents, both locomotor hyperactivity and stereotyped or perseverative behaviors are thought to reflect hypersensitivity of the subcortical dopamine system (43, 44). In humans, an excess of subcortical dopamine has been associated with delusions and hallucinations (45–47). Then, novelty-induced hyperactivity has been viewed as a preclinical model of positive symptoms of schizophrenia and of psychomotor agitation in particular (48). In the same line, both locomotor hyperactivity and striking stereotyped or perseverative behaviors induced by dopaminergic
28
Le Pen et al.
receptor agonists and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonists, at high doses, were shown to each have various degrees of face validity for behaviors that are reminiscent of changes observed in schizophrenia. In addition, these agents were also shown to exacerbate psychotic symptoms in schizophrenic patients, suggesting that dopaminergic and glutamatergic neurotransmission may be critical in schizophrenia (49–56). Thus, to investigate if MAM-exposed animals exhibit behaviors that could be reminiscent of positive symptoms of schizophrenia, hypersensitivity to novelty, to dopaminergic receptor agonists, and to the locomotor-activating effects of NMDA receptor antagonists as well as the stereotyped or perseverative induced behaviors have been evaluated. Spontaneous locomotor activity measured in MAM-exposed rats revealed discrepancies that could be related to differences in either the strain reactivity or the experimental procedure. In Sprague Dawley rats, MAM (25 mg/kg) induces a robust and persistent spontaneous hyperactivity from postnatal day 63 to 115 in various behavioral tests (open field, social interaction, Y maze) (34, 35). In addition, we showed that the increase in spontaneous locomotion was present only in the adult MAM-exposed rats (Fig. 2.1), suggesting that this behavioral feature reflects the interaction of early developmental disturbances with adolescent brain maturation (34, 35).
Total locomotor activity (cm) × 1000
2.1.1. Spontaneous Locomotor Activity
Sham MAM 12 10 8 6 4 2 0
NS
Pre-puberty
***
Post-puberty
Fig. 2.1. Spontaneous locomotor activity in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. Total distance (±SEM) travelled by sham and MAM-exposed rats during a 60-min testing period in the open field was measured at pre- and post-puberty. ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 vs. sham. (Adapted from Le Pen et al., 2006)
In contrast to these results, data obtained in Wistar rats with 22 mg/kg E17 MAM exposure are more inconsistent. Indeed, in a large open field, Flagstad et al. (32) did not observe any modification of locomotor activity level in MAM-exposed rats in comparison to sham animals, whereas, in the same arena, MAMexposed rats exhibited increased spontaneous locomotor activity when tested in the social interaction test. These discrepancies could be due to a different strain of rats or to the experimental
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
29
conditions (lighting during light (Sprague Dawley rats) vs. dark (Wistar rats) period of the light-dark cycle, MAM dose). 2.1.2. Dopaminergic Receptor Agonist-Induced Hyperactivity
Two different research groups have conducted experiments to investigate amphetamine-induced hyperactivity in animals preexposed to MAM (22 mg/kg) at E17 (32, 36) during the dark period of the light-dark cycle. These two studies performed in two different strains of rats (Wistar and Fisher 344) both revealed that MAM-exposed rats were hypersensitive to the locomotoractivating effects of D-amphetamine (Wistar: 2 mg/kg s.c. measured as salt 90 min and Fisher 344: 0.5 mg/kg i.p. 60 min). In addition, distinct experiments performed before and after puberty (Fig. 2.2) showed that hypersensitivity to amphetamine emerged only in adult animals (36). 600
Sham MAM
*
Ambulation distance (cm)
500
400
300
200
100
0
Pre-Puberty
Post-Puberty
Fig. 2.2. Amphetamine-evoked locomotor activity in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. salineexposed rats. Total distance (±SEM) travelled by sham and MAM-exposed rats during a 60-min testing period in the open field after amphetamine (0.5 mg/kg, i.p.) was measured at pre- and post-puberty. ∗ p < 0.05 vs. sham. (Adapted from Moore et al., 2006)
2.1.3. Glutamatergic NMDA Antagonist-Induced Hyperactivity and Stereotyped Behaviors 2.1.3.1. PCP
Hypersensitivity to PCP-induced hyperlocomotion in an open field has been shown at adulthood, in two strains of rats (Wistar and Fisher 344) pre-exposed to MAM (22 mg/kg) (36, 37) after administration of an acute s.c. (120 min) (Fig. 2.3) or i.p. (60 min) dose of 5 mg/kg. In addition, it has been shown that the increase of PCP-induced ataxia in the open field was similar in sham and MAM-exposed rats (Fig. 2.4) (36). Orofacial dyskinesia and stereotypies (vacuous chewing and biting stereotypies) induced either spontaneously or by PCP (2.5 and 5 mg/kg i.p.) were also investigated in adult MAM-exposed rats (36). Oral behaviors were manually scored by direct observation
Le Pen et al. 500
Sham MAM
Crossings
400
300
200
100
0 15
30
45 60 75 90 105 120 Time (minutes)
Fig. 2.3. Phencyclidine-evoked locomotor activity in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. salineexposed rats. Total distance (±SEM) travelled by sham and MAM-exposed rats during a 120-min testing period in the open field after phencyclidine (5 mg/kg, s.c.) was measured at adulthood. (Adapted from Penschuck et al., 2006)
2
Ataxia score
30
Sham MAM
1
0
0
5 PCP dose (mg/kg)
Fig. 2.4. Phencyclidine-evoked ataxia in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. Ataxia scores were measured in sham and MAM-exposed adult rats during a 60-min testing period after phencyclidine (5 mg/kg, i.p.) at adulthood. (Adapted from Moore et al., 2006)
(by an observer blind to gestational and drug conditions) and defined as follows: tremor, rapid oscillation of facial muscles progressing from lower jaw through cheek to extra-ocular region, stereotypic mouthing of the tail, and repetitive approaches to the tail with the mouth featuring horizontal and vertical motions of the jaw (follow-through biting rarely occurred). These behaviors were scored every 5 min during a 60-min period as absent [0], present/intermittent [1], or continuous [2]. MAM-exposed animals displayed a significantly higher frequency of spontaneous orofacial dyskinesias and stereotypies; moreover, these
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol) Vacuous chewing
*
Bouts per 5 min
*
4
*
2 1 0
*
6
Sham MAM
5
3
Biting stereotypy
5
Bouts per 5 min
6
31
*
4 3 2
*
1 0
0.25 5 PCP dose (mg/kg)
0
0
0.25 5 PCP dose (mg/kg)
Fig. 2.5. Phencyclidine-evoked orofacial dyskinesias in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. salineexposed rats. Vacuous chewing (left panel) and biting stereotypy (right panel) were measured following injection of saline or phencyclidine (PCP 2.5, 5.0 mg/kg, i.p.) at adulthood. ∗ p < 0.05 vs. sham. (Adapted from Moore et al., 2006)
behaviors were significantly more sensitive to exacerbation by PCP (Fig. 2.5) (36). 2.1.3.2. MK-801
The sensitivity to (+)-MK-801 (0.05 and 0.1 mg/kg) has been investigated in E17 MAM-exposed (25 mg/kg) Sprague Dawley rats at both pre- and post-puberty in different groups of rats (PD35 and PD115, respectively) to avoid multi-drug exposure (Fig. 2.6). Before MK-801 testing, animals were subjected to a 1-h session of habituation in the locomotor activity apparatus (open field: 40 cm × 40 cm), followed by another 1-h period of locomotor activity after a saline injection. Whereas both sham and MAM-exposed rats exhibited a similar response to the locomotoractivating effects of MK-801 before puberty, MAM-exposed rats developed hypersensitivity to the locomotor-activating effect of MK-801 in adulthood (35).
2.2. Behavioral Anomalies Reminiscent of Negative Symptoms of Schizophrenia
Social withdrawal is a frequent negative symptom of schizophrenia (1, 3). It exists prior to the onset of disease and is predictive of future schizophrenia (57). In rodents, social behavior can be easily evaluated using a social interaction test. This test consists in allowing the experimental subject to freely explore an unfamiliar congener in its home cage or in a neutral environment. Social exploration is measured in time spent by the experimental subject around the congener.
2.2.1. Procedure
The general design of the social interaction test used to evaluate MAM-exposed animals was adapted from the work of SamsDodd (58). The test was performed in an open arena (80 cm × 80 cm, 50 cm high) that was placed in a dimly lit room. Two unfamiliar rats that had approximately the same weight and had received identical prenatal treatment were placed simultaneously
Le Pen et al.
a-Pre-Puberty
4
Saline
5
Habituation
Horizontal locomotor activity (cm) × 1000
6
MK-801
Sham/MK-801 0.05 mg/kg MAM/MK-801 0.05 mg/kg Sham/MK-801 0.1 mg/kg MAM/MK-801 0.1 mg/kg
7
3 2 1
23 23 0
0
21 21 0
0
19 19 0
0
0 17 17 0
0 15
0 13
0 11
90
70
50
30
10
0 Time period (min)
b-Post-Puberty Sham/MK-801 0.05 mg/kg MAM/MK-801 0.05 mg/kg Sham/MK-801 0.1 mg/kg MAM/MK-801 0.1 mg/kg
7
4
Saline
5
MK-801
6
Habituation
Horizontal locomotor activity (cm) × 1000
3 2 1
15 0
13 0
11 0
90
70
50
30
0 10
32
Time period (min)
Fig. 2.6. (+) MK-801-evoked locomotor activity in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. salineexposed rats. The distance (±SEM) travelled by sham and MAM-exposed rats was explored at both (a) pre- and (b) post-puberty in a spontaneous exploration session, after saline injection and after (+)-MK-801 (0.05 or 0.1 mg/kg s.c.) administration. (Adapted from Le pen et al., 2006)
in the opposite corners of the arena and the track of each rat was recorded for a 10-min period with a video tracking system. Social interactions were measured by the time spent and number of contacts at a distance lower than 20 cm from each other. During the
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
33
10-min period of the test, locomotor activity and time spent in the central zone were also recorded. In Wistar and Sprague Dawley rats tested during the dark and light phase of the light-dark cycle, respectively, evaluation of social behavior produced similar results. Indeed, deficits in social interaction revealed by a decreased amount of time spent in contact with the congener were first shown in adult Wistar rats exposed to MAM at E17 (32). We extended these results (Fig. 2.7) by showing that Sprague Dawley MAM-exposed rats display these deficits at both pre-puberty and adulthood (34, 35). The deficits could be dissociated from motor performances, given that hyperactivity emerged only after puberty (34, 35) and were not the result of anxiogenic-like behaviors since the time spent in the central zone was similar in both groups (32, 34, 35). In addition, this abnormal behavior appeared to be the consequence of a lower duration of contacts and not a difference in the number of contacts (32, 34, 35). Sham
MAM
a 400
60
350 300
*** ***
250 200 0
Pre-Puberty
Post-Puberty
Number of contact
Time spent (s) in social interaction
2.2.2. Results
b
50 40 30 20 10 0
Pre-Puberty
Post-Puberty
Fig. 2.7. Social interactions in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. The time spent (±SEM) in interaction (a) and the number of contacts (b) between two rats from the same treatment group were explored at pre- and post-puberty. ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 vs. sham. (Adapted from Le pen et al., 2006)
2.3. Abnormalities Reminiscent of the Cognitive Symptoms of Schizophrenia
Cognitive impairments constitute a central feature of schizophrenia (59) that include deficits in abstraction, executive function, verbal memory, language function, vigilance, and attention (60–62). Schizophrenic patients are also commonly described with abnormalities in information processing (63–65). For some authors, sensorimotor gating deficits correlate highly with measures of perceptual and reasoning disturbances. This relationship may then form an important basis for cognitive dysfunction observed among schizophrenic patients (66). Thus, an important future direction of preclinical research relating to schizophrenia is the design of animal models and novel treatments that target cognitive dysfunctions associated with this disorder. It is possible to design behavioral paradigms in rodents that can evaluate cognitive constructs “comparable” with those measured in many human experimental paradigms.
34
Le Pen et al.
2.3.1. Working Memory
Working memory may be defined as the capacity to hold information online over short delays while that information is integrated with other ongoing mental operations. A number of studies have shown that schizophrenic patients have deficient working memory (67–69), and imaging studies have confirmed that these deficits in working memory are associated with abnormal activation of the prefrontal cortex (70). Although working memory as measured in rodent might differ from that in human because it is explored for a longer delay, performance in working memory has been investigated in MAM-exposed rats using various paradigms.
2.3.1.1. Radial Arm Maze
In the rat, interactive communication between the hippocampus and the medial prefrontal cortex is necessary to perform a spatial working memory task. Thus, a delay-interposed radial maze learning task, previously shown to be mediated by a distributed neural network linking the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex (71), has been used to investigate working memory in MAM-exposed rats (33). The radial arm maze task takes advantage of the natural tendency of food-deprived rodents to learn and remember different spatial locations for food in an eight-arm radial maze.
Procedure
The eight-arm radial maze used to test working memory in MAM-exposed rats consisted of a central arena (30 cm diameter) connected to eight equally spaced arms (80 cm × 13 cm) that extended radially. The maze was elevated 60 cm above the floor and surrounded by extra-maze three-dimensional cues in an illuminated room, with music on for background noise. The working memory task was adapted from Packard et al. (72) and Floresco et al. (73). After a 3-day period of habituation to the maze environment, adult food-deprived animals were trained. Each trial consisted of a training and a test phase, separated by a delay. Every day, a random set of four arms (the same for all animals in a given day) was blocked. Food pellets were placed in cups at the end of the remaining four open arms. Each rat was given 5 min to retrieve the pellets and then was replaced in its home cage for the delay period. Then, during the test phase, all arms were open, but food was placed only in the arms previously blocked. Rats were allowed a maximum of 5 min to retrieve the four pellets (Fig. 2.8). The initial delay between training and test phases was 5 min. The learning curves (5-min trials) were compared two by two between groups by means of the log rank test. After achieving criterion performance (i.e., all four pellets retrieved in four or five choices during the test phase for 2 consecutive days), the delay was increased to 30 min. On 30-min delay test days, we recorded the number and order of arm entries. Entries into non-baited arms during the test phase were considered as errors. We distinguished across-phase errors (entries into an arm entered
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
35
Delay
Fig. 2.8. Diagram of the radial arm maze task with delay. Food pellets (plain circle) were placed in a well at the tip of each maze arm. (Adapted from Gourevitch et al., 2004)
previously during the training phase) from within-phase errors (re-entries into an arm entered earlier during the test phase). Results
E17 MAM-exposed rats were able to reach the level of performance of control rats at the initial stages of training as revealed by similar number of 5-min trials necessary to learn the rule in both sham and MAM-exposed animals. Nevertheless, with a 30-min delay, MAM-exposed rats were unable to process and retrieve spatial information when a 30-min delay was interposed, indicating a significant impairment in working memory. Indeed, MAM-exposed rats made more total errors than did sham animals (Fig. 2.9). These errors were mostly related to working memory (entries during the test phase to an arm entered previously during the training phase), while errors related to visual short-term memory (re-entries into an arm entered earlier during the test phase) were similar in MAM-exposed rats compared to sham animals.
2.3.1.2. Spontaneous Alternation
The Y-maze spontaneous alternation procedure used to assess spatial working memory in MAM-exposed rats (34) is based on their natural tendency to explore novel environment. When placed in the Y maze, normal rats prefer to explore the least recently visited arm and thus tend to alternate visits between the three arms. To explore the three arms successively, the rat must maintain an ongoing record of most recently visited arms and continuously update such records. A rodent with impaired working memory cannot remember which arm it just visited and thus shows decreased spontaneous alternation (74, 75).
Procedure
The Y maze used for MAM-exposed rats has three identical arms (40 cm × 9 cm × 15 cm with walls 30 cm high) placed at 120◦ from each other. Numerous visual cues (geometrical forms) were placed on the ceiling and kept constant during the experiments. Rats were placed at the end of one arm of the Y maze and the sequence of arm entries was recorded during 10 min. The introduction arm was randomly assigned for each rat. An arm visit was recorded when a rat moved all four paws into the arm. An alternation was defined as consecutive entries into all three arms (e.g., 1, 2, 3 or 1, 3, 2). The number of maximum alternations was the total number of arm entries minus 2 and the percentage of
36
Le Pen et al.
Working memory errors
a
b
4
Sham MAM
*
3 2
**
1 0
5
Across-phase errors
Within-phase errors
Total errors
Sham MAM
Total errors
4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Sessions
Fig. 2.9. Working memory performance in a delay-interposed, eight-arm radial maze task in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. (a) E17 MAM-exposed animals made more total errors than did controls, and (b) these errors were mostly across-phase errors, whereas within-phase errors were not significantly different in treated and control rats (∗ p < 0.05; ∗∗ p < 0.01). (Adapted from Gourevitch et al., 2004)
alternations was calculated as the ratio of actual to maximum alternations multiplied by 100: (actual alternations/maximum alternations) × 100. Persevering behavior was defined as subjects making significantly fewer alternations than would be expected by chance (50%). Results
Deficits in alternation were present at both pre- and post-puberty in MAM-exposed rats compared to sham animals (Fig. 2.10). There was an improvement across puberty in both groups. Low performances before puberty in both sham and MAMexposed rats were linked to a low exploratory behavior that could reflect anxiety-like behaviors in both sham and MAM-exposed young rats (34). The deficit in alternation indicates a deficit in flexibility and short-term memory, further supporting that E17 MAM exposure impairs the hippocampo-prefrontal networks (76). Nevertheless, spontaneous alternation results also suggest that the working memory skills used in the spatial alternation test were acquired during puberty and that maturation of processes involved in that behavior is not affected by the MAM treatment, since the deficits in MAM-exposed rats are already seen before puberty (34).
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
80
#
70
% Alternation
60
*
37
Sham MAM
#
*
50 40 30 20 10 0
Pre-Puberty
Post-puberty
Fig. 2.10. Spontaneous alternation in prenatal E17 MAM-exposed vs. saline-exposed rats. Percentage of alternation (±SEM) at pre- and post-puberty. The dotted line represents the chance level (50%), # different from the chance level p < 0.001. ∗ p < 0.05 vs. sham group.
2.3.1.3. Delayed Non-match to Position
The delayed non-match-to-position task measures the ability of subjects to learn a rule in which they have to associate the position of a stimulus previously presented and an action for getting a reward. Flagstad and colleagues (31) have investigated working memory in MAM-exposed rats using this paradigm.
Procedure
In the procedure used by Flagstad and colleagues (31), in the first step, rats have to learn to press a lever to initiate the delivery of water (here rats were water deprived and water was used as the positive reinforcer). In the second step, the non-match-toposition (NMTP) training schedule, at the start of each trial, one of the two retractable levers is presented to the subject into the operant chamber. The subject has to press the lever for indicating that the sample has been registered. Then both levers are presented and the subject has to press the sample lever at the opposite of the one presented before when the rat received the reward. In the last step, the procedure was as in the NMTP experiment, except that a delay (0, 5, or 10 s, random) was inserted after the initial press on the right or left lever before the reward was given. The animals were initially trained for 25 sessions in this procedure including the delay and the subsequent three sessions were used as the test sessions.
Results
The investigation of working memory using the NMTP paradigm showed no consistent difference between the performances of the sham and MAM-exposed rats, either in the non-delay condition or in the delay condition (Fig. 2.11). The sham animals were superior only in four sessions out of 40 during the learning performance of the NMTP paradigm, and this did not appear to be a consistent effect (31).
Le Pen et al.
Percent correct responses
100
a-Zero delay
90 80
*
*
*
*
70 60 50 40 0 100
Percent correct responses
38
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
b-Delay 0 – 10
90 80 70 60 Sham MAM
50 40 0
5 10
0
5 10 Delay
0
5 10
Fig. 2.11. Working memory performance in a delay non-match-to-position (NMTP) task in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. (a) The mean percent correct responses (±SEM) of the groups during the 40 days of acquisition of the NMTP paradigm. ∗ p < 0.05 vs. sham. (b) The mean percent correct responses (±SEM) during three consecutive days of DNMTP. (Adapted from Flagstad et al., 2004)
In the delayed non-match-to-position procedure, a delaydependent decline was apparent, but no differences between groups were observed. The accuracy of the performance in the task is believed to be dependent on prefrontal cortical integrity, as lesions decrease performance in the task, although the published data suggest that the deficits of the prefrontal cortex-lesioned rats are apparent only at the beginning of training (77, 78). In Flagstad’s work (31), the animals were first trained on zero delay and then after an extensive training switched to the delay condition. The authors hypothesized that under the present protocol, the animals use a movement-mediated strategy, that is, the choice of the rat after the delay is mediated from the body position of the animal rather than by information kept online in working memory. Thus, this could possibly explain the lack of effect of the MAM treatment. During development, passively holding information available in working memory as well as recognition and recall from long-term memory is developed before active use
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
39
and manipulations of information (executive functions), and as such not as dependent on later maturation of the prefrontal cortex (79). This might suggest that disturbances in prefrontal functions in MAM-exposed rats are related to a later maturation of this structure, and hence less radical than a dysfunction of all processes governed by the prefrontal cortex. 2.3.1.4. Morris Water Maze
Short-term place memory, or spatial working memory, has been shown to be impaired in schizophrenia (80, 81). In rodents, a typical experimental working memory paradigm is the Morris water maze (MWM) in which rats search for a submerged platform whose position is changed daily in a circular pool filled with water (82). The water maze is a particularly useful tool for assessment of spatial memory ability in rats. Indeed, the motivating stimulus, escape from water, does not require the food or water deprivation that is common in appetitive tasks like the radial arm maze or T maze. Flagstad and colleagues (31) have used the Morris water maze procedure to evaluate spatial working memory in MAMexposed rats.
Procedure
The water maze consisted of a circular, black pool filled with water. The test room contained several permanent extra-maze cues such as the rat housing rack, laboratory table, posters on the walls, etc. In the working memory task, the platform was placed in a different quadrant every day (north-south-west-east) for 4 days. Before each trial, the rat was placed on the hidden platform for 15 s (inter-trial period). The initial placement on the platform each day served to reveal the new location of the platform. If the rat did not locate the platform within 60 s, the rat was gently placed on the platform. Key measures were path length (m), escape latency (s) to find the hidden platform, percent of path length near (10 cm) the sidewall (percent sidewall), and number of animals not finding the platform within 60 s (non-finders). These measures were recorded using a video tracking system.
Results
Flagstad et al. (31) showed that MAM-exposed rats performed significantly worse than controls on days 2 and 3 of testing but only on the basis of the percent sidewall and non-finder parameters. On the last day, however, they performed as well as the controls (Fig. 2.12). They concluded that the MAM treatment does not interfere with spatial working memory performance. Nevertheless, this result has to be confirmed since sham animals strangely performed worse on the last day of the test than on the penultimate day. The MAM treatment seems, however, to affect the search strategy of the rats. When the platform is moved, the rats spend more time swimming along the sidewall, rather than searching in the central part of the maze where the platform is positioned.
40
Le Pen et al. b
c 50
50
40
40 30 20
60
Percent non-finders
60
Percent sidewall
Latency (sec)
a
* 30 20 10
10 0
0 1
2
3
Day
4
1
2
3
Day
4
* 40
* 20
0
1
2
*
3
4
Day
Fig. 2.12. Working memory performance in the reversal learning paradigm of the Morris water maze in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. The mean (±SEM) of latency to reach the hidden platform (a), percent of path length near (10 cm) the sidewall (b), and number of animals not finding the platform within 60 s (c) were presented for four consecutive trials. ∗ p < 0.05 vs. sham. (Adapted from Flagstad et al., 2004)
2.3.2. Attention and Information Processing
Deficits in attention and information processing have been considered a central feature in schizophrenia, which might lead to stimulus overload, cognitive fragmentation, and thought disorder (65, 66, 83–88). Various physiological and neuropsychological techniques related to selective attention and pre-attentional mechanisms have been used to quantify information processing deficits in schizophrenia such as P50 (89, 90), prepulse inhibition (PPI) (83), latent inhibition (LI) (91, 92), continuous performance test (93, 94), span of apprehension (95, 96), visual backward masking (97, 98), and dichotic listening (99, 100). The advantage of PPI (101) and LI is that they can be used in both a human and an animal experimental setup. Whereas PPI assesses early attentional gating mechanisms, often referred to as pre-attentional deficits, LI assesses later stages of information processing related to attentional filtering. In addition, in rats, the fivechoice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT) developed by Carli and colleagues (102) exhibits closed analogies with the continuous performance test and Leonard’s 5-CSRTT used in humans to assess the processes involved in sustained attention (103, 104). Finally, attentional set-shifting task is considered as a rodent analog of the Wisconsin card sort task. PPI, LI, 5-CSRTT as well as attentional set-shifting tasks have been used to clarify attention and information processing in MAM-exposed rats (30, 31, 34, 36).
2.3.2.1. Prepulse Inhibition (PPI) of Startle Reflex
A well-established method for evaluating sensory filtering is the paradigm of PPI which refers to the inhibition of a startle reflex by presentation of a weak intensity prepulse immediately before the startle stimulus. The impact of MAM exposure on PPI has been investigated using two different protocols (34, 36).
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol) Procedures
41
Acoustic startle reactivity and prepulse inhibition of startle reflex are assessed in a single session using standard startle chambers (SR-Lab, San Diego Instruments). (A) In Sprague Dawley rats (34, 35), after a 5-min acclimatization period (68 dB background noise), 10 startle pulses (120 dB, 40 ms duration) were presented with an average inter-trial interval of 15 s. During the next 20 min, no stimulus (background noise, 68 dB), prepulses alone (72, 76, 80, or 84 dB, 20 ms duration), startle pulses alone, six times for each condition, were randomly distributed. (B) In Fischer 344 rats (36), after a 5-min acclimatization period (55 dB background noise), in a first experiment, the startle threshold of both sham and MAM-exposed rats was determined with a repeated ascending, then randomized, series of 40-ms noise bursts ranging from 70 to 105 dB and in a second experiment, for PPI evaluation, rats were exposed to 100 dB startle tone (white noise burst), and 62 and 70 dB prepulses (6 or 12 ms duration). Prepulses were presented on 75% of the startle trials. PPI was defined as the percent reduction in startle amplitude in the presence of the prepulse compared with the amplitude in the absence of the prepulse [100 × (startle amplitude on prepulse−pulse trials/startle amplitude on pulse-alone trials) × 100] (Fig. 2.13).
Fig. 2.13. Scheme of prepulse inhibition of startle reflex response.
Results
PPI experiments in both Sprague Dawley and Fisher 344 rats revealed that E17 MAM-exposed animals at adulthood exhibited sensorimotor gating deficits (34, 36). In addition, we showed that these deficits emerge only after puberty (Fig. 2.14) (35). Besides, Moore and colleagues (36) showed that the PPI deficits were neither due to a decrease in sensory processing, since MAM-exposed
Le Pen et al.
600
600
400
Sham MAM
200 0
100 80 60 40 20 0 –20
72
76 80 Prepulse intensity (dB)
Startle amplitude
b-Post-Puberty
84
Percent prepulse inhibition
Sham MAM Percent prepulse inhibition
a-Pre-Puberty Startle amplitude
42
400 200 0
100 80 60
**
40
**
**
20 0 –20
** 72
76 80 Prepulse intensity (dB)
84
Fig. 2.14. Effect of prenatal (E17) MAM vs. saline exposure on prepulse inhibition (PPI) of startle reflex. PPI of the acoustic startle reflex (±SEM) was measured at pre- (a) and post-puberty (b). The insets show acoustic startle amplitude. ∗∗ p < 0.01 vs. sham group. (Adapted from Le Pen et al., 2006)
rats showed a lower threshold for responding to startle stimuli than did sham animals, nor due to a change in motor function, since the maximal startle response of MAM-exposed rats did not differ from that of sham. 2.3.2.2. Latent Inhibition (LI)
The LI paradigm is based on the phenomenon of reduced conditioning after stimulus pre-exposure. It is used to measure a form of selective attention, namely the ability to ignore irrelevant stimuli.
Procedure
The experiment performed in MAM-exposed rats by Flagstad and colleagues (32) was conducted using an automated shuttle box (42 cm × 16 cm × 20 cm) divided into two compartments by a partition with one opening. On the first day, the animals were left undisturbed in the shuttle box for 50 min with the houselights turned on. On the second day, the animals were divided into groups of pre-exposed (PE) and non-pre-exposed (NPE). The PE rats were subjected to 50 presentations (10–60 s randomized inter-trial interval) of the to-be-conditioned stimulus (cue light on for 10 s). The NPE rats were left undisturbed in the test box with only the houselights on for a corresponding duration. On the third day (test session), the animals were subjected to 100 trials of avoidance learning (10–60 s randomized inter-trial interval). Upon presentation of a light signal, the rat had 10 s to avoid a 0.5-mA scrambled foot shock by moving to the other compartment (avoidance). If the rat did not move to the other compartment within 10 s, a foot shock of a maximum duration of 10 s was delivered. The shock was terminated if the rat moved to
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
43
the other compartment. The position of the animal and crossings from one compartment to the other were detected by two photocells placed on either side of the dividing wall. The outcome measure was the number of avoidances (i.e., rats moving to the other compartment of the shuttle box after the presentation of the light but before the shock was delivered). In these experiments (Fig. 2.15), sham animals exhibited a greater number of avoidances in the NPE conditions than in the PE conditions that revealed good latent inhibition processes in those animals. In contrast, MAM-exposed animals exhibited the same number of avoidances in the two conditions, indicating the absence of latent inhibition in those rats (32). The latent inhibition experiment, however, also revealed that under the present experimental setup, MAM-exposed rats had a slower acquisition of the conditioned avoidance.
*
a
**
Conditioned reactions + SEM
60
40
20
0
NPE
NPE
PE
PE MAM
Sham
c 20
Avoidances/10 trials
b Avoidances/10 trials
Results
Sham NPE PE
8 6 4 2 0 20
40 60 Trial
80
100
20
MAM NPE PE
8 6 4 2 0 20
40 60 Trial
80
100
Fig. 2.15. Latent inhibition (LI) performances in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. (a) Number of avoidance responses (±SEM) during a 100-trial learning session in a two-way avoidance shuttle box in pre-exposed (PE) and non-pre-exposed (NPE) conditions. ∗ p < 0.05, ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 vs. sham group in NPE conditions. The number of avoidances made during the conditioning in 10 trial bins for sham (b) and MAM-exposed (c) animals. (Adapted from Flagstad et al., 2004)
44
Le Pen et al.
2.3.2.3. Attentional Set Shifting
In the set-shifting task, rats are required to solve a series of discriminations by attending a particular perceptual dimension of a multidimensional stimulus. A critical discrimination occurs when rats are required to shift to an alternate perceptual dimension after having acquired an attentional set to the previous dimension. The extradimensional shift (EDS) required to solve this discrimination is analogous to what is required to solve the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task, a test that has repeatedly shown impaired prefrontal cortex-mediated executive function in schizophrenia (105). The rodent set-shifting task also requires the prefrontal cortex (106, 107) and, thus, has been used for assessing attentional processes in MAM-exposed rats (30).
Procedure
For this test, Featherstone and colleagues (30) used a black twocompartment chamber (60 cm × 42 cm × 30 cm). Animals had access to either compartment where a small ceramic bowl (7 cm in diameter and 4 cm in depth) was placed. Each bowl was covered with a texture and filled with bedding material that was scented with an odor. Animals were initially trained to reliably dig and retrieve food from two bowls containing a small amount of bedding and a food reward (Fruit loop cereal). Animals were then trained on a simple discrimination (SD), in which food was paired with one stimulus dimension (either odor or texture) but not the other. Training continued until the animals chose correctly over six consecutive trials. The next phase of training involved the full set of discriminations with only one of the two bowls baited. The location of the correct bowl was randomly determined on every trial. The bowls differed according to odor and texture, with food being consistently associated with a particular odor or texture depending on the current discrimination. Discrimination training began with an SD. For this discrimination, only one stimulus dimension was present. In the second discrimination (CD), another stimulus dimension was added, although food remained paired with the original stimulus. The third discrimination involved a reversal of the second discrimination, in which an alternate exemplar of the relevant stimulus dimension was now paired with food. The fourth discrimination required animals to choose between a new series of stimuli, with the previously relevant stimulus dimension still paired with food (intradimensional shift (IDS)). This discrimination was followed by a reversal in which the alternate exemplar of the relevant stimulus dimension was now paired with food. In the sixth discrimination (EDS) the relevant stimulus dimension was changed such that the animal had to learn to attend to the previously irrelevant dimension. The final discrimination involved a reversal of the EDS discrimination in which the animal had to choose the previously irrelevant exemplar of the new stimulus dimension. In a first part, saline- and MAM-exposed animals received initial training (SD, CD, Rev 1,
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
45
and IDS) with odor as the relevant dimension and were required to make an EDS to texture. In a second part, saline- and MAMexposed animals received initial training with texture as the relevant dimension, followed by a shift to odor during the EDS. A correct trial was considered to have occurred when the animal successfully retrieved food from the bowl. If the animal dug in the incorrect bowl, the trial was scored as an error. Animals were moved onto the next discrimination once they had made six correct responses in a row. The individual who carried out the set-shifting task was unaware of the group identity of any given animal. On the attentional set-shifting task (Fig. 2.16), compared to controls, MAM-exposed animals required a greater number of trials to learn the EDS component of the task, indicative of a deficit in shifting attentional set (30). MAM-exposed rats were able to solve an SD and perform an IDS, suggesting that the deficit in EDS ability was not due to a generalized performance or cognitive impairment (30). Since deficits in performing an EDS are found in animals with a compromised prefrontal cortex, as has been shown on the attentional set-shifting task (106, 107), results obtained in MAM-exposed rats are in good accordance with prefrontal morphology and function abnormalities shown in those animals (30, 33, 35, 36, 39). Nevertheless, it is unclear whether difficulties in shifting attentional set are due to a failure to ignore a previously reinforced
30
*
*
25
Trials to criterion
Results
20
* 15
10
5
0
SD
CD
Rev1
IDS Rev2 Task phase
EDS
Rev3
Fig. 2.16. Mean (±SEM) number of trials to reach criterion on the attentional setshifting task in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. ∗ p < 0.05 vs. sham. (Adapted from Featherstone et al., 2007)
46
Le Pen et al.
dimension or to attend to a previously irrelevant dimension. Lesions of the prefrontal cortex have been shown to selectively increase perseveration to a previously learned dimension in non-human primates (108) and humans (109, 110). Since MAM injection at E17 disrupts prefrontal cortex structure and function, Featherstone and colleagues (30) hypothesized that MAMexposed animals would likewise show increases in perseveration and that this might serve as the basis for the deficit observed in the present study. In addition to impairment in EDS learning, MAM-exposed animals were impaired when required to make a simple reversal of a previously acquired discrimination (for two out of three reversals). These results are in good agreement with previous findings. Indeed, difficulties in reversal learning have been reported in E17 MAM-exposed animals within a water maze paradigm (31), as well as in a Y-maze task (36). 2.3.2.4. Five-Choice Serial Reaction Time (5-CSRT) Task
Visual attentional processes in E17 MAM-exposed rats have been evaluated by Featherstone and colleagues (30) using five-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT). The 5-CSRTT provides the possibility to test the effects of E17 MAM exposure on discrete and somewhat independent measures of behavioral control, including accuracy of discrimination, impulsivity, perseverative responses, and response latencies. Several aspects of the animal performance are assessed (accuracy = % correct), anticipatory responses (premature nose pokes which are thought to be analogous to impulsivity in humans) and perseverative responses (additional nose pokes following a correct response before a new trial is initiated, which are thought to be analogous to compulsive behaviors in humans).
Procedure
In their version of the task, the rats are required to attend and respond to visual stimuli displayed at different positions in an array of five niches placed along the wall of an operant chamber to receive a reward (here, 0.06 ml of a 10% sucrose/water solution) distributed in a magazine/liquid dipper located on the opposite wall. The magazine and light apertures contained photodetectors to detect nose poke entries. First, rats were initially trained to respond to signaled presentations of the dipper. Then, they were trained for 40 daily sessions during which they gradually learn to respond in the appropriate aperture within a certain amount of time. If they failed to respond, or respond in the wrong hole or at an inappropriate time, a short period of darkness (time-out) is presented as a punishment and no reward is delivered (Fig. 2.17). Task acquisition: For all subjects, the stimulus duration (SD) was progressively reduced from 30 s until a criterion duration of 1 s was achieved (all other parameters unchanged: ITI 5 s, limited hold 5 s, 100 trials per sessions). At 30 and 20 s, animals were required to perform at least 30 (out of 100) correct responses.
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
a
47
Food magazine
Stimulus lights
b
Dispense free pellet Houselight on
Collect pellet
Intertrial interval (5 sec) Premature response
Punishment (time-out, 5 sec)
Present stimulus (1s) Incorrect response
Punishment (time-out, 5 sec)
Correct response Perseverative response
Dispense pellet
Fig. 2.17. Schematic illustration of the five-hole box operant chamber. (a) General plan of the 5-choice serial reaction time test apparatus. (b) Flow diagram to illustrate the general task requirements.
48
Le Pen et al.
At 10 s, the level was increased to 50 correct trials. For 5, 2.5, 1.25, and 1 s, animals were required to make 50 or more correct responses, have greater than 80% accuracy, and less than 20% omitted trials. Animals were trained for a total of 41 sessions in order to reach criterion at the lowest stimulus duration of 1 s. Behavioral challenges: Once stable performance had been achieved at the 1-s SD, a series of manipulations of the stimulus parameters were employed. First, animals were challenged with different SDs (0.125, 0.25, 0.5, and 1 s). This was performed using a withinsubject design, with each test day separated by a maintenance session using 1 s as SD. The purpose of this manipulation was to increase demand on attentional resources. Second, animals were given an additional session using a prolonged ITI (9 s instead of 5 s). This manipulation typically increases premature responding, since animals must delay responding across a longer duration. Finally, animals were given a session of training with variable ITIs (3.5, 5.5, 7.5, and 9.5 s) occurring within the same session. Results
MAM-exposed rats were able to acquire the task similarly to sham animals since they needed the same amount of time as sham to reach the criterion at each training stage. In addition, at the end of the training, no obvious group differences were observed when comparing sham and MAM-exposed rats in any of the following performances: percent accuracy, percentage of omitted trials, number of perseverative responses, latency (in seconds) to perform the correct response, latency (in seconds) to collect reinforcement, and number of premature responses (30). MAM treatment did not disrupt accurate performance during any of the post-acquisition manipulations (shortened stimulus durations, variable ITI, and lengthened ITI), suggesting that performance in this task is not compromised by MAM treatment. Additionally, the lack of change in omitted trials and response latency suggests that attentional function was spared and, incidentally, indicates that motor and/or motivational functions were unaffected by MAM treatment. Given the effects of MAM treatment on attentional set shifting and reversal learning, as well as on prefrontal morphology, it is surprising that MAM treatment had little effect on the five-choice performance. Indeed, previous work has shown that the prefrontal cortex plays a major role in several aspects of performance in the five-choice task. Hence, prefrontal lesions alter accuracy (111, 112) and can induce changes in perseverative and premature responding (111, 113). MAM-exposed animals did show a trend toward an increase in premature responding during acquisition, suggesting a difficulty in behavioral inhibition (30). However, this change was nowhere near as significant as changes seen in premature responding following other prefrontal manipulations (113). It has to be noticed that rats evaluated in 5-CSRT task by Featherstone and colleagues (30) had
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
49
been previously evaluated in the attentional set-shifting paradigm. Thus, it cannot be excluded that a facilitation of five-choice performance in MAM-exposed rats could result from their attentional set-shifting experience. Therefore the absence of impact of E17 MAM exposure on sustained visual attention should be confirmed in an independent set of experiment. 2.3.3. Behavioral Flexibility
Impairment in different forms of behavioral flexibility such as set shifting and reversal learning has been frequently observed in schizophrenic patients. Indeed, they display great difficulty in shifting between different rules or strategies on tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting task (114–117). These impairments appear to be due in part to an inability to shift attentional set from one stimulus dimension to another, as similar impairments have been observed in patients tested on an intradimensional/extradimensional shifting (IDS/EDS) task (118, 119). Furthermore, a subset of these patients also display impairments in reversal learning, a simpler form of behavioral flexibility entailing shifts between different stimulus-reward associations within a particular dimension (120, 121). In both rats and humans, multiple prefrontal cortex regions and subcortical regions connected to the prefrontal cortex contribute to different component processes of behavioral flexibility. The orbital and prelimbic/infralimbic regions of the prefrontal cortex mediate some forms of reversal learning in the rat and human (109, 122–126). Then, it is likely that abnormalities in multiple prefrontal regions contribute to the cognitive inflexibility in the E17 MAM model. Thus, behavioral flexibility have been investigated in MAM-exposed rats through a reversal learning procedure that was tested in a Y maze (36) and a water maze procedure that we previously described (31).
2.3.3.1. Reversal Learning in a Y Maze
The test was performed in a Y maze that consisted of a central chamber with guillotine doors opening into three 36-in. runways, each terminating in a food magazine. Adult control rats and MAM-exposed rats were habituated to the Y maze and then trained in E17 the conditional discrimination task. At the beginning of the task, the rat was placed in an arm and the trial initiated by opening the door to the central chamber. Three seconds later, the doors to the choice arms opened and each food magazine was illuminated. A head poke into the CS+ magazine resulted in the termination of the light and delivery of food pellets. A poke into the CS− magazine resulted in the termination of the light and a 15-s time-out. The next trial was initiated from the choice arm of the previous trial; sessions were 30 trials long. After the rat achieved 67% accuracy for 3 consecutive days, the reward contingency was reversed (i.e., the CS+ became the CS− and vice versa). The number of trials to criterion for acquisition and
Procedure
50
Le Pen et al.
reversal phases was compared between E17 MAM-exposed rats and control rats. Results
First, relative to sham animals, MAM-exposed rats did not show a deficit in learning the novel discrimination. Indeed, these rats learned the discrimination significantly faster than did control rats, indicating that forebrain circuits involved in basic discriminated approach learning were not affected. However, under reversal conditions, MAM-exposed rats required significantly more trials to reach criterion than did sham animals (Fig. 2.18). Thus, MAM-exposed rats exhibited a significant deficit in reversal learning. Regarding the selective deficit in reversal learning, they observed in E17 MAM-exposed rats, Moore and colleagues (36) suggested that (1) cortical circuits involved in sensory and motor processes and association learning are intact; (2) the faster learning may be indicative of a perseverative response, in which the rats fail to test the non-rewarded arm once the association in the rewarded arm has been experienced; and (3) the perseveration is further revealed when the contingencies are reversed. Their learning phenotype is also consistent with the E17 MAM-exposed rats being more sensitive to the negative consequences associated with the original CS−. Each of these abnormalities implicates frontal cortical function.
Trials to criterion
400
Acquisition Reversal
*
300
200
+
100
0
Sham
MAM
Fig. 2.18. Reversal learning performances in a Y maze in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. salineexposed rats. Acquisition of the initial discrimination was significantly faster in MAMexposed rats. +p < 0.05 vs. sham group in acquisition conditions. However, MAMexposed rats required significantly more trials to learn the reversal of the discrimination. ∗ p < 0.05 vs. sham group in reversal conditions. (Adapted from Moore et al., 2006)
2.3.3.2. Reversal Learning in the Morris Water Maze
As previously mentioned, one asset of the water maze tests is that many aspects of performance can be examined, providing several measures of spatial memory. Another asset is its capacity to examine reversal learning as a component of behavioral flexibility processes where rat has to switch from an established strategy to develop a new one.
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
51
Procedure
As previously described (see Section 2.3.1), the water maze consisted of a circular, black pool filled with water. The test room contained several permanent extra-maze cues such as the rat housing rack, laboratory table, and posters on the walls. In the experiment performed by Flagstad and colleagues (32), behavioral flexibility can be evaluated between the first and second days of the working memory version of the task. Indeed, the working memory task takes place after the reference memory version of the task (see below) that involves the acquisition of rules about spatial location of the platform that are constant (north quadrant of the pool) across all trials of the task. On the first day of the working memory task, the platform was located in the same position as for the reference memory task but on the second day the platform was positioned in the opposite quadrant. The rationale underlying this task is that animals displaying behavioral flexibility will rapidly learn to search the platform in its new location, while rats impaired in behavioral flexibility will spend more time around the old location. Key measures were path length (m), escape latency (s) to find the hidden platform, percent of path length near (10 cm) the sidewall (percent sidewall), and number of animals not finding the platform within 60 s (nonfinders). These measures were recorded using a video tracking system.
Results
On the first day of the working memory task, sham and MAM-exposed animals performed similarly to locate the hidden platform. On the second day, when the platform moved to the opposite quadrant, Flagstad and colleagues (32) showed that MAM-exposed rats performed significantly worse than the controls but only on the basis of the percent sidewall and non-finder parameters (Fig. 2.12). These results suggest that MAM-exposed rats exhibit abnormal behavioral flexibility in a reversal learning version of the Morris water maze task.
2.3.4. Episodic Memory
Human episodic memory refers to the recollection of a unique past experience in terms of its details, its locale, and temporal occurrence (127). Episodic memory deficits among individuals with schizophrenia are well established (128–131). Interestingly, studies suggest that non-human mammals have the ability to build higher-order memory for unique events that incorporate information about what, where, and when, lending strong support to the idea that animals are endowed with episodic-like memory (132– 135). Thus, object and spatial recognition tasks as well as spatial reference memory paradigm have been used to evaluate episodic memory in MAM-exposed rats.
2.3.4.1. Object Recognition Task
Previous studies in schizophrenic patients have demonstrated episodic memory deficits reflected by impaired recognition of
52
Le Pen et al.
visually presented objects (128, 136, 137). In parallel, neuroimaging studies have provided evidence that schizophrenia is associated with abnormal brain activation within the hippocampus, the thalamus, and/or the prefrontal cortex during the recognition of previously seen or new objects (13, 138, 139). Ennaceur and Delacour (135) presented evidence that rodents are able to form an integrated memory for “what,” “where,” and “when” aspects of single experiences by combining different versions of the novelty-preference paradigm, i.e., object recognition memory, the memory for locations in which objects were explored, and the temporal order memory for objects presented at distinct time points. The novel object recognition task (NORT) was used to assess if MAM-exposed animals are able to form an integrated episodic memory for “what.” This test is based on the natural tendency of rodents to explore a novel object by comparison to a familiar one (135). Naive animals will spend more time exploring a novel object rather than a familiar one. Procedure
The test was performed in an open field (45 cm × 65 cm × 29 cm). On the day before the test, rats were habituated to the test box for 10 min. On the test day, rats were put in the test box, and after 3-min habituation, two similar objects were introduced in the two corners. The time spent investigating the two objects was scored in the next 3-min period (denoted pretest). The rats were then removed and put back in the housing cage. After a delay period of 20 min, the rats were reintroduced to the test box and 3 min later the objects [one similar to that used in the pretest (known) and one the rat had never encountered before (new)] were introduced. The objects were placed in the same position as in the pretest. In the next 3 min, the time spent investigating each of the objects was scored. All scoring was performed using a video tracking system.
Results
The MAM-exposed rats spent significantly less time exploring the objects on the pretest. There was also a main effect of the group in the test session, indicating that the MAM-exposed rats spent less time exploring the objects (Fig. 2.19). There was no main effect of the known/new object factor; however, there was an interaction between the group and the known/new factor. This interaction was caused by a significant difference between the known/new objects in controls, but not in the MAM-exposed rats. Furthermore, there was no difference between the amount of time the animals in each group spent exploring the known object, whereas the controls spent a longer time exploring the new object than the MAM-exposed did. Overall, these results revealed that MAM-exposed rats exhibited object recognition deficits that could be reminiscent of those observed in schizophrenic patients.
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
Investigation time (sec)
60
53
a-Pre-test
50 40
*
30 20 10 0 Sham
Investigation time (sec)
60 50
MAM
b-Test
**
40 30 20 10 0 Known New
Known New
Sham
MAM
Fig. 2.19. Object recognition in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. (a) Mean investigation time (±SEM) during the pretest of the object recognition paradigm. ∗ p < 0.05 vs. sham. (b) Mean investigation time (±SEM) of the known and the new object during the object recognition test. ∗∗ p < 0.01 known vs. knew. (Adapted from Flagstad et al., 2004)
2.3.4.2. Spatial Recognition Memory in a Y Maze
Memory for spatial context has been shown to be impaired in schizophrenic patients (140–145). Recently, Da Silva and colleagues (132) proposed that spatial recognition memory in the rodent can be likened to human episodic memory. In rodents, various brain areas, including the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus, are known to be pivotal in spatial recognition memory (146–148). A simple two-trial recognition test in a Y maze was performed to investigate spatial recognition memory in MAMexposed rats. This test is based on the innate tendency of rodents to explore novel environments (149–151). The paradigm avoids the effects of punishment (such as electric shock) or reward (such as food) that is commonly used in other paradigms and that may have non-specific effects on the results. In addition, it does not require learning of a rule and is independent of locomotor activity; thus, it is useful for studying memory in rodents (150–153).
54
Le Pen et al.
Procedure
This two-trial memory task is based on exploration of novelty (151). Experiments were carried out in a Y maze which consisted of three identical alleys (40 cm long and 15 cm large with walls 30 cm high), diverging at a 120◦ angle from the central point. The experiments were performed in a dimly illuminated room. Numerous visual cues were placed on the walls and were kept constant during the experiments. The test consisted of two trials, separated by a 2-h time interval. During the first trial (acquisition phase), one arm of the Y maze was closed, allowing the rats to explore the remaining two arms for 3 min. The position of the closed arm and the introduction arm was randomized. During the second trial (retrieval phase), the rats had access to the three arms for a 3-min period. During this period, for each rat, the time spent in each arm and the total locomotor activity was measured by a video tracking system. The ability of the animals to remember the two arms already visited during the first phase and to explore preferentially the novel arm opened during the second phase. Then, after a 2-h time interval, the control rats are expected to spend more time in the “novel” arm, which remained closed during the first trial, compared with the two “familiar” ones that were already open during the first phase (151).
Results
Before puberty, both MAM-exposed and sham rats showed poor spatial performances revealed by the absence of clear preference for the novel arm (Fig. 2.20). This is consistent with the literature which suggests that rats younger than 40 days exhibit spatial navigation deficits in relation to the immature hippocampus (154, 155). At adulthood, the same MAM-exposed animals spent approximately the same amount of time in the three arms of the maze, in contrast to sham animals that exhibited a clear preference for the novel arm. The data reflect the physiological brain maturation occurring during puberty in control animals. In contrast, in either pre- or post-puberty, the MAM-exposed rats
Introduction arm
Novel arm
Third arm Post-Puberty
Pre-Puberty 70
70
a Time spent (s)
Time spent (s)
b
60
60 50 40 30 20
50 40
#
#
30 20 10
10 0
0 Sham
MAM
Sham
MAM
Fig. 2.20. Spatial recognition memory (Y-maze paradigm) in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. saline-exposed rats. Time spent (±SEM) in the novel arm was compared with the time spent in the introduction and third arms at pre-puberty (a) and post-puberty (b). # p < 0.05 vs. novel arm. (Adapted from Le Pen et al., 2006)
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
55
did not exhibit a preference for the novel arm and remained approximately the same amount of time in the three arms of the maze, suggesting that the normal spatial recognition memory skills were not acquired during puberty in the MAM-exposed animals. In addition, MAM-exposed rats spent less time than sham animals in the central zone of the Y maze at both pre-puberty and adulthood, suggesting that memory deficits are unlikely a consequence of anxiety-like behavior. Conversely, when tested in the social interaction test, MAM-exposed rats were not different from controls in the time spent in the central area. So the results in the Y maze could reflect impulsive-like behavior in the MAM-exposed rats. In conclusion, spatial recognition memory, as explored by the Y maze, is thus relevant to follow the late maturation of the hippocampo-prefrontal networks occurring during the rat’s puberty and the related acquisitions of cognitive skills. Y maze results also present interesting similarities with observations of visuo-spatial memory deficits seen in schizophrenic patients (140–145). 2.3.5. Spatial Reference Memory
Reference memory is regarded as a long-term memory for information that remains constant over repeated trials (156). The retrieval of information, based on spatiotemporal context, from long-term memory is affected in schizophrenia. Morris water maze paradigm is frequently used in rodents to assess reference memory in a spatial context (82, 157). In this paradigm, animals have to navigate to a goal (hidden platform) in an allocentric spatial environment and must build a representation of the goal in relation to distal cues (constellations of cues) to form a cognitive map. Then, they must be able to use this information flexibly regardless of where in the environment the subject begins navigating. In animals (158) and humans (159), the hippocampus is involved in this kind of spatial learning, because encoding is based on a flexible knowledge of relationships between environmental cues. Hippocampus-dependent spatial memory in animals is thus phylogenetically viewed as a homologue of human episodic memory (160, 161). Thus, in order to evaluate spatial reference memory in MAM-exposed rats, the reference memory version of the Morris water maze has been used by Hazane and colleagues (34) and Flagstad and colleagues (31).
Procedure
The water maze consisted of a circular, black pool filled with water. The test room contained several permanent extra-maze cues such as the rat housing rack, laboratory table, and posters on the walls. In the reference memory task, the rats were trained to locate a hidden platform positioned 1 cm below the surface of the water. The platform remained in a fixed position throughout the test, although the animal’s starting position varied. Key measures were path length (m), escape latency (s) to find the
56
Le Pen et al.
hidden platform, percent of path length near (10 cm) the sidewall (percent sidewall), and number of animals not finding the platform within 60 s (non-finders). These measures were recorded using a video tracking system. Reference memory capacities measured in MAM-exposed rats revealed various results that could result from differences in either the strain reactivity or the experimental procedure. Thus, in the reference memory paradigm of the water maze task, Flagstad and colleagues (31) showed that MAM-exposed rats performed significantly worse than the controls on days 2 and 3 of testing but only on the basis of the percent sidewall and non-finder parameters. On the last day, however, they performed as well as the controls. Thus, it does not appear that the MAM treatment interferes with spatial working memory, as this should also result in a deficit on the last day of testing. The MAM treatment seems, however, to affect the search strategy of the rats. When the platform is moved, rats spend more time swimming along the sidewall, rather than searching in the central part of the maze where the platform is positioned. On the other hand, in Sprague Dawley rats, Hazane and colleagues (34) observed that MAM-exposed rats were significantly impaired in learning to locate the hidden platform compared to control animals (latency to reach the platform) (Fig. 2.21). Analysis of performance in each group revealed that, in contrast to sham rats, MAM-exposed rats did not improve their latency to locate the platform across trials. This contrasts with previous reports showing no effect of prenatal MAM exposure in Wistar rats using a similar spatial reference memory version of the Morris water maze task and a MAM exposure at E17 Sham MAM
40
Latency to reach the platform (sec)
Results
***
35
*** ***
30
***
25 20 15 10 5 0 1
2
3 4 Block number
5
6
Fig. 2.21. Reference memory in the Morris water maze in prenatal E17 MAM- vs. salineexposed rats. Time (sec) to reach the platform in (±SEM) E17 MAM-exposed vs. sham female rats in Morris water maze. ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 vs. sham group. (Adapted from Hazane et al., 2009)
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
57
(22 mg/kg) (31). This impaired reference memory is of particular interest since patients with schizophrenia were similarly impaired on the hippocampal-dependent hidden-platform version of a virtual Morris water maze task. Indeed, patients were slower and had longer search paths than did controls to find the hidden platform (162, 163).
3. Neurochemical Changes Observed in E17 MAM-Exposed Rats
3.1. Neurochemical Abnormalities Associated with Hypersensitivity to D-AmphetamineInduced Hyperlocomotion in MAM-EXPOSED Rats
If the behavioral phenotype of E17 MAM-exposed rats is now pretty well characterized, fewer studies have characterized the neurochemical alterations associated with this neurodevelopmental animal model for schizophrenia and its behavioral abnormalities. Structural and/or functional alterations, presumed to reflect abnormal brain development, have consistently been found in schizophrenic patients in several interconnected brain regions such as the prefrontal cortex (164–166), the hippocampus (139, 167–169) and the striatum (170–172). In particular, dysfunctions of glutamatergic and/or dopaminergic neurotransmission have been demonstrated within these structures (164, 169–172) and are thought to play a central role in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (173, 174). In addition, abnormalities in markers for GABAergic neurons in prefrontal cortex and hippocampus are also well documented in schizophrenia (175–181) with more recent studies suggesting a central role of impairment in the glutamate-GABA interactions, both in the hippocampus and in the prefrontal cortex. In line with these findings, neurochemical abnormalities associated with hypersensitivity to D-amphetamineand MK-801-induced hyperlocomotion in MAM-exposed rats have been investigated using in vivo microdialysis techniques. In addition, possible abnormalities in GABAergic markers have also been explored in MAM-exposed rats using immunohistochemistry. The prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens shell regions play a crucial role in mediating the behavioral effects of increased dopaminergic activity in schizophrenia (51, 182–185). In rodents, the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex are strongly implicated in psychostimulant action. Dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens is believed to be the main mediator of the reinforcing and locomotor-activating properties of psychostimulants (186). The prefrontal cortex modulates the nucleus accumbens activity through direct and indirect connections and there is evidence that dopamine release in the prefrontal cortex may be related to the inhibition of reward and locomotor activity (187). Thus, in order to further assess possible corticolimbic dysfunctions associated with D-amphetamine-
58
Le Pen et al.
induced hyperlocomotion in MAM-exposed rats, Flagstad and colleagues (32) investigated changes in dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens and the medial prefrontal cortex after stimulation with D-amphetamine in sham and MAM-exposed Wistar rats. 3.1.1. Procedure
At adulthood, intracerebral guide cannulae were stereotaxically implanted in anesthetized Wistar rats. The guide cannula for the nucleus accumbens was implanted in the right hemisphere at AP +1.7 mm, L 0.8 mm, and V –6 mm from the brain surface (188). The guide cannula for the medial prefrontal cortex was implanted in the right hemisphere at an angle of 20 degrees, at AP +3.8 mm, L 0.7 mm, V −0.35 mm. After a 2-day period of recovery, rats were then placed in a hemispheric bowl and the microdialysis probes were inserted in the guide cannulae (0.5 mm diameter, 2 mm probe length for nucleus accumbens, and 4 mm probe length for the medial prefrontal cortex). The probe was connected to a microinfusion pump via a dual-channel liquid swivel, which allows the animals to move freely in the cage during the experiment. The rats had free access to food and water in the test bowls. The dialysis probe was perfused with Ringer solution at a constant flow rate of 1 ml/min. The perfusate was discarded during the initial 100 min and then collected in 20-min intervals. The first four fractions represented the baseline level. Rats were then given D-amphetamine (2 mg/kg, s.c.) and fractions were collected for 160 min. The samples were stored at −80◦ C until analysis. Dopamine was analyzed by HPLC with electrochemical detection. The levels of dopamine in the dialysates were not corrected for probe recovery. Dopamine was separated by reversephase liquid chromatography. Electrochemical detection used a colorimetric detector.
3.1.2. Results
Basal levels of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and the prefrontal cortex of MAM-exposed rats and sham animals were similar. Flagstad and colleagues (32) demonstrated that acute systemic injection of D-amphetamine induced an exaggerated increase of dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens of MAMexposed rats in line with the striatal dopaminergic hyper-reactivity observed in schizophrenic patients (170–172). In contrast, Damphetamine-induced increase in dopamine concentration in the dialysate from the prefrontal cortex was similar in both sham and MAM-exposed rats.
3.2. Neurochemical Abnormalities Associated with Hypersensitivity to MK-801-Induced Hyperlocomotion in MAM-EXPOSED Rats
In rodents, NMDA antagonists appear to exert their disruptive effects on behavior through the activation of glutamatergic (at non-NMDA receptors), dopaminergic, and noradrenergic transmission (189–195). Thus, acute systemic injection of MK-801, another non-competitive NMDA antagonist, to MAM-exposed rats could exacerbate the neurochemical dysfunctions induced
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
59
during brain development by prenatal exposure to MAM. To test this hypothesis, Lena and colleagues (40), using in vivo microdialysis, examined the effects induced by MK-801 (0.1 mg/kg s.c.) on the extracellular levels of glutamate, dopamine, and noradrenaline in the medial prefrontal cortex and the nucleus accumbens of adult Sprague Dawley rats prenatally exposed to MAM at E17. 3.2.1. Procedure
At adulthood, rats were anaesthetized and mounted on a stereotaxic frame. Two guide cannulae were implanted bilaterally, one in the left medial prefrontal cortex (AP from bregma, +3.0 mm; L, 1.6 mm angled 10◦ toward midline; V, −2.8 mm) and the other in the right nucleus accumbens (AP, +1.4 mm; L, 0.8 mm; V, −6.2 mm) according to the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (188). The two guide cannulae were secured to the skull using dental cement and two stainless steel screws. After 5 days of recovery, rats were placed in a rectangular Plexiglas cage (35 mm × 35 mm × 38 mm), and two concentric microdialysis probes (CMA/12, 500 μm diameter, 20 kDa cut-off) with a membrane length of 3 mm for medial prefrontal cortex or 2 mm for nucleus accumbens were inserted into the guide cannulae. Artificial cerebrospinal fluid was perfused through the probes at a constant rate of 1 μl/min. The probes were connected to the microperfusion syringes via FEP tubing using a dual-channel liquid swivel, allowing free movement of the animal in the experimental cage. After a 4-h stabilization period, dialysates were collected every 10 min over a period of 180 min and immediately stored at −80◦ C before analysis by capillary electrophoresis. Three samples were collected before injection of MK-801 (0.1 mg/kg) or saline to rats prenatally exposed to MAM or saline and were used to determine the basal levels of neurotransmitters. The time delay due to the dead volume of the microdialysis system (probe and output tubing) was taken into account to synchronize the measurement of locomotor activity with sample collection. Catecholamines and glutamate analysis were analyzed by capillary electrophoresis. The concentrations of catecholamines (dopamine and noradrenaline) and glutamate in the dialysate samples were determined using an automatic P/ACETM MDQ system equipped with an external laser-induced fluorescence and ZETALIF detector. Separations were performed using a fused-silica capillary.
3.2.2. Results
The study demonstrates that the neurodevelopmental insults induced by prenatal MAM exposure lead, in adult rats, to dysfunctions of the neurotransmission in the medial prefrontal cortex but not in the nucleus accumbens, in response to a noncompetitive NMDA antagonist. The MK-801-induced increase in cortical glutamate levels was reduced, whereas the cortical
60
Le Pen et al.
noradrenaline release was potentiated. These neurochemical alterations were associated with an exaggerated locomotor hyperactivity. Given the implication of the medial prefrontal cortex and the noradrenergic system in the locomotor effects of non-competitive NMDA antagonists, it is conceivable that the potentiation of cortical noradrenaline release might underlie the enhanced locomotor response in MAM-exposed rats. Future studies examining the interactions between the noradrenergic and the glutamatergic systems in the medial prefrontal cortex will be of particular interest in elucidating the mechanisms underlying this abnormal behavioral response. 3.3. Reelin Level in Organotypic Hippocampal Cultures of MAM-EXPOSED Rats
In addition, “postnatal” synthesis of reelin in the hippocampus was investigated in vitro with cultures of hippocampal slices, which allow conservation of cytoarchitectural properties. Hippocampal slices done at 1 week postnatal (P8) and maintained in culture for 3 weeks reach the development of “post-pubertal” hippocampus as it would have in vivo. This method is thus interesting for studying postnatal modifications at the cellular level. Briefly at the age of postnatal day 8 (P8), rats were anesthetized by hypothermia and their brain was aseptically removed. Hippocampal organotypic slice cultures were prepared using the Stoppini method. The hippocampus was dissected out under microscopic control. Tissue pieces were cut perpendicular to the septotemporal axis of the hippocampus with a McIllwain tissue chopper. Hippocampal slices were then transferred into the culture medium, separated, and put onto Millicell-CM membranes. A total of 12 organotypic cultures of adjacent hippocampal slices were obtained per brain. The Millicell membranes were kept in six-well plates above 900 μl of defined medium (neurobasal medium with 20% B27 serum-free supplement and 0.5 mM L-glutamine). Slices were incubated at 36◦ C in 5% CO2 . Cultures were stopped after 1 and 3 weeks and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 1 h. For reelin labeling, the tissue was stained using the diaminobenzidine immunoperoxidase method with anti-reelin G10 monoclonal antibody (Chemicon). Neuronal density was evaluated using the neuronal marker NeuN (neuronal nuclei; Chemicon) coupled to rhodamine fluorophore. Double fluorescence immunohistochemistry to detect expression of both reelin and Calretinin was also performed with anti-calretinin polyclonal antibody (Chemicon) as primary antibody and the secondary antibodies were fluorescent dyes: confocal microscopy and image analysis software were used to analyze the results. Finally, reelin’s expression and methylation levels as well as the number of reelin-positive cells have been investigated in the hippocampal formation of MAM-exposed animals (214).
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
61
Coronal slices from MAM-exposed and control animals were stained with anti-reelin antibody (G10 Chemicon) and 3,39diaminobenzidine used as chromogen. For methylation, hippocampal deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) was treated with sodium bisulfite using the EpiTect Kit H (Epigenomics; Qiagen, BBBB). Modified DNA was purified by EpiTect H spin columns and used immediately or stored at −20◦ C. Bisulfite-modified genomic DNA was amplified with primers specific for the reelin promoter [59-CGTTTTTTTATTTTGGTTTGGT-39 (forward) and 59-TCATATCATACATAACCACTATCCCTA-39 (reverse)]. The polymerase chain reaction (PCR) conditions were 95◦ C for 15 min, 5 cycles of 95◦ C for 30 s, 56◦ C for 90 s, 72◦ C for 120 s, 25 cycles of 95◦ C for 30 s, 56◦ C for 90 s, 72◦ C for 90 s, and finally 7 min at 72◦ C. The PCR mixture contained 12.5 ml of HotStarTaq Mix (Qiagen), 0.5 ml of each primer at a 20 mM concentration, and 2 ml bisulfite-modified DNA. These first-round PCR products were then used as template (1 ml) and reamplified by tagged primers (59-CCACTCACTCACCCACCC + forward primer-39 and 59-GGGTGGGAGGTGGGAGGG + reverse primer-39). The second-round PCR conditions were 95◦ C for 15 min, 35 cycles of 95◦ C for 30 s, 55◦ C for 90 s, 65◦ C for 120 s, and finally 7 min at 65◦ C. PCR products were then purified with a gel extraction kit (Qiagen) and subjected to direct cycle sequencing on an ABI3100Avant automated DNA sequencer (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA), using the primers from the second round of PCR. Sequence chromatographs were analyzed by the software ESMEH (Epigenomics), which performs quality control, normalizes signals, corrects for incomplete bisulfite conversion, and maps positions. 3.4. Abnormal GABAergic Markers in MAM-EXPOSED Rats
Abnormal GABAergic transmission has also been found in several brain regions, including the hippocampus (196, 197) and the prefrontal cortex (198, 199). Decreases in glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD)-1 mRNA and GAD-67 protein are observed postmortem throughout the cortex of schizophrenic patients (200–202). An increasing number of studies are focusing on GABAergic interneurons. Postmortem studies have consistently reported a significant reduction of GABA interneurons (small, non-pyramidal) in layer 2 of the anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortices (176, 198) and hippocampus (203) of schizophrenia brains. GABAergic interneurons comprise a variety of different subpopulations that can be identified by their neurochemical profile (204). The relative density of subpopulations of GABAergic interneurons is compared using antibodies directed against the calcium-binding proteins, calretinin (CR), parvalbumin (PV), and calbindin (CB). CR-containing neurons have been consistently
62
Le Pen et al.
reported to be unchanged in any regions of schizophrenia brains (175, 178–181). CB-containing neurons have been found to be significantly increased in layers 3 and 5/6 of prefrontal cortex (areas 9 and 46) (205) or decreased in these same prefrontal regions (areas 9 and 46) (179). Others have found no significant difference in anterior cingulate (206), prefrontal (area 46), entorhinal (207), or posterior cingulate cortices (areas 30 and 23) (208). Concerning PV-containing neurons, if reduced densities were observed in the hippocampus of schizophrenic patients (181, 209), conflicting results were obtained in the prefrontal cortex. Indeed, most investigators reported lower PV+ cell counts (175, 178, 179, 210), in particular in layers 3 and 4 of the cortex, although others found no change (180, 206, 211). Another marker found in GABAergic neurons is reelin. A marked decrease in reelin in the superficial layer of the cortex has been consistently found in postmortem studies in schizophrenia (reviewed in (2)). In order to investigate the possible GABA neurotransmission abnormalities in MAM-exposed rats, possible changes in the expression of PV, CR, CB, reelin, and GAD-67 have been examined in the prefrontal cortex and/or the hippocampus of MAMexposed rats using immunochemistry (37, 41). 3.4.1. Procedures
Adult male rats were deeply anesthetized and perfused transcardially with PBS or saline followed by paraformaldehyde [4% (w/v) PFA in 0.1 M PBS]. Rats were decapitated and their brains removed, postfixed, and stored in 0.1 M PBS or in 30% sucrose solution until sectioning. In their study, Penschuck and colleagues (37) used 40 μm sections with AP from bregma, 3.7–2.7 mm for the prefrontal cortex and −2.8 to −3.3 mm for the hippocampus The tissue was stained using the diaminobenzidine immunoperoxidase method and primary antibodies for either PV (1:5000), CR (1:2000), or CB (1:10000), according to the manufacturer’s instructions (Swant, Bellinzona, Switzerland). The semi-quantitative analysis was performed blind using stereology software. Lodge and colleagues (41) used a different sampling procedure with sequential sections (50 μm) comprising the prefrontal region (bregma −5.0 mm to −2.0 mm), dorsal (bregma −1.5 mm to −4.5 mm), and ventral (bregma −4.5 mm to −7.5 mm) hippocampus (six slices per region), and double fluorescence immunohistochemistry to detect expression of both GAD67 and PV as described previously (212). Primary antibodies were used for GAD67 (monoclonal 1:2000; Millipore; MAB5406) and PV (1:3000; Swant; PV28) and the secondary antibodies were fluorescent dyes: Alexa Fluor-conjugated goat anti-rabbit (568) or goat anti-mouse (488). Mounted slices were evaluated for fluorescence using confocal microscopy and image analysis software.
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
3.4.2. Results
63
Some of the results remain controversial. Penschuck and colleagues (37) showed that the number of PV-immunoreactive cells/mm2 was significantly decreased in the dorsal part of the hippocampus of MAM-exposed rats in comparison to controls and unchanged in the prefrontal cortex. No changes in the numbers of CR- or CB-immunoreactive cells were found in either structure. In contrast, Lodge and colleagues (41) showed that MAM-exposed rats exhibited a regionally selective reduction in the density of PV-positive neurons throughout the medial prefrontal cortex and in the ventral subiculum of the hippocampus with no significant differences in the number of GAD-67-positive/PV-negative neurons in either the medial prefrontal cortex or the ventral subiculum. These differences in PV cell density were not observed in the dorsal subiculum of the hippocampus, while a small decrease in PV cell density was observed throughout the anterior cingulate cortex. No significant differences in fluorescence intensity for either GAD-67 or PV were found between MAM-exposed and control rats. The negative results concerning GAD-67 obtained by Lodge and colleagues (41) are not in light with those obtained in schizophrenic patients where a decrease was generally observed. Concerning the data on the subset of interneurons that contain the calcium buffer PV, the conflicting findings reported in the two studies (37, 41) could be explained in part by methodological discrepancies. Indeed, the two groups have performed their experiments in two different rat strains (Wistar and Sprague Dawley rats) that have been exposed at E17 to two different MAM doses (22 and 20 mg/kg). In addition, the method of slice sampling could also explain such a difference since the two groups used different brain coordinates to anatomically define the regions being investigated. Thus, results for PV in the E17 MAM model are as much conflicting as those observed in schizophrenic patients for this marker of GABAergic interneurons. Regarding the expression of reelin, the picture is not so clear. In organotypic slices, the number of cells was decreased in the number of neurons that express reelin was increased in the stratum oriens, concomitantly with a decreased number of neurons in the stratum oriens, while no change was seen in the dentate gyrus (a region where neurogenesis is still active in adulthood). In postmortem whole brain studies, the number of reelin-positive cells was not changed in the hippocampus of MAM-exposed rats compared to controls, nor was the methylation or the expression (214). The effect of E17 MAM exposure is thus not so clear and will require further exploration.
64
Le Pen et al.
4. Anatomical and Functional Anomalies in E17 MAM-Exposed Rats 4.1. Anatomical Anomalies
Neuroimaging studies in schizophrenic patients have identified morphological and functional anomalies which can be explained by early neurodevelopmental factors. Main findings include enlargement of the lateral ventricles, reduced size of temporal lobe structures, decreased thalamic volume, and enlarged basal ganglia (213). The loss of cortical volume without loss of neurons in frontal and temporal association cortex is associated with the greatest reduction in spine number in schizophrenia. Morphometric data from other brain regions like the hippocampal formation, the entorhinal cortex, and the mediodorsal thalamus coming from brain of patients with schizophrenia is reviewed in Matricon et al. (214).
4.1.1. Prefrontal Cortex and Other Cortices
Brain abnormalities identified postmortem in adult rats prenatally exposed to MAM at E17 point to a size reduction and a distortion in superficial layers of the prefrontal cortex (32, 33, 35–37) (Fig. 2.22). The decrease in cortical thickness appears significant in the prefrontal, occipital, and entorhinal cortices, whereas no significant differences between MAM-exposed rats and controls are noted in the parietal and perirhinal cortices (36, 214). Using complementary staining methods (Bodian-Luxol for myelin and neuronal soma and NeuN, a protein specific for neuron and restricted to the axon hillock), Matricon et al. (214) also evidenced a disruption in columnar organization in the entorhinal cortex (Fig. 2.23). A reduction in the soma size could be observed in both the perirhinal and entorhinal cortices, while this decrease in the entorhinal cortex was associated with a laminar disorganization and the presence of aberrantly clustered neurons in layers 2 and 3. However, no significant difference was observed in the number of neurons in the prefrontal, entorhinal, and perirhinal cortices between MAM-exposed animals and control (36, 214). Conversely, stereological analyses showed increases in neuronal density in the prefrontal, perirhinal, and occipital cortices (36).
4.1.2. Hippocampus and Subcortical Regions
In the MAM-exposed rats, hippocampal and mediodorsal thalamus size reductions have been reported (35, 36, 214). No significant difference between MAM-exposed and control animals could be detected in the size of the ventral tegmental area, the substantia nigra, and the amygdala (214). Within the hippocampal formation, changes in neuronal density appear quite difficult to assess due to the heterogeneity in the different subfields and the
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
a
b
c
d
65
CA1/sub
CA1/sub
rsp
rsp
ent
ent
f
e mpfc
mpfc
Fig. 2.22. Representative sections of the hippocampus and adjacent cortices (a–d), the prefrontal cortex (e and f ) from embryonic day 17 (E17) methylazoxymethanol (MAM)-exposed rats (a, c, e) as compared to controls (b, d, f). Dorsal hippocampus sections show heterotopias (white squares) in CA1 in E17 MAM-exposed rats (a). Ventral hippocampus sections illustrate abnormalities in the distribution of CA1/subicular cells (white squares and arrowheads) extended to some cells in E17 MAM-exposed rats (c, CA1/sub). Note alterations in the entorhinal (black square, ent) and retrosplenial cortices (black square, rsp), with dispersed cell and gaps in E17 rats (c). In the same sections, the rhinal fissure (black arrowhead ) points to a deformation of the perirhinal cortex when compared to E17 (c) and control rats (d). Medial prefrontal cortex sections (mpfc, higher magnification) show a distortion in layers II and III, with a decrease in cell density in E17 MAM-exposed rats (e). (Adapted from Gourevich et al., 2004)
structural changes in MAM-exposed rats. Neuronal soma size reductions were mostly present in hippocampal subfields CA3 and CA4 (214). Neuronal density did not differ between MAMexposed and control rats in the mediodorsal thalamus.
66
Le Pen et al.
Fig. 2.23. Entorhinal cortex in control and MAM-exposed rats. Coronal sections stained with Bodian-Luxol (a, d) and NeuN immunostaining (b, e and c, f). In controls (a) a well-defined columnar organization and myelin with parallel directions can be observed. Arrows point to myelin pathways. (b) Anti-NeuN immunostaining shows axon hillocks with a parallel orientation and neuronal bodies regularly organized. (c) Neuronal somas and axon hillocks are clearly distinguished in this picture magnified 400×. Arrows point to axon hillock direction. In MAM-exposed animals (d), a less obvious columnar organization is observed, while myelin direction appears random. (e) Neuronal disorganization with clusters of neurons is evident mainly in layers II and III. Circles highlight neuronal clusters. (f) Neuronal disorganization, neuronal size variability, and axon hillocks with random directions can be observed in this picture magnified 400×. Arrows point to axon hillock direction. (Adapted from Matricon et al., 2010)
Brain volume reductions could originate from a variety of causes. Neuronal soma size reduction could be at the origin of the volume reduction reported in some brain regions in both MAM-exposed rats and patients with schizophrenia (198–214). On the other hand, changes in cortical neuropil architecture as revealed by a loss of dendritic spines could explain the loss of cortical volume. Indeed, measurements of spine density and dendrite morphology of pyramidal neurons in schizophrenic patients showed the greatest reduction in spine number in frontal and temporal association cortex where a significant volume loss is reported (215). More detailed investigations on dendrite morphology are necessary to answer this question in MAM-exposed rats. Altogether, these anatomical changes found in E17 MAMexposed animals on top of a significant enlargement of the lateral
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
67
ventricles indicate significant similarities to neuropathological deficiencies encountered in patients with schizophrenia. 4.2. Functional Anomalies
Although the use of neuroimaging techniques has implicated a variety of brain regions in schizophrenia, the emphasis in functional imaging studies has been and continues to be on the frontal lobes. The finding of hypofrontality has been supported by meta-analysis both at rest and under neuropsychological activation (216). Schizophrenic patients perform poorly on working memory tests and these deficits are consistently associated with prefrontal cortex dysfunction, although the basis of this abnormality is unknown. The convergent findings of neuroimaging and pathological studies give strong support to a functional limbic prefrontal network dysfunction in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia, with probably a volumetric alteration of limbic structures more specific to the anterior hippocampus, which would correspond to the ventral hippocampus in animals (8, 139, 217–220). The medial prefrontal cortex in rats is comparable to the ventromedial and subgenual prefrontal cortex in humans, regions which are particularly at risk in schizophrenia. Tamminga and Medoff (219) have shown that regional cerebral blood flow is abnormal in the hippocampal-anterior cingulate circuit in schizophrenic patients who cannot fully activate this circuit with cognitive tasks. Prefrontal cortical activity has been investigated in E17 MAM-exposed animals through in vivo field potential and singleunit recordings in adult anaesthetized rats. The absence of slow and fast oscillations added to an abnormal regular spike firing pattern in the medial prefrontal cortex (38) suggests an alteration in the inputs to the prefrontal cortex. Indeed an increased spike firing in response to ventral tegmental stimulation was observed in the prefrontal cortex of MAM-exposed rats when compared to controls. Using microinjection of either tetrodotoxin (TTX) to inactivate the ventral hippocampus or NMDA in the ventral hippocampus to activate dopamine transmission in the prefrontal cortex, Lodge and Grace have shown an aberrant dopamine response that could result from hippocampal dysfunction (42). Later on, they showed that the deficit in intrinsic GABAergic interneurons within the ventral subiculum may be at the origin of this dopamine dysfunction (41). Knowing the critical control of the mesocortical system on prefrontal circuits by simultaneously activating pyramidal neurons and fast spiking interneurons (221), it remains to be determined which prefrontal neuronal subpopulation is at the origin of the dopamine dysfunction observed in MAM-exposed animals.
68
Le Pen et al.
5. Conclusions In summary, data acquired from the last years have emphasized the face validity of the E17 MAM-exposed rat model for schizophrenia by showing its capacity to produce a behavioral phenotype that is reminiscent of positive, negative, and cognitive symptoms observed in schizophrenic patients. Anatomical and functional findings observed in the E17 MAM model indicate that there are significant similarities in neuropathological and imaging data in patients with schizophrenia and MAMexposed rats. However, studies investigating direct deficits in glutamatergic and GABAergic systems or dysfunctions in the interaction of glutamate and dopamine and GABA are still needed. It will also be interesting to directly compare with other models for schizophrenia, in particular genetically based model (such as mice deficient in DISC1 or in neuregulin) or other developmental model (e.g., neonatal lesion of the hippocampus or prenatal infection). Lastly, while most studies have focused on the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus, subcortical regions in the brain could also participate in the phenotype. The future will determine the precise cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying the anatomical and functional changes observed in the brains of adult MAM-exposed rats. This experimental model also provides an integrative approach to understanding the effects of a developmental insult that induces behavioral anomalies close to those observed in schizophrenia. It may help forward in establishing early preventive interventions that can successfully reduce the risk of developing schizophrenia in exposed individuals.
References 1. Mueser KT, McGurk SR. (2004) Schizophrenia. Lancet 363:2063–72. 2. Harrison PJ, Weinberger DR. (2005) Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology: on the matter of their convergence. Mol Psychiatry 10:40–68. 3. Rapoport JL, Addington AM, Frangou S, Psych MR. (2005) The neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: update 2005. Mol Psychiatry 10:434–49. 4. Waddington JL, Lane A, Larkin C, O’Callaghan E. (1999) The neurodevelopmental basis of schizophrenia: clinical clues from cerebro-craniofacial dysmorphogenesis, and the roots of a lifetime trajectory of disease. Biol Psychiatry 46:31–9.
5. Bramon E, Walshe M, McDonald C, et al. (2005) Dermatoglyphics and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis and investigation of the impact of obstetric complications upon a-b ridge count. Schizophr Res 75:399–404. 6. Sidman RL, Rakic P. (1973) Neuronal migration, with special reference to developing human brain: a review. Brain Res 62: 1–35. 7. Fletcher P. (1998) The missing link: a failure of fronto-hippocampal integration in schizophrenia. Nat Neurosci 1:266–7. 8. Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Suddath R, Torrey EF. (1992) Evidence of dysfunction of a prefrontal-limbic network in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging and
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
9. 10. 11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
regional cerebral blood flow study of discordant monozygotic twins. Am J Psychiatry 149:890–7. Friston KJ. (1998) The disconnection hypothesis. Schizophr Res 30(2):115–25. Schmidt-Kastner R, Freund TF. (1991) Selective vulnerability of the hippocampus in brain ischemia. Neuroscience 40:599–636. Weinberger DR. (1987) Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44:660–9. Benetti S, Mechelli A, Picchioni M, Broome M, Williams S, McGuire P. (2009) Functional integration between the posterior hippocampus and prefrontal cortex is impaired in both first episode schizophrenia and the at risk mental state. Brain 132:2426–36. Heckers S, Goff D, Schacter DL, et al. (1999) Functional imaging of memory retrieval in deficit vs nondeficit schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:1117–23. Meyer-Lindenberg AS, Olsen RK, Kohn PD, et al. (2005) Regionally specific disturbance of dorsolateral prefrontal-hippocampal functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:379–86. Zhou Y, Shu N, Liu Y, et al. (2008) Altered resting-state functional connectivity and anatomical connectivity of hippocampus in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 100: 120–32. Balduini W, Lombardelli G, Peruzzi G, Cattabeni F. (1991) Treatment with methylazoxymethanol at different gestational days: physical, reflex development and spontaneous activity in the offspring. Neurotoxicology 12:179–88. Cattabeni F, Di Luca M. (1997) Developmental models of brain dysfunctions induced by targeted cellular ablations with methylazoxymethanol. Physiol Rev 77: 199–215. Bayer S, Altman J. (2004) Development of the telencephalon: neural stem cells, neurogenesis and neuronal migration. In: Paxinos G, ed. The rat nervous system, 3rd edition. London: Academic, pp. 27–73. Singh SC. (1977) Ectopic neurones in the hippocampus of the postnatal rat exposed to methylazoxymethanol during foetal development. Acta Neuropathol 40:111–6. Battaglia G, Pagliardini S, Saglietti L, et al. (2003) Neurogenesis in cerebral heterotopia induced in rats by prenatal methylazoxymethanol treatment. Cereb Cortex 13:736–48. Colacitti C, Sancini G, DeBiasi S, et al. (1999) Prenatal methylazoxymethanol treat-
22.
23.
24.
25.
26. 27.
28.
29.
30.
31.
32.
69
ment in rats produces brain abnormalities with morphological similarities to human developmental brain dysgeneses. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 58:92–106. Colacitti C, Sancini G, Franceschetti S, et al. (1998) Altered connections between neocortical and heterotopic areas in methylazoxymethanol-treated rat. Epilepsy Res 32:49–62. Sancini G, Franceschetti S, Battaglia G, et al. (1998) Dysplastic neocortex and subcortical heterotopias in methylazoxymethanoltreated rats: an intracellular study of identified pyramidal neurones. Neurosci Lett 246:181–5. Matsumoto H, Spatz M, Laqueur GL. (1972) Quantitative changes with age in the DNA content of methylazoxymethanolinduced microencephalic rat brain. J Neurochem 19:297–306. Rodier PM. (1986) Behavioral effects of antimitotic agents administered during neurogenesis. In: Riley EP, Vorhees CV, eds. Handbook of behavioral teratology. New York, NY: Plenum Press, pp. 185–209. Spatz M, Dougherty WJ, Smith DW. (1967) Teratogenic effects of methylazoxymethanol. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 124:476–8. Johnston MV, Coyle JT. (1979) Histological and neurochemical effects of fetal treatment with methylazoxymethanol on rat neocortex in adulthood. Brain Res 170:135–55. Nagata Y, Matsumoto H. (1969) Studies on methylazoxymethanol: methylation of nucleic acids in the fetal rat brain. Proc Soc Exp Biol Med 132:383–5. Cattaneo E, Reinach B, Caputi A, Cattabeni F, Di Luca M. (1995) Selective in vitro blockade of neuroepithelial cells proliferation by methylazoxymethanol, a molecule capable of inducing long lasting functional impairments. J Neurosci Res 41:640–7. Featherstone RE, Rizos Z, Nobrega JN, Kapur S, Fletcher PJ. (2007) Gestational methylazoxymethanol acetate treatment impairs select cognitive functions: parallels to schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 32:483–92. Flagstad P, Glenthoj BY, Didriksen M. (2005) Cognitive deficits caused by late gestational disruption of neurogenesis in rats: a preclinical model of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:250–60. Flagstad P, Mork A, Glenthoj BY, Van Beek J, Michael-Titus AT, Didriksen M. (2004) Disruption of neurogenesis on gestational day 17 in the rat causes behavioral changes relevant to positive and negative schizophrenia symptoms and alters amphetamine-induced
70
33.
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
41.
42.
43.
Le Pen et al. dopamine release in nucleus accumbens. Neuropsychopharmacology 29:2052–64. Gourevitch R, Rocher C, Le Pen G, Krebs MO, Jay TM. (2004) Working memory deficits in adult rats after prenatal disruption of neurogenesis. Behav Pharmacol 15: 287–92. Hazane F, Krebs MO, Jay TM, Le Pen G. (2009) Behavioral perturbations after prenatal neurogenesis disturbance in female rat. Neurotox Res 15:311–20. Le Pen G, Gourevitch R, Hazane F, Hoareau C, Jay TM, Krebs MO. (2006) Peri-pubertal maturation after developmental disturbance: a model for psychosis onset in the rat. Neuroscience 143:395–405. Moore H, Jentsch JD, Ghajarnia M, Geyer MA, Grace AA. (2006) A neurobehavioral systems analysis of adult rats exposed to methylazoxymethanol acetate on E17: implications for the neuropathology of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 60:253–64. Penschuck S, Flagstad P, Didriksen M, Leist M, Michael-Titus AT. (2006) Decrease in parvalbumin-expressing neurons in the hippocampus and increased phencyclidineinduced locomotor activity in the rat methylazoxymethanol (MAM) model of schizophrenia. Eur J Neurosci 23:279–84. Goto Y, Grace AA. (2006) Alterations in medial prefrontal cortical activity and plasticity in rats with disruption of cortical development. Biol Psychiatry 60:1259–67. Lavin A, Moore HM, Grace AA. (2005) Prenatal disruption of neocortical development alters prefrontal cortical neuron responses to dopamine in adult rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:1426–35. Lena I, Chessel A, Le Pen G, Krebs MO, Garcia R. (2007) Alterations in prefrontal glutamatergic and noradrenergic systems following MK-801 administration in rats prenatally exposed to methylazoxymethanol at gestational day 17. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 192:373–83. Lodge DJ, Behrens MM, Grace AA. (2009) A loss of parvalbumin-containing interneurons is associated with diminished oscillatory activity in an animal model of schizophrenia. J Neurosci 29:2344–54. Lodge DJ, Grace AA. (2007) Aberrant hippocampal activity underlies the dopamine dysregulation in an animal model of schizophrenia. J Neurosci 27: 11424–30. Powell CM, Miyakawa T. (2006) Schizophrenia-relevant behavioral testing in rodent models: a uniquely human disorder? Biol Psychiatry 59:1198–207.
44. Rebec GV, Grabner CP, Johnson M, Pierce RC, Bardo MT. (1997) Transient increases in catecholaminergic activity in medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens shell during novelty. Neuroscience 76:707–14. 45. Crow TJ. (1980) Positive and negative schizophrenic symptoms and the role of dopamine. Br J Psychiatry 137:383–6. 46. Guillin O, Abi-Dargham A, Laruelle M. (2007) Neurobiology of dopamine in schizophrenia. Int Rev Neurobiol 78:1–39. 47. Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A. (1999) Dopamine as the wind of the psychotic fire: new evidence from brain imaging studies. J Psychopharmacol 13:358–71. 48. Arguello PA, Gogos JA. (2006) Modeling madness in mice: one piece at a time. Neuron 52:179–96. 49. Angrist B, van Kammen DP. (1984) CNS stimulants as a tool to study schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci 7:388–90. 50. Javitt DC, Zukin SR. (1991) Recent advances in the phencyclidine model of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 148:1301–8. 51. Jentsch JD, Roth RH. (1999) The neuropsychopharmacology of phencyclidine: from NMDA receptor hypofunction to the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 20:201–25. 52. Lieberman JA, Kane JM, Alvir J. (1987) Provocative tests with psychostimulant drugs in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 91:415–33. 53. Luby ED, Cohen BD, Rosenbaum G, Gottlieb JS, Kelley R. (1959) Study of a new schizophrenomimetic drug; sernyl. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry 81:363–9. 54. Malhotra AK, Pinals DA, Adler CM, et al. (1997) Ketamine-induced exacerbation of psychotic symptoms and cognitive impairment in neuroleptic-free schizophrenics. Neuropsychopharmacology 17:141–50. 55. Steinpreis RE. (1996) The behavioral and neurochemical effects of phencyclidine in humans and animals: some implications for modeling psychosis. Behav Brain Res 74: 45–55. 56. Wolkin A, Sanfilipo M, Angrist B, et al. (1994) Acute D-amphetamine challenge in schizophrenia: effects on cerebral glucose utilization and clinical symptomatology. Biol Psychiatry 36:317–25. 57. Jones P, Rodgers B, Murray R, Marmot M. (1994) Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet 344:1398–402. 58. Sams-Dodd F, Lipska BK, Weinberger DR. (1997) Neonatal lesions of the rat ventral hippocampus result in hyperlocomotion and
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol)
59. 60.
61.
62.
63. 64.
65. 66.
67.
68. 69.
70.
71.
72.
deficits in social behaviour in adulthood. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 132:303–10. Weinberger DR, Gallhofer B. (1997) Cognitive function in schizophrenia. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 12:S29–36. Hughes C, Kumari V, Soni W, et al. (2003) Longitudinal study of symptoms and cognitive function in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 59:137–46. Joober R, Rouleau GA, Lal S, et al. (2002) Neuropsychological impairments in neuroleptic-responder vs. -nonresponder schizophrenic patients and healthy volunteers. Schizophr Res 53:229–38. Sharma T. (1999) Cognitive effects of conventional and atypical antipsychotics in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 44–51. Braff DL. (1993) Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 19:233–59. Nuechterlein KH, Dawson ME, Green MF. (1994) Information-processing abnormalities as neuropsychological vulnerability indicators for schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 384:71–9. Perry W, Braff DL. (1994) Informationprocessing deficits and thought disorder in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 151:363–7. Perry W, Geyer MA, Braff DL. (1999) Sensorimotor gating and thought disturbance measured in close temporal proximity in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:277–81. Gold JM, Carpenter C, Randolph C, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. (1997) Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:159–65. Goldman-Rakic PS. (1994) Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 6:348–57. Silver H, Feldman P, Bilker W, Gur RC. (2003) Working memory deficit as a core neuropsychological dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 160: 1809–16. Manoach DS. (2003) Prefrontal cortex dysfunction during working memory performance in schizophrenia: reconciling discrepant findings. Schizophr Res 60: 285–98. Floresco SB, Seamans JK, Phillips AG. (1997) Selective roles for hippocampal, prefrontal cortical, and ventral striatal circuits in radial-arm maze tasks with or without a delay. J Neurosci 17:1880–90. Fleming K, Goldberg TE, Binks S, Randolph C, Gold JM, Weinberger DR. (1997) Visu-
73.
74.
75.
76. 77.
78.
79.
80.
81. 82. 83. 84.
71
ospatial working memory in patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 41:43–9. Packard MG, Hirsh R, White NM. (1989) Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems. J Neurosci 9:1465–72. Holcomb LA, Gordon MN, Jantzen P, Hsiao K, Duff K, Morgan D. (1999) Behavioral changes in transgenic mice expressing both amyloid precursor protein and presenilin-1 mutations: lack of association with amyloid deposits. Behav Genet 29:177–85. Wall PM, Messier C. (2002) Infralimbic kappa opioid and muscarinic M1 receptor interactions in the concurrent modulation of anxiety and memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 160:233–44. Lalonde R. (2002) The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:91–104. van Haaren F, De Bruin JP, Heinsbroek RP, Van de Poll NE. (1985) Delayed spatial response alternation: effects of delay-interval duration and lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex on response accuracy of male and female Wistar rats. Behav Brain Res 18:41–9. van Haaren F, van Zijderveld G, van Hest A, de Bruin JP, van Eden CG, van de Poll NE. (1988) Acquisition of conditional associations and operant delayed spatial response alternation: effects of lesions in the medial prefrontal cortex. Behav Neurosci 102: 481–8. Diamond A. (2002) Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In: Stuss D, Knight RT, eds. Principles of frontal lobe functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 466–503. Keefe RS, Roitman SE, Harvey PD, et al. (1995) A pen-and-paper human analogue of a monkey prefrontal cortex activation task: spatial working memory in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 17:25–33. Park S, Holzman PS. (1992) Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Arch Gen Psychiatry 49:975–82. Morris R. (1984) Developments of a watermaze procedure for studying spatial learning in the rat. J Neurosci Methods 11:47–60. Braff DL, Swerdlow NR. (1997) Neuroanatomy of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 23:509–12. Cornblatt BA, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. (1985) Global attentional deviance as a marker of risk for schizophrenia: specificity and predictive validity. J Abnorm Psychol 94:470–86.
72
Le Pen et al.
85. Freedman R, Waldo M, Bickford-Wimer P, Nagamoto H. (1991) Elementary neuronal dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 4:233–43. 86. Higashima M, Urata K, Kawasaki Y, et al. (1998) P300 and the thought disorder factor extracted by factor-analytic procedures in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 44: 115–20. 87. Nuechterlein KH, Dawson ME. (1984) Information processing and attentional functioning in the developmental course of schizophrenic disorders. Schizophr Bull 10:160–203. 88. Strauss ME, Buchanan RW, Hale J. (1993) Relations between attentional deficits and clinical symptoms in schizophrenic outpatients. Psychiatry Res 47:205–13. 89. Erwin RJ, Turetsky BI, Moberg P, Gur RC, Gur RE. (1998) P50 abnormalities in schizophrenia: relationship to clinical and neuropsychological indices of attention. Schizophr Res 33:157–67. 90. Olincy A, Ross RG, Harris JG, et al. (2000) The P50 auditory event-evoked potential in adult attention-deficit disorder: comparison with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 47: 969–77. 91. Lubow RE, Gewirtz JC. (1995) Latent inhibition in humans: data, theory, and implications for schizophrenia. Psychol Bull 117: 87–103. 92. Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Hartston H, Perry W, Geyer MA. (1996) Latent inhibition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 20: 91–103. 93. Elvevag B, Weinberger DR, Suter JC, Goldberg TE. (2000) Continuous performance test and schizophrenia: a test of stimulusresponse compatibility, working memory, response readiness, or none of the above? Am J Psychiatry 157:772–80. 94. Ito M, Kanno M, Mori Y, Niwa S. (1997) Attention deficits assessed by continuous performance test and span of apprehension test in Japanese schizophrenic patients. Schizophr Res 23:205–11. 95. Miller MB, Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, Barnett EM. (1990) Schizophrenic deficit in span of apprehension. J Abnorm Psychol 99:313–6. 96. Rund BR, Oie M, Zeiner P, Sundet K. (1999) Span of apprehension in adolescents with schizophrenia or ADHD. Schizophr Res 40:257–9. 97. Cadenhead KS, Serper Y, Braff DL. (1998) Transient versus sustained visual channels in the visual backward masking deficits
98.
99.
100. 101.
102.
103. 104.
105.
106. 107.
108.
109.
of schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry 43:132–8. Saccuzzo DP, Braff DL, Sprock J, Sudik N. (1984) The schizophrenia spectrum: a study of the relationship among the Rorschach, MMPI, and visual backward masking. J Clin Psychol 40:1288–94. Bruder G, Kayser J, Tenke C, et al. (1999) Left temporal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia: event-related potential and behavioral evidence from phonetic and tonal dichotic listening tasks. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:267–76. Wishner J, Wahl O. (1974) Dichotic listening in schizophrenia. J Consult Clin Psychol 42:538–46. Swerdlow NR, Braff DL, Taaid N, Geyer MA. (1994) Assessing the validity of an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:139–54. Carli M, Robbins TW, Evenden JL, Everitt BJ. (1983) Effects of lesions to ascending noradrenergic neurones on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction task in rats; implications for theories of dorsal noradrenergic bundle function based on selective attention and arousal. Behav Brain Res 9: 361–80. Leonard JA. (1961) Choice reaction time experiments and information theory. London: Butterworths. Mirsky AF, Rosvold HE. (1960) The use of psychoactive drugs as a neuropsychological tool in studies of attention in man. In: Uhr I, Miller JG, eds. Drugs and behavior. New York, NY: Wiley, pp. 375–92. Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Pliskin NH, Podd MH. (1987) Further evidence for dementia of the prefrontal type in schizophrenia? A controlled study of teaching the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Arch Gen Psychiatry 44:1008–14. Birrell JM, Brown VJ. (2000) Medial frontal cortex mediates perceptual attentional set shifting in the rat. J Neurosci 20:4320–4. McAlonan K, Brown VJ. (2003) Orbital prefrontal cortex mediates reversal learning and not attentional set shifting in the rat. Behav Brain Res 146:97–103. Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. (1996) Primate analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: effects of excitotoxic lesions of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset. Behav Neurosci 110:872–86. Elliott R, McKenna PJ, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. (1995) Neuropsychological evidence for frontostriatal dysfunction in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 25:619–30.
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol) 110. Owen AM, Roberts AC, Hodges JR, Summers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. (1993) Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson’s disease. Brain 116:1159–75. 111. Chudasama Y, Passetti F, Rhodes SE, Lopian D, Desai A, Robbins TW. (2003) Dissociable aspects of performance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task following lesions of the dorsal anterior cingulate, infralimbic and orbitofrontal cortex in the rat: differential effects on selectivity, impulsivity and compulsivity. Behav Brain Res 146:105–19. 112. Passetti F, Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. (2002) The frontal cortex of the rat and visual attentional performance: dissociable functions of distinct medial prefrontal subregions. Cereb Cortex 12:1254–68. 113. Murphy ER, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. (2005) Local glutamate receptor antagonism in the rat prefrontal cortex disrupts response inhibition in a visuospatial attentional task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 179: 99–107. 114. Berman KF, Zec RF, Weinberger DR. (1986) Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. II. Role of neuroleptic treatment, attention, and mental effort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 43: 126–35. 115. Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. (1988) Probing prefrontal function in schizophrenia with neuropsychological paradigms. Schizophr Bull 14:179–83. 116. Morice R. (1990) Cognitive inflexibility and pre-frontal dysfunction in schizophrenia and mania. Br J Psychiatry 157:50–4. 117. Prentice KJ, Gold JM, Buchanan RW. (2008) The Wisconsin Card Sorting impairment in schizophrenia is evident in the first four trials. Schizophr Res 106:81–7. 118. Jazbec S, Pantelis C, Robbins T, Weickert T, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE. (2007) Intra-dimensional/extra-dimensional set-shifting performance in schizophrenia: impact of distractors. Schizophr Res 89: 339–49. 119. Pantelis C, Barber FZ, Barnes TR, Nelson HE, Owen AM, Robbins TW. (1999) Comparison of set-shifting ability in patients with chronic schizophrenia and frontal lobe damage. Schizophr Res 37:251–70. 120. Murray GK, Cheng F, Clark L, et al. (2008) Reinforcement and reversal learning in first-episode psychosis. Schizophr Bull 34: 848–55. 121. Waltz JA, Gold JM. (2007) Probabilistic reversal learning impairments in schizophre-
122.
123.
124.
125.
126.
127. 128.
129.
130.
131. 132.
133.
134.
73
nia: further evidence of orbitofrontal dysfunction. Schizophr Res 93:296–303. Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. (2003) Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to Pavlovian autoshaping and discrimination reversal learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal cortex. J Neurosci 23:8771–80. Clark L, Cools R, Robbins TW. (2004) The neuropsychology of ventral prefrontal cortex: decision-making and reversal learning. Brain Cogn 55:41–53. Kim J, Ragozzino ME. (2005) The involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in learning under changing task contingencies. Neurobiol Learn Mem 83:125–33. Seidman LJ, Yurgelun-Todd D, Kremen WS, et al. (1994) Relationship of prefrontal and temporal lobe MRI measures to neuropsychological performance in chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 35:235–46. Szeszko PR, Bilder RM, Lencz T, et al. (2000) Reduced anterior cingulate gyrus volume correlates with executive dysfunction in men with first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 43:97–108. Tulving E. (2000) Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol 53:1–25. Aleman A, Hijman R, de Haan EH, Kahn RS. (1999) Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 156:1358–66. Cirillo MA, Seidman LJ. (2003) Verbal declarative memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: from clinical assessment to genetics and brain mechanisms. Neuropsychol Rev 13:43–77. Goldberg TE, Gold JM. (1995) Neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia. In: Hirsch SR, Weinberger DR, eds. Schizophrenia. Oxford: Blackwell Science, pp. 146–62. Leavitt VM, Goldberg TE. (2009) Episodic memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychol Rev 19:312–23. Da Silva Costa V, Duchatelle P, Boulouard M, Dauphin F. (2009) Selective 5-HT6 receptor blockade improves spatial recognition memory and reverses age-related deficits in spatial recognition memory in the mouse. Neuropsychopharmacology 34: 488–500. Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA. (2005) Episodic-like memory in mice: simultaneous assessment of object, place and temporal order memory. Brain Res Protoc 16:10–9. Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA. (2005) Integrated memory for objects,
74
135.
136.
137. 138.
139.
140.
141.
142.
143.
144.
145.
146.
147.
Le Pen et al. places, and temporal order: evidence for episodic-like memory in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem 84:214–21. Ennaceur A, Delacour J. (1988) A new onetrial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1: Behavioral data. Behav Brain Res 31:47–59. Clare L, McKenna PJ, Mortimer AM, Baddeley AD. (1993) Memory in schizophrenia: what is impaired and what is preserved? Neuropsychologia 31:1225–41. Laws KR, Leeson VC, McKenna PJ. (2006) Domain-specific deficits in schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 11:537–56. Heckers S, Curran T, Goff D, et al. (2000) Abnormalities in the thalamus and prefrontal cortex during episodic object recognition in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 48:651–7. Heckers S, Rauch SL, Goff D, et al. (1998) Impaired recruitment of the hippocampus during conscious recollection in schizophrenia. Nat Neurosci 1:318–23. Brebion G, David AS, Pilowsky LS, Jones H. (2004) Recognition of visual stimuli and memory for spatial context in schizophrenic patients and healthy volunteers. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 26:1093–102. Elvevag B, Duncan J, McKenna PJ. (2000) The use of cognitive context in schizophrenia: an investigation. Psychol Med 30:885– 97. Joyce E, Hutton S, Mutsatsa S, et al. (2002) Executive dysfunction in first-episode schizophrenia and relationship to duration of untreated psychosis: the West London Study. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 43:s38–44. McGurk SR, Green MF, Wirshing WC, et al. (2004) Antipsychotic and anticholinergic effects on two types of spatial memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 68: 225–33. O’Donnell BF, Swearer JM, Smith LT, Nestor PG, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. (1996) Selective deficits in visual perception and recognition in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 153:687–92. Wood SJ, Proffitt T, Mahony K, et al. (2002) Visuospatial memory and learning in first-episode schizophreniform psychosis and established schizophrenia: a functional correlate of hippocampal pathology? Psychol Med 32:429–38. Kesner RP, Dakis M. (1993) Phencyclidine disrupts acquisition and retention performance within a spatial continuous recognition memory task. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 44:419–24. McDonald RJ, White NM. (1993) A triple dissociation of memory systems: hippocam-
148.
149.
150.
151.
152.
153.
154.
155. 156. 157.
158.
159.
160.
pus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum. Behav Neurosci 107:3–22. Thinus-Blanc C, Save E, Poucet B, Foreman N. (1996) Effects of parietal cortex lesions on spatial problem solving in the rat. Behav Brain Res 81:115–21. Akwa Y, Ladurelle N, Covey DF, Baulieu EE. (2001) The synthetic enantiomer of pregnenolone sulfate is very active on memory in rats and mice, even more so than its physiological neurosteroid counterpart: distinct mechanisms? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:14033–7. Dellu F, Fauchey V, Le Moal M, Simon H. (1997) Extension of a new two-trial memory task in the rat: influence of environmental context on recognition processes. Neurobiol Learn Mem 67:112–20. Dellu F, Mayo W, Cherkaoui J, Le Moal M, Simon H. (1992) A two-trial memory task with automated recording: study in young and aged rats. Brain Res 588:132–9. Conrad CD, Galea LA, Kuroda Y, McEwen BS. (1996) Chronic stress impairs rat spatial memory on the Y maze, and this effect is blocked by tianeptine pretreatment. Behav Neurosci 110:1321–34. Cyr M, Calon F, Morissette M, Di Paolo T. (2002) Estrogenic modulation of brain activity: implications for schizophrenia and Parkinson’s disease. J Psychiatry Neurosci 27:12–27. Chambers RA, Moore J, McEvoy JP, Levin ED. (1996) Cognitive effects of neonatal hippocampal lesions in a rat model of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 15:587–94. Martin PD, Berthoz A. (2002) Development of spatial firing in the hippocampus of young rats. Hippocampus 12:465–80. Olton DS, Becker JT, Handelmann GE. (1979) Hippocampus, space, and memory. Behav Brain Sci 2:313–65. Vorhees CV, Williams MT. (2006) Morris water maze: procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat Protoc 1:848–58. Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O’Keefe J. (1982) Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 297: 681–3. Ekstrom AD, Kahana MJ, Caplan JB, et al. (2003) Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature 425: 184–8. Burgess N, Maguire EA, O’Keefe J. (2002) The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron 35: 625–41.
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol) 161. O’Keefe J, Nadel L. (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Cambridge: Oxford University Press. 162. Folley BS, Astur R, Jagannathan K, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD. (2010) Anomalous neural circuit function in schizophrenia during a virtual Morris water task. Neuroimage 49:3373–84. 163. Hanlon FM, Weisend MP, Hamilton DA, et al. (2006) Impairment on the hippocampal-dependent virtual Morris water task in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 87: 67–80. 164. Akbarian S, Sucher NJ, Bradley D, et al. (1996) Selective alterations in gene expression for NMDA receptor subunits in prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics. J Neurosci 16:19–30. 165. Glantz LA, Lewis DA. (2000) Decreased dendritic spine density on prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57:65–73. 166. Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Zec RF. (1986) Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. I. Regional cerebral blood flow evidence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 43:114–24. 167. Bogerts B, Meertz E, Schonfeldt-Bausch R. (1985) Basal ganglia and limbic system pathology in schizophrenia. A morphometric study of brain volume and shrinkage. Arch Gen Psychiatry 42:784–91. 168. Csernansky JG, Joshi S, Wang L, et al. (1998) Hippocampal morphometry in schizophrenia by high dimensional brain mapping. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:11406–11. 169. Pilowsky LS, Bressan RA, Stone JM, et al. (2006) First in vivo evidence of an NMDA receptor deficit in medicationfree schizophrenic patients. Mol Psychiatry 11:118–9. 170. Abi-Dargham A, Gil R, Krystal J, et al. (1998) Increased striatal dopamine transmission in schizophrenia: confirmation in a second cohort. Am J Psychiatry 155:761–7. 171. Breier A, Su TP, Saunders R, et al. (1997) Schizophrenia is associated with elevated amphetamine-induced synaptic dopamine concentrations: evidence from a novel positron emission tomography method. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94:2569–74. 172. Laruelle M, Abi-Dargham A, van Dyck CH, et al. (1996) Single photon emission computerized tomography imaging of amphetamine-induced dopamine release in drug-free schizophrenic subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:9235–40. 173. Krystal JH, D’Souza DC, Mathalon D, Perry E, Belger A, Hoffman R. (2003) NMDA
174.
175.
176.
177.
178.
179.
180.
181.
182.
183.
184.
185.
75
receptor antagonist effects, cortical glutamatergic function, and schizophrenia: toward a paradigm shift in medication development. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 169:215–33. Laruelle M, Kegeles LS, Abi-Dargham A. (2003) Glutamate, dopamine, and schizophrenia: from pathophysiology to treatment. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1003:138–58. Beasley CL, Reynolds GP. (1997) Parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons are reduced in the prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics. Schizophr Res 24:349–55. Benes FM, Berretta S. (2001) GABAergic interneurons: implications for understanding schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 25:1–27. Lewis CM, Levinson DF, Wise LH, et al. (2003) Genome scan meta-analysis of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, part II: Schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet 73:34–48. Reynolds GP, Beasley CL. (2001) GABAergic neuronal subtypes in the human frontal cortex?development and deficits in schizophrenia. J Chem Neuroanat 22: 95–100. Reynolds GP, Zhang ZJ, Beasley CL. (2001) Neurochemical correlates of cortical GABAergic deficits in schizophrenia: selective losses of calcium binding protein immunoreactivity. Brain Res Bull 55:579–84. Tooney PA, Chahl LA. (2004) Neurons expressing calcium-binding proteins in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 28:273–8. Zhang ZJ, Reynolds GP. (2002) A selective decrease in the relative density of parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the hippocampus in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 55:1–10. Bertolino A, Esposito G, Callicott JH, et al. (2000) Specific relationship between prefrontal neuronal N-acetylaspartate and activation of the working memory cortical network in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 157:26–33. Grace AA. (2000) Gating of information flow within the limbic system and the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Brain Res Rev 31:330–41. Lewis DA, Pierri JN, Volk DW, Melchitzky DS, Woo TU. (1999) Altered GABA neurotransmission and prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 46:616–26. Selemon LD, Goldman-Rakic PS. (1999) The reduced neuropil hypothesis: a circuit based model of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 45:17–25.
76
Le Pen et al.
186. Koob GF, Sanna PP, Bloom FE. (1998) Neuroscience of addiction. Neuron 21:467–76. 187. Tzschentke TM, Schmidt WJ. (2000) Functional relationship among medial prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, and ventral tegmental area in locomotion and reward. Crit Rev Neurobiol 14:131–42. 188. Paxinos G, Watson C. (1998) The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates, 4th ed. San Diego: Academic Press. 189. Adams B, Moghaddam B. (1998) Corticolimbic dopamine neurotransmission is temporally dissociated from the cognitive and locomotor effects of phencyclidine. J Neurosci 18:5545–54. 190. Harkin A, Morris K, Kelly JP, O’Donnell JM, Leonard BE. (2001) Modulation of MK-801-induced behaviour by noradrenergic agents in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 154:177–88. 191. Lorrain DS, Baccei CS, Bristow LJ, Anderson JJ, Varney MA. (2003) Effects of ketamine and N-methyl-D-aspartate on glutamate and dopamine release in the rat prefrontal cortex: modulation by a group II selective metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist LY379268. Neuroscience 117: 697–706. 192. Loscher W, Honack D. (1992) The behavioural effects of MK-801 in rats: involvement of dopaminergic, serotonergic and noradrenergic systems. Eur J Pharmacol 215:199–208. 193. Ouagazzal A, Nieoullon A, Amalric M. (1993) Effects of dopamine D1 and D2 receptor blockade on MK-801-induced hyperlocomotion in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 111:427–34. 194. Takahata R, Moghaddam B. (2003) Activation of glutamate neurotransmission in the prefrontal cortex sustains the motoric and dopaminergic effects of phencyclidine. Neuropsychopharmacology 28: 1117–24. 195. Verma A, Moghaddam B. (1996) NMDA receptor antagonists impair prefrontal cortex function as assessed via spatial delayed alternation performance in rats: modulation by dopamine. J Neurosci 16:373–9. 196. Benes FM, Khan Y, Vincent SL, Wickramasinghe R. (1996) Differences in the subregional and cellular distribution of GABAA receptor binding in the hippocampal formation of schizophrenic brain. Synapse 22: 338–49. 197. Reynolds GP, Czudek C, Andrews HB. (1990) Deficit and hemispheric asymmetry of GABA uptake sites in the hippocampus in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 27:1038–44.
198. Benes FM, McSparren J, Bird ED, SanGiovanni JP, Vincent SL. (1991) Deficits in small interneurons in prefrontal and cingulate cortices of schizophrenic and schizoaffective patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 48: 996–1001. 199. Ohnuma T, Augood SJ, Arai H, McKenna PJ, Emson PC. (1999) Measurement of GABAergic parameters in the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: focus on GABA content, GABA(A) receptor alpha1 subunit messenger RNA and human GABA transporter-1 (HGAT-1) messenger RNA expression. Neuroscience 93: 441–8. 200. Akbarian S, Kim JJ, Potkin SG, et al. (1995) Gene expression for glutamic acid decarboxylase is reduced without loss of neurons in prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52:258–66. 201. Hashimoto T, Volk DW, Eggan SM, et al. (2003) Gene expression deficits in a subclass of GABA neurons in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with schizophrenia. J Neurosci 23:6315–26. 202. Volk DW, Austin MC, Pierri JN, Sampson AR, Lewis DA. (2000) Decreased glutamic acid decarboxylase67 messenger RNA expression in a subset of prefrontal cortical gamma-aminobutyric acid neurons in subjects with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57:237–45. 203. Benes FM, Kwok EW, Vincent SL, Todtenkopf MS. (1998) A reduction of nonpyramidal cells in sector CA2 of schizophrenics and manic depressives. Biol Psychiatry 44:88–97. 204. DeFelipe J. (1997) Types of neurons, synaptic connections and chemical characteristics of cells immunoreactive for calbindin-D28K, parvalbumin and calretinin in the neocortex. J Chem Neuroanat 14:1–19. 205. Daviss SR, Lewis DA. (1995) Local circuit neurons of the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: selective increase in the density of calbindin-immunoreactive neurons. Psychiatry Res 59:81–96. 206. Cotter D, Landau S, Beasley C, et al. (2002) The density and spatial distribution of GABAergic neurons, labelled using calcium binding proteins, in the anterior cingulate cortex in major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 51:377–86. 207. Reynolds GP, Beasley CL, Zhang ZJ. (2002) Understanding the neurotransmitter pathology of schizophrenia: selective deficits of subtypes of cortical GABAergic neurons. J Neural Transm 109:881–9.
Gestational MAM (Methylazoxymethanol) 208. Wheeler DG, Dixon G, Harper CG. (2006) No differences in calcium-binding protein immunoreactivity in the posterior cingulate and visual cortex: schizophrenia and controls. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 30:630–9. 209. Torrey EF, Barci BM, Webster MJ, Bartko JJ, Meador-Woodruff JH, Knable MB. (2005) Neurochemical markers for schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depression in postmortem brains. Biol Psychiatry 57: 252–60. 210. Lewis DA, Hashimoto T, Volk DW. (2005) Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:312–24. 211. Woo TU, Miller JL, Lewis DA. (1997) Schizophrenia and the parvalbumincontaining class of cortical local circuit neurons. Am J Psychiatry 154:1013–5. 212. Behrens MM, Ali SS, Dao DN, et al. (2007) Ketamine-induced loss of phenotype of fast-spiking interneurons is mediated by NADPH-oxidase. Science 318: 1645–7. 213. Harrison PJ. (1999) The neuropathology of schizophrenia. A critical review of the data and their interpretation. Brain 122: 593–624. 214. Matricon J, Bellon A, Frieling H, et al. (2010) Neuropathological and Reelin deficiencies in the hippocampal formation of rats exposed to MAM; differences and similarities with schizophrenia. PLoS One 5: e10291.
77
215. Garey L. (2010) When cortical development goes wrong: schizophrenia as a neurodevelopmental disease of microcircuits. J Anat 217(4):324–33. 216. Hill K, Mann L, Laws KR, Stephenson CM, Nimmo-Smith I, McKenna PJ. (2004) Hypofrontality in schizophrenia: a metaanalysis of functional imaging studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand 110:243–56. 217. Harrison PJ, Eastwood SL. (2001) Neuropathological studies of synaptic connectivity in the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia. Hippocampus 11:508–19. 218. Szeszko PR, Goldberg E, Gunduz-Bruce H, et al. (2003) Smaller anterior hippocampal formation volume in antipsychotic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 160:2190–7. 219. Tamminga CA, Medoff DR. (2002) Studies in schizophrenia: pathophysiology and treatment. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 4:432–7. 220. Tamminga CA, Vogel M, Gao X, Lahti AC, Holcomb HH. (2000) The limbic cortex in schizophrenia: focus on the anterior cingulate. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 31: 364–70. 221. Tseng KY, Mallet N, Toreson KL, Le Moine C, Gonon F, O’Donnell P. (2006) Excitatory response of prefrontal cortical fast-spiking interneurons to ventral tegmental area stimulation in vivo. Synapse 59:412–7.
Chapter 3 Prenatal Infection and Immune Models of Schizophrenia Alan S. Brown Abstract An increasing number of epidemiologic studies have implicated in utero exposure to infection in the etiopathogenesis of schizophrenia. Recent work has capitalized on the use of prospectively acquired data on infection based on maternal biomarkers. These studies suggest that several maternal infections, including rubella, influenza, toxoplasmosis, herpes simplex virus/other genital-reproductive infections, and elevations in the cytokines interleukin-8 and TNF-α, are associated with increased schizophrenia risk among offspring. Animal models of in utero infection offer the potential to corroborate these findings under controlled conditions and address etiopathogenic mechanisms. Models of maternal immune activation (MIA) and behavioral and brain anomalies in schizophrenia have primarily employed three agents: polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid (poly I:C), lipopolysaccharide, each of which mimic viral and bacterial infections, respectively, and direct maternal inoculation with influenza. Advantages and disadvantages of these approaches are discussed and the findings are reviewed. Each of these MIA models has yielded evidence, reviewed in detail, for neurochemical, neuropathologic, and behavioral anomalies in offspring that are consistent with phenotypes found in patients with schizophrenia. In addition, putative mechanisms by which MIA causes these phenotypes, the role of gestational timing of the insult, and long-term modulation of the immune system following prenatal immune challenge are discussed. Finally, the “back translation” of these findings to the design and implementation of new epidemiologic studies aimed at further refining and testing this hypothesis is discussed. Key words: Infection, immune, schizophrenia, influenza, animal models, cytokines, poly I:C, LPS, epidemiology, risk factors.
1. Introduction An increasing number of epidemiologic studies have implicated in utero exposure to infection in the etiopathogenesis of schizophrenia (1, 2). Although initial studies based on epidemics in populations reported conflicting findings, a new generation of investigations based on birth cohort studies has documented intriguing P. O’Donnell (ed.), Animal Models of Schizophrenia and Related Disorders, Neuromethods 59, DOI 10.1007/978-1-61779-157-4_3, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
79
80
Brown
associations between prenatal infection and risk of schizophrenia in offspring. These studies capitalized on prospective measures of maternal infection, or indicators of infection, to selected agents during pregnancy, follow-up of offspring, and diagnoses based on direct interviews, psychiatric records, and validated psychiatric registry data. Among other findings, this work revealed that exposure to rubella (3), influenza, elevated toxoplasma IgG antibody (4), periconceptional genital/reproductive infection (5), elevated HSV-2 IgG antibody (6), and increases in the pro-inflammatory cytokines interleukin-8 (7) and TNF-α (8) were associated with increases in risk of adult schizophrenia. The effect sizes in these studies were not trivial, with most ranging from approximately two- to fivefold, significantly higher than effect sizes for most individual susceptibility genes. Although replication of these findings is essential, these promising results suggest that in utero exposure to infection may play an important role in the etiology of schizophrenia. Yet, the biological basis of these findings remains unknown.
2. Rationale for Studies of Animal Models of In Utero Infection
2.1. Maternal Immune Activation Models: Poly I:C and LPS
Animal models of in utero infection offer the potential to provide corroborative evidence that under controlled conditions the associations between infection and schizophrenia-like pathology or behavior can be validated, and address etiopathogenic mechanisms by which in utero exposure to infection can increase liability to schizophrenia. Inspired by the epidemiologic findings reviewed above, a proliferation of such “reverse translational” studies has emerged in recent years. There are two primary types of models of in utero infection that have been tested in paradigms of schizophrenia: noninfectious maternal immune activation and a direct infectious insult. We describe each of these study designs and findings in turn. The first approach relies on induction of a maternal immune response which mimics that of an actual infection. Two agents have been used to stimulate immune activity in animal models of schizophrenia: poly I:C and lipopolysaccharide (LPS). Poly I:C. The first immune-activating agent is the synthetic polymer poly I:C (polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid), which bears structural similarity to double-stranded RNA. Poly I:C acts by binding to the toll-like receptor TLR3, which is expressed intracellularly in B cells and dendritic cells. It is generally thought
Prenatal Infection and Immune Models of Schizophrenia
81
that poly I:C most closely mimics a viral infection. Poly I:C stimulates a potent immune response involving marked increases in cytokines, including interleukin-6 (IL-6), interleukin-8 (IL-8), interleukin-1-β (IL-1-β), tumor necrosis factor-α (TNFα), interferon-γ (IFN-γ), and interleukin-10 (IL-10). Studies have demonstrated that maternal administration of poly I:C increases several of these cytokines in the placenta, and the fetus, including the fetal brain (9–13). Moreover, IL-6 response genes were demonstrated to be upregulated in fetal brain following maternal poly I:C injection (13, 14). Lipopolysaccharide (LPS). LPS is a large molecule which contains a lipid and a polysaccharide moiety and is found in the outer membranes of gram-negative bacteria, and is therefore believed to most closely simulate a bacterial infection. This agent is an endotoxin, which stimulates the release of pro-inflammatory cytokines. Like poly I:C, maternal administration of LPS increases cytokine secretion in several compartments in the maternal and fetal circulation, and in the fetal brain. Maternal administration of LPS has also been related to changes in brain-derived neurotrophic factors and nerve growth factor expression in the fetal brain (15). 2.2. Advantages of Poly I:C and LPS Models
The use of poly I:C and LPS in animal models of maternal immune activation has several advantages. First, unlike the influenza model, the timing of exposure can be precisely controlled experimentally. The maternal and fetal cytokine response to a single injection of poly I:C appears to be limited to a period of no more than several hours. Consequently, it is possible to narrow the timing of the effect of immune activation to a particular period of gestation. Second, both models allow for testing a nonspecific model of maternal immune activation common to different infections, such as activation of cytokines and other components of the immune response without the confounding influences of varying consequences of different infections. Third, it is not believed that influenza and certain other infections that have been implicated in schizophrenia in epidemiologic studies act by direct effects on the fetus. As noted below, influenza does not appear to be transmitted to the fetal brain. For certain maternal infections that have been associated with schizophrenia in offspring, such as toxoplasma (4) and HSV-2 (8, 16), infection during pregnancy is unlikely to have occurred; hence, it is more likely that the causative factor is the immune response to these agents. MIA models are especially informative for modeling these infections since most cytokines examined in animal model studies have been shown to cross the placenta. Fourth, both poly I:C and LPS circumvent the precautions that need to be taken for studies involving infectious micro-organisms and are therefore safer and more convenient in experimental studies.
82
Brown
3. Experimental Protocols 3.1. Experimental Protocol: Poly I:C
In order to elaborate the paradigm, we provide an example of the methodologic approach, based on a study by Shi et al. (17). In this study, BALB/c mice that were pregnant on gestational day 9.5 were inoculated by intraperitoneal injections with poly I:C diluted in PBS at dosages ranging from 20 to 0 mg/kg. The offspring of these mice were separated from their mothers after 3 weeks and male and female mice were caged separately. Mice were tested at different ages ranging from adolescence to adulthood, depending upon the protocol. Testing included exploratory behavior and anxiety by open-field and novel object tests, spontaneous locomotor activity by a photobeam activity system, sensorimotor coordination by a Rota-rod Treadmill, social behavior by examining social interaction between pairs of mice, and pre-pulse inhibition to the acoustic startle response using a standard protocol.
3.2. Experimental Protocol: LPS
The methodologic approach for these animal models of schizophrenia is exemplified by a recent publication from Romero et al. (18). In this study, LPS from Escherichia coli was dissolved in saline and subcutaneously administered to pregnant rats at a dosage of 2 mg/kg. Injections were administered daily between days 1 and 21 of pregnancy. Controls were administered saline alone. At 21 days, offspring were weaned and caged in selected groups of animals of the same prenatal treatment and sex. Rats from LPS and control groups were tested in the acoustic startle PPI paradigm at 6 months of age. One day after PPI testing, animals were decapitated, and brains were removed, frozen, and stored for neurochemical assays. Tissue dissections were performed for sampling of the nucleus accumbens, caudate nucleus, putamen, frontal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala. Samples from nucleus accumbens and neostriatum were homogenized for subsequent high-performance liquid chromatography assays for dopamine and dopamine metabolites. Additional studies of PPI were conducted with and without treatment with haloperidol (see (18) for details).
3.3. Experimental Protocol: Influenza
To exemplify the experimental approach, we discuss the method used in a study by Fatemi et al. (19). In this study, anesthetized pregnant female C57BL/6 mice on 9 days after breeding were inoculated by intranasal instillation of influenza A/NWS/33 (H1N1) virus. Control mice were exposed to sterile virus diluent using the same protocol as the infected animals. Pregnant mice delivered pups and groups of infected and sham-infected neonates were anesthetized. Brains were removed following birth,
Prenatal Infection and Immune Models of Schizophrenia
83
cryopreserved, and coronal sections were cut. Cell counting was conducted for pyramidal and non-pyramidal cell density and pyramidal nuclear size was obtained. In a second study, offspring were allowed to mature to various ages from adolescence to adulthood for testing using the same paradigms as in Shi et al. (17). Of note, loss of pregnancy was not observed to be common so long as the mice were not disturbed. Mothers were noted to have mild lung consolidation with viral titers ranging from 3 to 4 × 103 pfu soon after infection, which became undetectable, and fever was not observed. 3.4. Inoculation with an Infectious Microbe: Influenza
In this model, pregnant animals are inoculated with an infectious microbe, and their offspring are studied for the behaviors or psychophysiologic functions in question. To date, inoculation with influenza is the only known animal model involving a microbial pathogen aimed specifically at investigating schizophrenia. There are two main advantages of this paradigm. First, this model attempts to closely emulate the naturalistic experience involving maternal exposure to the pathogenic microbe in humans. Since it is unclear which of several potential consequences of influenza result in an increased risk of schizophrenia, exposure to the actual infection ensures that the animal is exposed to most or all of these contributory components. This includes, but is not limited to, the respiratory consequences, the full inflammatory response, including elevations of pro-inflammatory cytokines and specific immunoglobulin antibodies (along with the time course by which these antibodies become elevated), and other maternal physiologic changes resulting from these consequences. Second, this model is based on the postulate that if in utero infection plays a role in schizophrenia, not all infectious agents will be implicated. Using this approach, an investigator could theoretically test for a wide array of pathogens to evaluate their specificity for causing brain and behavioral alterations found in schizophrenia. There are three main disadvantages of this approach. First, the infection can last as long as several days after inoculation and the duration of infection can vary between individual animals. This makes it difficult to control the gestational period of susceptibility, a problem that is especially critical in studies of rodents in which the period of gestation is markedly compressed relative to humans. Second, influenza RNA and nucleoprotein corresponding to the serotype of influenza have been identified in fetal brain from pregnancies with maternal inoculation with influenza (20), indicating placental and blood–brain barrier transfer of the virus. This differs from that of the natural influenza serotype in human populations, in which influenza has only rarely been documented in fetal brains of exposed pregnancies in humans. Third, extensive care must be taken to prevent exposure to the infectious agent in
84
Brown
laboratory personnel, and the methods to prevent exposure and to successfully execute the research paradigm are more complex than alternative approaches discussed below.
4. Review of Study Findings from Animal Models of Schizophrenia
4.1. Findings from Poly I:C Models
Below, we provide a summary of findings that have emerged from animal models of influenza and maternal immune activation. For a more comprehensive review of these findings, we refer the reader to Patterson et al. (10). A plethora of studies have documented phenotypes believed to resemble those observed in schizophrenia following gestational exposure to poly I:C. Patterson and colleagues have demonstrated that impaired PPI, social interaction deficits, and reduced novel object exploration have each been observed in poly I:C-exposed mice (17). Similarities in these outcomes to mice exposed in utero to influenza (see “section 4.3” below) suggest that at least these behaviors may be mediated by the maternal immune response to this infection, in the absence of the virus itself. Several additional findings have been observed among offspring of poly I:C-exposed mice. These include abnormalities in latent inhibition, working memory, and disturbances of dopamine-related functions including enhanced amphetamine-induced locomotion, increased striatal dopamine release, and altered reversal learning (10). Interestingly, in two studies, amphetamine-induced anomalies were not observed in pre-pubertal offspring but emerged during adulthood, reminiscent of the delayed onset of psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia (21, 22). Other findings that relate to a disturbance of the dopamine system include an elevation of tyrosine hydroxylase in the dorsal striatum and the shell of the nucleus accumbens (23), a reduction of dopamine type 1 and type 2 receptors in the medial prefrontal cortex (23), and an elevation of DOPAC in the striatum, suggesting increased dopamine turnover (22). Additional evidence of neurochemical dysfunction has been observed in glutamatergic and GABA-related systems in poly I:C-exposed offspring. An increase in locomotion following administration of MK-801, an NMDA receptor antagonist, has been observed in adult mice in three studies (23–25), and this abnormality was not observed pre-pubertally, similar to the amphetamine-related findings. These results point to NMDA-related dysfunction that appears to be analogous to findings from the extant literature on this receptor in schizophrenia. Moreover, abnormalities in immunohistochemical markers for GABAergic neurons have been demonstrated, including a reduction of reelin and parvalbumin
Prenatal Infection and Immune Models of Schizophrenia
85
in the medial prefrontal cortex, and an elevation of GABA-α-2 receptor subunits in the ventral hippocampus (25). Offspring exposed to poly I:C also evidenced neuropathologic abnormalities. These include disruptions in postnatal neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus, increased apoptosis, and moderate to severe cell loss in the CA1 region of the hippocampus (21). Maternal poly I:C exposure also led to ventricular enlargement, the most well-replicated structural anomaly in schizophrenia (14). Curiously, treatment with antipsychotic medications in pre-pubertal animals exposed to gestational poly I:C prevented ventricular enlargement and other anomalies (26). 4.2. Findings from LPS Models
In utero treatment with LPS appears to produce some of the same behavioral deficits observed with poly I:C. These include PPI deficits (18), which were corrected with administration of antipsychotics (18, 27), and a disruption of social interaction and learning. Dopaminergic abnormalities include amphetamine-induced hyperlocomotion and increases in DOPAC and dopamine in the striatum (18), as well as decreases in tyrosine hydroxylase neurons in the substantia nigra (28). LPS also caused astrogliosis, evidenced by increased glial fibrillary acid protein (GFAP) staining and changes in microglial immunostaining (29).
4.3. Findings from Influenza Models
Fatemi and colleagues (19) have shown that exposure to influenza during mid-pregnancy causes increased packing of pyramidal cells, alterations in reelin, SNAP-25, and nNOS expression. Increased density of pyramidal cells has also been observed. Offspring exposed to influenza in mid-gestation also evidence a decreased number of cerebellar Purkinje cells and a disruption of neuronal migration to layers 2 and 3 of the cortex (30). Patterson and colleagues have demonstrated behavioral abnormalities in influenzaexposed offspring, including social interaction deficits, decreased PPI, and reduced exploration in the open field and in response to novel objects (30).
4.4. Mechanisms of Maternal Immune Activation and In Utero Infection
A key question raised by the MIA and influenza models concerns the mechanisms by which this in utero factor alters fetal brain development in such a way that it leads to the behavioral, histologic, neuroanatomic, and neurochemical abnormalities reviewed above. It is first worth considering the maternal– fetal compartments that are affected by the insult. Studies have demonstrated that maternal serum pro-inflammatory cytokine levels are increased following exposure to poly I:C and LPS (10). Maternal LPS administration has also been demonstrated to increase cytokine levels in the placenta and the amniotic fluid (9, 10). For poly I:C, several cytokines were shown to be increased in the fetal brain (13), and an increase in fetal expression of the IL-6 gene has also been demonstrated. Hence, it is
86
Brown
conceivable that MIA causes a cascade of events that begin with the maternal immune response and that lead to an elevation in fetal cytokine levels either through placental passage or via activation of a fetal immune response. Another essential question that studies are beginning to address is whether certain cytokines are responsible for the observed effects. Patterson and colleagues have attempted to isolate the role of IL-6 in the brain and behavioral anomalies observed following MIA (31). These investigators found that co-injection of maternal poly I:C and an antibody which neutralized the effect of IL-6 blocked the observed behavioral abnormalities following MIA. This antibody also blocked alterations in gene transcription in the fetal brain that were induced by poly I:C. In a model which utilized intracerebral injection of LPS, ventriculomegaly was diminished and behavioral outcomes were improved, following administration of IL-6 antibody (32). In an extension of this work to IL-6 knockout mice, MIA with poly I:C had no effect on behavior. In a similar vein, genetic overexpression of interleukin-10, and anti-inflammatory cytokine, by macrophages was shown to diminish both behavioral and pharmacological effects of MIA with poly I:C (33). However, when there was no inflammatory stimulus, increased maternal/fetal IL-10 levels were associated with behavioral anomalies in offspring. These intriguing results argue for the importance of a balance between pro- and anti-inflammatory effects of in utero cytokines on behavioral outcomes. 4.5. Gestational Timing of Infection
The identification of windows of vulnerability is of critical importance toward understanding the etiopathogenic effects of MIA and influenza infection, and perhaps of in utero exposures in schizophrenia. Meyer et al. (13) demonstrated that the timing of in utero immune activation in mice may have differential effects on the observed brain and behavioral phenotypes. For example, poly I:C on gestational day 9 (roughly equivalent to midpregnancy in humans), but not gestational day 17 (roughly equivalent to late pregnancy), diminished spatial exploration, while MIA treatment on gestational day 17, but not gestational day 9, resulted in perseverative behavior. Exposure to MIA on GD9, but not GD17, resulted in an impairment in latent inhibition associative learning. MIA in mid-pregnancy caused a more pronounced decrease in hippocampal reelin expression, while exposure during late pregnancy, but not mid-pregnancy, caused apoptosis. These findings suggest an interaction between exposure to MIA and the specific developmental stage of the brain at the time of induction of the exposure. Moreover, these authors also suggest that differences in the fetal brain cytokine response to MIA may contribute to these varying effects.
Prenatal Infection and Immune Models of Schizophrenia
4.6. Long-Term Immune Modulation Following MIA and In Utero Infection
5. “Back Translation” to Epidemiology
87
Another particularly exciting avenue of research is addressing the question of whether MIA and in utero infection alter fetal programming such that it produces long-term effects on the expression of cytokines, and of other molecules implicated in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. An extensive literature has documented abnormalities in peripheral cytokine levels in patients with schizophrenia, though these findings conflict to some degree. Although there are many potential causes for these abnormalities, including infections of childhood or adult onset, the possibility remains that in utero cytokines lead to permanent alterations in gene expression later in life, analogous to fetal programming hypotheses for obesity and hypertension (34). In this regard, the findings of Smith et al. (31), which demonstrated that IL-6 caused alterations in gene expression in adult animals, are particularly intriguing. This group has also demonstrated that MIA results in an upregulation of several IL-6 response genes in fetal brain (14). As previously discussed, it would clearly be of benefit to examine whether this upregulation persists into adulthood and contributes to the observed brain and behavioral anomalies.
Animal models of MIA and in utero infection provide one of the most instructive examples of how epidemiologic research can be translated to basic neuroscience investigations in order to validate observed phenotypes and elucidate causal mechanisms. Given the proliferation of highly informative animal models, it is now worth considering whether the knowledge gained thus far from studies of animal models has implications for the design and implementation of new epidemiologic studies. We suggest three potential avenues of research that may result from such findings. First, as described in detail above, the animal models have documented a variety of brain and behavioral phenotypes that are associated with MIA. For obvious practical reasons, the epidemiologic studies, however, have restricted the phenotypic outcome to the diagnosis of schizophrenia. Research efforts are underway in an attempt to transcend traditional diagnostic classification by assessing neuromorphometric and neuropsychological outcomes in individuals from birth cohorts in which prospective, biomarker-based documentation of in utero infection was obtained. Early studies have demonstrated that in utero exposure to infection is associated with executive function deficits, including anomalies of cognitive set-shifting and working memory (35),
88
Brown
and with neuromorphometric anomalies, including cavum septum pellucidum (36), which is indicative of in utero brain developmental disruption. One of the caveats of this work is that the samples have been generally small and most of the phenotypes observed in the basic neuroscience literature were not testable, given practical constraints. However, it is conceivable that several of the psychophysiologic and neuromorphometric anomalies currently being examined in the literature on animal models can be directly examined in some of the large birth cohort studies currently underway. Second, as reviewed above, the literature on animal models has revealed differential sensitivity to MIA based on the gestational timing of infection. Although challenging, the potential to investigate this question is plausible in an epidemiologic design. Third, studies of animal models have potential to reveal new candidate cytokines, profiles of cytokine release, or related molecules, which can be tested in maternal or neonatal sera in epidemiologic designs. Fourth, work on alterations in gene expression by MIA may be informative in the selection of candidate genes that can be investigated for interactions between infectious exposures and maternal/infant cytokines. Recent evidence from genome-wide association studies (GWAS) indicating a small but significant association between genetic variants in the extended major histocompatibility complex (MHC) (37–39) suggests that inclusion of data in both in utero infection and immune-related genes in informative samples followed up for schizophrenia may yield significantly larger effect sizes. These samples include prospective birth cohorts with available biological specimens dating back to the in utero period. Presently, efforts are underway to better understand the relationship between in utero exposure to infections and immune disturbances in far larger birth cohorts that have been studied to date.
6. Summary Epidemiologic studies based on in utero biomarkers have revealed tantalizing evidence that prenatal infection may increase the risk of schizophrenia. Inspired by these epidemiologic data, a plethora of studies of animal models of in utero infection have emerged. These studies include maternal challenge with the immuneactivating agents poly I:C and LPS and inoculation with influenza, and assessment of offspring at various stages of development. These studies suggest that prenatal immune activation and infection cause brain and behavioral alterations that are analogous to some phenotypes observed in schizophrenia. Although the causal mechanisms are not well understood, it is clear that MIA
Prenatal Infection and Immune Models of Schizophrenia
89
induces elevated cytokine levels in both the maternal and fetal compartments, including the fetal brain. Among the cytokines investigated to date in these models, it appears that IL-6 may play a particularly important role in the induction of brain and behavioral anomalies in offspring. There appears to be differential vulnerability to certain outcomes of MIA depending upon the gestational period of infection. The potential that these outcomes are transduced by early life programming changes by immune activation and infection is clearly worthy of further exploration. These studies may also enrich epidemiologic studies by a “backtranslational” approach in which cytokines and other molecules shown to be related to immune stimulation in animal models can be examined in prospective studies of informative birth cohorts. This work promises to shed light on the etiologies of schizophrenia, on the pathogenic mechanisms that give rise to this devastating condition, and on new molecular targets for treatment.
Acknowledgments This manuscript was supported by the following grants: National Institutes of Mental Health (NIMH) 1R01MH-60249 (A.S.B), NIMH 2K02MH065422-06 (A.S.B.). References 1. Brown, A. S. (2006) Prenatal infection as a risk factor for schizophrenia, Schizophr Bull 32, 200–202. 2. Penner, J. D., and Brown, A. S. (2007) Prenatal infectious and nutritional factors and risk of adult schizophrenia, Expert Rev Neurother 7, 797–805. 3. Brown, A. S., Cohen, P., Harkavy-Friedman, J., Babulas, V., Malaspina, D., Gorman, J. M., and Susser, E. S. (2001) A.E. Bennett Research Award. Prenatal rubella, premorbid abnormalities, and adult schizophrenia, Biol Psychiatry 49, 473–486. 4. Brown, A. S., Schaefer, C. A., Quesenberry, C. P., Jr., Liu, L., Babulas, V. P., and Susser, E. S. (2005) Maternal exposure to toxoplasmosis and risk of schizophrenia in adult offspring, Am J Psychiatry 162, 767–773. 5. Babulas, V., Factor-Litvak, P., Goetz, R., Schaefer, C. A., and Brown, A. S. (2006) Prenatal exposure to maternal genital and reproductive infections and adult schizophrenia, Am J Psychiatry 163, 927–929.
6. Brown, A. S., Schaefer, C. A., Quesenberry, C. P., Jr., Shen, L., and Susser, E. S. (2006) No evidence of relation between maternal exposure to herpes simplex virus type 2 and risk of schizophrenia, Am J Psychiatry 163, 2178–2180. 7. Brown, A. S., Hooton, J., Schaefer, C. A., Zhang, H., Petkova, E., Babulas, V., Perrin, M., Gorman, J. M., and Susser, E. S. (2004) Elevated maternal interleukin-8 levels and risk of schizophrenia in adult offspring, Am J Psychiatry 161, 889–895. 8. Buka, S. L., Tsuang, M. T., Torrey, E. F., Klebanoff, M. A., Wagner, R. L., and Yolken, R. H. (2001) Maternal cytokine levels during pregnancy and adult psychosis, Brain Behav Immun 15, 411–420. 9. Jonakait, G. M. (2007) The effects of maternal inflammation on neuronal development: possible mechanisms, Int J Dev Neurosci 25, 415–425. 10. Patterson, P. H., Xu, W., Smith, S. E. P., Deverman, B. E. (2008) Maternal immune
90
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
Brown activation, cytokines and autism, in Autism: current theories and evidence (289–308) (Zimmerman, A. W., Ed.), Humana Press, Totowa, NJ. Smith, S., and Patterson, P. (2009) Alteration of neurodevelopment and behavior by maternal immune activation, in The neuroimmunological basis of behavior and mental disorders (111–130) (Siegal, A., and Zalcman, S. S., Eds.), Springer, Norwell, MA. Meyer, U., Feldon, J., and Yee, B. K. (2008) A review of the fetal brain cytokine imbalance hypothesis of schizophrenia, Schizophr Bull 35, 959–972. Meyer, U., Nyffeler, M., Engler, A., Urwyler, A., Schedlowski, M., Knuesel, I., Yee, B. K., and Feldon, J. (2006) The time of prenatal immune challenge determines the specificity of inflammation-mediated brain and behavioral pathology, J Neurosci 26, 4752–4762. Patterson, P. H. (2008) Immune involvement in schizophrenia and autism: Etiology, pathology and animal models, Behav Brain Res 204, 313–321. Gilmore, J. H., Jarskog, L. F., and Vadlamudi, S. (2003) Maternal infection regulates BDNF and NGF expression in fetal and neonatal brain and maternal-fetal unit of the rat, J Neuroimmunol 138, 49–55. Buka, S. L., Cannon, T. D., Torrey, E. F., and Yolken, R. H. (2008) Maternal exposure to herpes simplex virus and risk of psychosis among adult offspring, Biol Psychiatry 63, 809–815. Shi, L., Fatemi, S. H., Sidwell, R. W., and Patterson, P. H. (2003) Maternal influenza infection causes marked behavioral and pharmacological changes in the offspring, J Neurosci 23, 297–302. Romero, E., Ali, C., Molina-Holgado, E., Castellano, B., Guaza, C., and Borrell, J. (2007) Neurobehavioral and immunological consequences of prenatal immune activation in rats. Influence of antipsychotics, Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 1791–1804. Fatemi, S. H., Earle, J., Kanodia, R., Kist, D., Emamian, E. S., Patterson, P. H., Shi, L., and Sidwell, R. (2002) Prenatal viral infection leads to pyramidal cell atrophy and macrocephaly in adulthood: implications for genesis of autism and schizophrenia, Cell Mol Neurobiol 22, 25–33. Aronsson, F., Lannebo, C., Paucar, M., Brask, J., Kristensson, K., and Karlsson, H. (2002) Persistence of viral RNA in the brain of offspring to mice infected with influenza A/WSN/33 virus during pregnancy, J Neurovirol 8, 353–357.
21. Zuckerman, L., Rehavi, M., Nachman, R., and Weiner, I. (2003) Immune activation during pregnancy in rats leads to a postpubertal emergence of disrupted latent inhibition, dopaminergic hyperfunction, and altered limbic morphology in the offspring: a novel neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia, Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 1778–1789. 22. Ozawa, K., Hashimoto, K., Kishimoto, T., Shimizu, E., Ishikura, H., and Iyo, M. (2006) Immune activation during pregnancy in mice leads to dopaminergic hyperfunction and cognitive impairment in the offspring: a neurodevelopmental animal model of schizophrenia, Biol Psychiatry 59, 546–554. 23. Meyer, U., Nyffeler, M., Schwendener, S., Knuesel, I., Yee, B. K., and Feldon, J. (2008) Relative prenatal and postnatal maternal contributions to schizophrenia related neurochemical dysfunction after in utero immune challenge, Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 441–456. 24. Zuckerman, L., and Weiner, I. (2005) Maternal immune activation leads to behavioral and pharmacological changes in the adult offspring, J Psychiatr Res 39, 311–323. 25. Meyer, U., Nyffeler, M., Yee, B. K., Knuesel, I., and Feldon, J. (2008) Adult brain and behavioral pathological markers of prenatal immune challenge during early/middle and late fetal development in mice, Brain Behav Immun 22, 469–486. 26. Meyer, U., Spoerri, E., Yee, B. K., Schwarz, M. J., and Feldon, J. (2008) Evaluating early preventive antipsychotic and antidepressant drug treatment in an infectionbased neurodevelopmental mouse model of schizophrenia, Schizophr Bull 36, 607–623. 27. Borrell, J., Vela, J. M., Arevalo-Martin, A., Molina-Holgado, E., and Guaza, C. (2002) Prenatal immune challenge disrupts sensorimotor gating in adult rats. Implications for the etiopathogenesis of schizophrenia, Neuropsychopharmacology 26, 204–215. 28. Ling, Z., Chang, Q. A., Tong, C. W., Leurgans, S. E., Lipton, J. W., and Carvey, P. M. (2004) Rotenone potentiates dopamine neuron loss in animals exposed to lipopolysaccharide prenatally, Exp Neurol 190, 373–383. 29. Cai, Z., Pan, Z. L., Pang, Y., Evans, O. B., and Rhodes, P. G. (2000) Cytokine induction in fetal rat brains and brain injury in neonatal rats after maternal lipopolysaccharide administration, Pediatr Res 47, 64–72.
Prenatal Infection and Immune Models of Schizophrenia 30. Shi, L., Smith, S. E., Malkova, N., Tse, D., Su, Y., and Patterson, P. H. (2009) Activation of the maternal immune system alters cerebellar development in the offspring, Brain Behav Immun 23, 116–123. 31. Smith, S. E., Li, J., Garbett, K., Mirnics, K., and Patterson, P. H. (2007) Maternal immune activation alters fetal brain development through interleukin-6, J Neurosci 27, 10695–10702. 32. Pang, Y., Fan, L. W., Zheng, B., Cai, Z., and Rhodes, P. G. (2006) Role of interleukin-6 in lipopolysaccharide-induced brain injury and behavioral dysfunction in neonatal rats, Neuroscience 141, 745–755. 33. Meyer, U., Murray, P. J., Urwyler, A., Yee, B. K., Schedlowski, M., and Feldon, J. (2008) Adult behavioral and pharmacological dysfunctions following disruption of the fetal brain balance between pro-inflammatory and IL-10-mediated anti-inflammatory signaling, Mol Psychiatry 13, 208–221. 34. Godfrey, K. M., and Barker, D. J. (2001) Fetal programming and adult health, Pub Health Nutr 4, 611–624. 35. Brown, A. S., Vinogradov, S., Kremen, W. S., Poole, J. H., Deicken, R. F., Penner, J. D., McKeague, I. W., Kochetkova, A., Kern, D., and Schaefer, C. A. (2009) Prenatal exposure to maternal infection and executive dysfunction in adult schizophrenia, Am J Psychiatry 166, 683–690. 36. Brown, A. S., Deicken, R. F., Vinogradov, S., Kremen, W. S., Poole, J. H., Penner, J. D., Kochetkova, A., Kern, D., and Schaefer, C. A. (2009) Prenatal infection and cavum septum pellucidum in adult schizophrenia, Schizophr Res 108, 285–287. 37. Shi, J., Levinson, D. F., Duan, J., Sanders, A. R., Zheng, Y., Pe’er, I., Dudbridge, F., Holmans, P. A., Whittemore, A. S., Mowry, B. J., Olincy, A., Amin, F., Cloninger, C. R., Silverman, J. M., Buccola, N. G., Byerley, W. F., Black, D. W., Crowe, R. R., Oksenberg, J. R., Mirel, D. B., Kendler, K. S., Freedman, R., and Gejman, P. V. (2009) Common variants on chromosome 6p22.1 are associated with schizophrenia, Nature 460, 753–757. 38. Purcell, S. M., Wray, N. R., Stone, J. L., Visscher, P. M., O’Donovan, M. C., Sullivan, P. F., Sklar, P., Purcell Leader, S. M., Ruderfer, D. M., McQuillin, A., Morris, D. W., O’Dushlaine, C. T., Corvin, A., Holmans, P. A., Macgregor, S., Gurling, H., Blackwood, D. H., Craddock, N. J., Gill, M., Hultman, C. M., Kirov, G. K., Lichtenstein, P., Muir,
91
W. J., Owen, M. J., Pato, C. N., Scolnick, E. M., St Clair, D., Sklar Leader, P., Williams, N. M., Georgieva, L., Nikolov, I., Norton, N., Williams, H., Toncheva, D., Milanova, V., Thelander, E. F., Sullivan, P., Kenny, E., Quinn, E. M., Choudhury, K., Datta, S., Pimm, J., Thirumalai, S., Puri, V., Krasucki, R., Lawrence, J., Quested, D., Bass, N., Crombie, C., Fraser, G., Leh Kuan, S., Walker, N., McGhee, K. A., Pickard, B., Malloy, P., Maclean, A. W., Van Beck, M., Pato, M. T., Medeiros, H., Middleton, F., Carvalho, C., Morley, C., Fanous, A., Conti, D., Knowles, J. A., Paz Ferreira, C., Macedo, A., Helena Azevedo, M., Kirby, A. N., Ferreira, M. A., Daly, M. J., Chambert, K., Kuruvilla, F., Gabriel, S. B., Ardlie, K., and Moran, J. L. (2009) Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, Nature 460, 748–752. 39. Stefansson, H., Ophoff, R. A., Steinberg, S., Andreassen, O. A., Cichon, S., Rujescu, D., Werge, T., Pietilainen, O. P., Mors, O., Mortensen, P. B., Sigurdsson, E., Gustafsson, O., Nyegaard, M., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Ingason, A., Hansen, T., Suvisaari, J., Lonnqvist, J., Paunio, T., Borglum, A. D., Hartmann, A., Fink-Jensen, A., Nordentoft, M., Hougaard, D., Norgaard-Pedersen, B., Bottcher, Y., Olesen, J., Breuer, R., Moller, H. J., Giegling, I., Rasmussen, H. B., Timm, S., Mattheisen, M., Bitter, I., Rethelyi, J. M., Magnusdottir, B. B., Sigmundsson, T., Olason, P., Masson, G., Gulcher, J. R., Haraldsson, M., Fossdal, R., Thorgeirsson, T. E., Thorsteinsdottir, U., Ruggeri, M., Tosato, S., Franke, B., Strengman, E., Kiemeney, L. A., Group, Melle, I., Djurovic, S., Abramova, L., Kaleda, V., Sanjuan, J., de Frutos, R., Bramon, E., Vassos, E., Fraser, G., Ettinger, U., Picchioni, M., Walker, N., Toulopoulou, T., Need, A. C., Ge, D., Lim Yoon, J., Shianna, K. V., Freimer, N. B., Cantor, R. M., Murray, R., Kong, A., Golimbet, V., Carracedo, A., Arango, C., Costas, J., Jonsson, E. G., Terenius, L., Agartz, I., Petursson, H., Nothen, M. M., Rietschel, M., Matthews, P. M., Muglia, P., Peltonen, L., St Clair, D., Goldstein, D. B., Stefansson, K., Collier, D. A., Kahn, R. S., Linszen, D. H., van Os, J., Wiersma, D., Bruggeman, R., Cahn, W., de Haan, L., Krabbendam, L., and Myin-Germeys, I. (2009) Common variants conferring risk of schizophrenia, Nature 460, 744–747.
Chapter 4 The Hypoxic Rat Model for Obstetric Complications in Schizophrenia Andrea Schmitt, Peter Gebicke-Haerter, Ulrich Sommer, Markus Heck, Anja Lex, Mario Herrera-Marschitz, Mathias Zink, Markus Fendt, and Peter Falkai Abstract Hypoxia has been discussed as a possible factor of obstetric complications in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. This study investigated the effects of chronic neonatal hypoxia in rats as an animal model of schizophrenia. Methods: (1) After chronic neonatal hypoxia between postnatal day (PD) 4 and 8, half of the pups were fostered by normally treated nurse animals to control for possible maternal effects and (2) tested on PD 36, 86, 120, and 150 using three different behavioral tests: prepulse inhibition (PPI), social interaction and recognition, and motor activity in an open field. (3) Before the PD 150 test, 50% of the animals had been chronically treated with the antipsychotic drug clozapine (45 mg/kg/day). (4) At PD 155, different brain regions have been used for expression profiling of synaptic genes on cDNA microarrays (“glutamate chip”) with qRT-PCR confirmation. Additionally, at PD 11 and 120, NMDA receptor binding and expression of NMDA receptor subunits have been performed. Rats exposed to hypoxia exhibited deficits in locomotor activity on PD 86, 120, and 150, as well as a PPI deficit on PD 120 and 150 in adulthood, but not before. Chronic treatment with clozapine reversed hypoxia-induced PPI deficits, but not the decreased locomotor activity. In a second experiment, where clozapine was chronically administered before PD 120, development of the PPI deficit in the animals exposed to hypoxia was prevented. In several brain regions, presynaptic genes such as SNAP-25, syntaxin 1A, neurexin, neuropeptide Y, and complexin I were downregulated and the NR1 subunit of the NMDA receptor was upregulated by hypoxia. These differential gene regulations could be partially compensated for by clozapine treatment. NMDA receptor binding was decreased at PD 11 and expression of the NR1 subunit was increased at PD 11 and 120. The time course of hypoxia-induced PPI deficits and their reversal by clozapine support the validity of our animal model and the hypothesis that hypoxia as a factor of obstetric complications plays a role in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Differential gene expression in cortical and subcortical brain regions as well as correlations to deficits of PPI support the view of an involvement of synapse-associated gene products and glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission in the pathophysiology of behavioral deficits occurring as delayed responses to neonatal hypoxia in adulthood. Key words: Neonatal hypoxia, schizophrenia, animal model, PPI, gene expression, clozapine, presynaptic genes, NMDA receptor.
P. O’Donnell (ed.), Animal Models of Schizophrenia and Related Disorders, Neuromethods 59, DOI 10.1007/978-1-61779-157-4_4, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
93
94
Schmitt et al.
1. Introduction In addition to genetic predisposition, the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia includes a number of environmental factors like maternal infection or obstetric and birth complications with ensuing neuronal damage in the pathophysiology of the disease (Fig. 4.1). Hence, the risk of schizophrenia increases with the number and severity of hypoxia-associated obstetric complications (1–3). Furthermore, twin studies revealed fetal hypoxia as a predictor of reduced cortical gray matter and extended ventricular volume (4), smaller hippocampus sizes (5, 6), and early age of onset (7) in schizophrenia patients (8). In the early postnatal period, the developing rat brain is highly vulnerable to hypoxic damage (9). In contrast to humans, important steps of brain development in rats occur mainly postnatally and the brain growth spurt of early postnatal rats is comparable to the third trimester in humans (10). At this stage, overexpression of NMDA receptors has been observed reaching peak levels at postnatal day 8 (11–13). This resembles the peak in NMDA receptor binding and mRNA expression in gestational stages in the human fetus (14, 15). The tetrameric receptor is composed of several distinct subunits, the obligatory NR1 and several facultative NR2 subunits (NR2A, NR2B, NR2C, and NR2D), responsible for different functional properties (16, 17). During postnatal development, NMDA receptors are composed of more NR2B than NR2A subunits and the NR2D subunit is most prominent around postnatal day 7 (17). NMDA receptors
Fig. 4.1. Risk genes and environmental factors such as hypoxia play a role in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia during the perinatal period, whereas symptoms occur in young adulthood after the synaptic pruning process and wiring of the prefrontal cortex. In some patients with a chronic disease, further neurodegenerative changes with volume loss in several brain regions may play an additional role.
Hypoxic Rat Model for Obstetric Complications in Schizophrenia
95
assembled from NR2B or NR2D subunits show higher affinity for the co-agonist glycine than NMDA receptors with more NR2A subunits in the adult brain. They are also less prone to blockade by magnesium than more mature channels (13). During brain development, NMDA receptors are involved in synaptic plasticity, as well as migration of neurons to their final destination (18–22). In this respect, the early postnatal period of rats is most suitable for investigating the role of hypoxia in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia, as it represents a highly vulnerable time period in brain development. In schizophrenia, NMDA receptor hypofunction has been hypothesized, but the etiology of this dysfunction remains elusive. An early developmental event has been proposed to be involved in the process leading to NMDA receptor hypofunction (23). Hypoxia may be part of this course of events and is investigated in the present animal model. This may be additive with genetic factors, many of which involve the area of glutamatergic transmission (24). We have shown that postnatal, repeated hypoxia can precipitate alterations in NMDA receptor binding and gene expression of subunits early after the event and in adulthood. Indeed, in our animal model of chronic neonatal hypoxia, rats at PD 120 exhibited – along with the PPI deficit – increased gene expression of the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor subunit NR1 in hippocampal, frontal, and temporal subregions as well as in nucleus accumbens (25), which suggests an involvement of the NMDA receptor in the pathophysiology of hypoxia-induced alterations. The pathogenetic implications of obstetric complications are unclear. One hypothesis suggests that obstetric complications are caused by neurodevelopmental pathology (26–28), another speculates that obstetric complications itself cause developmental neuropathology (26, 29). Either argument is consistent with the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia, which suggests that abnormal brain development and neuropathology occur perinatally, whereas symptoms of the disease appear in early adulthood (8). Thus, if a common factor of obstetric complications such as perinatal hypoxia causes neurodevelopmental disturbances, an animal model of neonatal hypoxia should express schizophrenia-related behavioral alterations in early adulthood of the affected rats. Schizophrenia patients show deficits in attention and processing of sensory information. A reliable model of sensorimotor gating in vertebrates is prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic startle response, which is disrupted in schizophrenia patients and restored by antipsychotics, mainly clozapine (30). In rats, PPI also reflects sensorimotor gating, and several brain regions such as hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, and striatum are involved in PPI-modulating circuitry (31). In a neurodevelopmental animal model in rats, neonatal hippocampal lesion led to decreased PPI, increased locomotor
96
Schmitt et al.
activity after amphetamine challenge, and disturbed social interaction in early adulthood (32), while medial prefrontal lesions led to attenuated locomotor response (33). Additional animal models would be important to further elucidate possible roles of neurodevelopmental factors in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Thus, the present approach is meant to mirror the impact of chronic hypoxia during human obstetric and birth complications in more detail. Exposure to anoxia at postnatal day (PD) 9 showed no effects on the PPI paradigm in adulthood (34). However, it must be kept in mind that the duration of the anoxia period of the study be kept relatively short, and long-term hypoxia may indeed affect behavior in adulthood. In a recent study applying chronic neonatal hypoxia, rats at PD 120 exhibited a PPI deficit and increased gene expression of the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor subunit NR1 at PD 11 and 120 in hippocampal, frontal, and temporal subregions as well as in nucleus accumbens (25). This points to the involvement of the NMDA receptor in the pathophysiology of hypoxia-induced alterations. In another study, Hermans and Longo (35) exposed rat pups to nearly the same oxygen concentrations for 6 h/day and showed increased rearing activity and stereotypies in hypoxia-treated animals. However, in our study, hypoxia-treated rats have been closely examined from the preand postpubertal phase until adulthood and effects of antipsychotic treatment have been examined. Rats exposed to hypoxia exhibited deficits in locomotor activity on PD 86, 120, and 150, as well as a PPI deficit on PD 120 and 150 in adulthood, but not before. Chronic treatment with clozapine reversed hypoxiainduced PPI deficits, but not the decreased locomotor activity. In a second experiment, where clozapine was chronically administered before PD 120, development of the PPI deficit in the animals exposed to hypoxia was prevented (36). Here we show that schizophrenia-related behavioral alterations occur in early adulthood of the affected rats. Schizophrenic patients have deficits in processing of sensory information and attention. A reliable model of sensorimotor gating is prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle response (PPI), which is disrupted in schizophrenic patients and restored by antipsychotics (30, 37). In a neurodevelopmental animal model in rats, postnatal hippocampal lesion led to decreased PPI in early adulthood (38). Additional animal models would be important to further elucidate possible roles of neurodevelopmental factors in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Thus, our approach is meant to mirror in more detail the impact of chronic hypoxia during human obstetric and birth complications. In schizophrenia, there is strong support for disturbances of the glutamatergic and GABAergic microcircuitry, mainly affecting
Hypoxic Rat Model for Obstetric Complications in Schizophrenia
97
the expression of presynaptic vesicle proteins including SNAP25 and syntaxin, which form the SNARE complex (39–48). The trimeric SNARE complex consists of the vesicle protein synaptobrevin (VAMP) and the plasma membrane proteins syntaxin and SNAP-25, which drive membrane fusion by guiding the vesicle and plasma membrane proteins in close proximity, thus overcoming the energy barrier for fusion. Complexin I (in inhibitory and excitatory synapses) and complexin II (in excitatory synapses) operate at a post-priming step in synaptic vesicle exocytosis by stabilizing the SNARE complex (49). Neuropeptide Y and neurexins play a role in cell adhesion and are hypothesized to play a role in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (45, 50–52). Little is known about the influence of chronic hypoxia on the expression of those and other presynaptic proteins. Snails exposed to hypoxia showed a decreased expression of syntaxin 1 (53), but long-term effects of perinatal hypoxia on expression of these genes in mammals and their contribution to behavioral deficits in adulthood are unknown. Additionally, there is only sparse information about the influence of antipsychotic treatment on gene expression, as has been shown for neuropeptide Y and complexins in normal rats (54, 55). In our last study, we showed that in several brain regions, presynaptic genes such as SNAP-25, syntaxin 1A, neurexin, neuropeptide Y, and complexin I were downregulated and the NR1 subunit of the NMDA receptor was upregulated by hypoxia. These differential gene regulations could be partially compensated for by clozapine treatment (56). Differential gene expression in cortical and subcortical brain regions as well as correlations to deficits of PPI support the view of an involvement of synapse-associated gene products and glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission in the pathophysiology of behavioral deficits occurring as delayed responses to neonatal hypoxia in adulthood.
2. Materials 1. Air-tight hypoxia chamber 2. Oxymeter (GMH3690, Conrad, Hirschau, Germany) 3. Startle device (for example, TSE systems, Germany, or San Diego Instruments, USA) 4. Arena (ActiMot, TSE Systems, Bad Homburg, Germany) 5. Rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson (63))
98
Schmitt et al.
6. Cryostat (Leica) 7. Image analysis system (Inter Active Systems, Interfocus, Germany) R , Amersham, Germany) 8. [3 H]-sensitive film (Hyperfilm
9. [3 H] plastic standards (Amersham, Germany) 10.
35 S-sensitive
11.
14 C
X-ray films (Biomax MR1)
plastic standards (Amersham, Germany)
12. Clozapine (Novartis, Switzerland) 13. RNA extraction kit (RNeasy, Qiagen, Germany) 14. Brain Punch Set (Föhr Medical Instruments, Germany) 15. Agilent 2100 Bioanalyzer 16. Packard ScanArray 5000 scanner 17. Fast Real-Time PCR cycler (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA)
3. Methods 3.1. Animals
A total of 116 Sprague Dawley rats from 12 different litters (bred from 12 females and 5 males; Animal Facility, Department of Animal Physiology, University of Tübingen) were used for the experiments. The rats were housed in groups of three to four animals under a 12 h light/dark cycle (light on at 7 a.m.) with food and drinking water available ad libitum. The experiments were carried out in accordance with the international ethical guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals for experiments (Declaration of Helsinki, NIH guidelines) and were approved by the local animal care committee (Regierungspräsidium Tübingen, ZP 3/02).
3.1.1. Neonatal Hypoxia
From postnatal day (PD) 4–8, we imposed mild chronic hypoxia (11% O2 , 89% N2 ) on six litters (n = 64) and their mothers by placing them in an air-tight plastic chamber (31 × 40 × 15 cm3 ) for a period of 6 h/day (Fig. 4.2). The six other litters (n = 52) were subjected to identical handling conditions but were placed in identical chambers with regular oxygen concentrations (21% O2 = normoxia). In both chambers, the oxygen concentration was controlled by an oxygen measuring device (GMH3690, Conrad, Hirschau, Germany). During hypoxia, mothers were able to care for their pups. Unsystematic observations revealed no obvious effects of hypoxia on maternal behavior during hypoxia or normoxia.
Hypoxic Rat Model for Obstetric Complications in Schizophrenia
99
Fig. 4.2. Air-tight hypoxia chamber with oxymeter and separate oxygen and nitrogen inflow.
3.1.2. Cross-Fostering
To control for possible effects of hypoxia on maternal behavior, the offspring of the four litters in experiment 1 (n = 62) were cross-fostered on PD 9, meaning that half of the offspring of a hypoxia-treated litter were exchanged with half of the offspring of a normoxia-treated litter. All mothers accepted the unknown offspring without problems. At an age of 4–5 weeks, the young rats were separated from the mother or nurse, respectively, and were housed in groups of the same gender under the conditions described above.
3.2. Testing Procedure
Behavioral tests were carried out at three or four different age levels: between PD 34 and 38, between PD 84 and 88, between PD 118 and 122, and between PD 148 and 152 (only experiment 1). For practical purposes, these test phases are named PD 36, PD 86, PD 120, and PD 150 throughout the manuscript. During each test phase (lasting 1 week), every rat was tested first on motor activity in an open field (day 1 or 2 of the test week), second on social interaction and social recognition (day 3 or 4), and third on baseline startle magnitude and prepulse inhibition (day 4 or 5, for details see below). All behavioral tests were carried out in the light phase between 9 a.m. and 4 p.m. During the last two test phases (experiment 1: between PD 120 and PD 150, experiment 2: between PD 86 and PD 120), half of the rats were being treated over a period of 4 weeks with ca. 45 mg/kg/day clozapine (Novartis Pharma AG, Basel, Switzerland) diluted in some drops of HCl in the drinking water adjusted to pH 6.5. Control rats received some drops of HCl in the drinking water to match the pH levels of the drug solutions. The drug dosage was calculated by the amount of ingested water and body weight, which was measured every day and the dose of 45 mg/kg/day
100
Schmitt et al.
is an estimate of the average daily intake over the whole period of administration. This route of administration leads to clozapine plasma levels of ca. 22 ng/ml (57), a level which is shown to be pharmacologically active (57). Furthermore, clozapine, using the same doses and the same route of administration, has been shown to alter the glutamatergic and GABAergic system in rat brain (58–61). 3.2.1. Test on Motor Activity
Spontaneous motor activity was tested in a square arena with walls made from transparent Plexiglass (92 × 92 × 39 cm; ActiMot, TSE Systems, Bad Homburg, Germany) where animals were placed over a 10 min period. Illumination of the arena was between 200 and 250 lux. Rat movements were monitored by infrared detectors (distance between two detectors: 2.5 cm and height: 2.5 cm) and rearings were detected by a second arrangement of infrared detectors (height: 12.5 cm). The ActiMot software automatically calculated motor activity (distance traveled in meters), number of rearings, and total time spent in the middle of the arena (size of the middle: 46 cm × 46 cm).
3.2.2. Test on Social Interaction and Social Recognition
Juvenile rats, ca. 30 days old, were used as a social stimulus. Both the test rats and the social stimulus rats were housed in isolation for 1 h before testing. For testing on social interaction (62), both groups of rats were placed in a cage (38 cm × 22 cm × 15 cm) where their behavior could be observed via camera and monitor. The duration of all social behavior (anogenital sniffing, social sniffing, social grooming, play fights) of our test rats within a 5 min period was recorded by an observer who was not aware of the group (anoxia/normoxia, clozapine treatment) to which the observed animal belonged. The animals were then separated and kept in isolation for a period of 30 min. Thereafter, a second test on social interaction between these two rats was carried out. Test values for social recognition were expressed as the ratio of the time spent on social interaction during the second test divided by the time spent on social interaction during the first test.
3.2.3. Test on Baseline Startle Magnitude and Prepulse Inhibition
The startle magnitude was measured with four identical acoustic startle devices in sound-attenuated chambers. For the tests, rats were placed in wire mesh cages with steel floors (20 × 10 × 12 cm3 ) which were fixed onto piezoelectric accelerometers. Movements of the rats caused changes in the voltage output of the accelerometers. These voltage changes were amplified, digitized, and analyzed by a PC. The startle magnitude was computed as the difference of the accelerometer output 80 ms before and 80 ms after the onset of the startling stimulus. Acoustic stimuli were generated by a function synthesizer (Hortmann, Germany) and delivered through loudspeakers mounted 40 cm away from the test cages. Intensity was measured using a 0.5 in. condenser
Hypoxic Rat Model for Obstetric Complications in Schizophrenia
101
microphone and a measuring amplifier (Brüel and Kjaer, Denmark) after bandpass (0.25–80 kHz) filtering. After a 5 min adaptation period, during which the rats received no stimuli except the background noise, the startle test session began with five initial startle stimuli (100 dB SPL broadband noise pulse, 20 ms duration) followed by four different trial types presented in a pseudorandom order: (1) Startle pulse alone (100 dB sound pressure level (SPL) broadband noise pulse, 20 ms duration), (2) 75 dB prepulse (75 dB SPL, 10 kHz tone pulse, 20 ms duration including 0.4 ms rise/fall times) followed by a startle pulse 100 ms after prepulse onset, (3) 75 dB prepulse alone, and (4) no stimulus. Background noise intensity was 55 dB SPL. A total of five presentations of each trial type was given with an interstimulus interval of 20 s. PPI was measured as the difference between the pulse-alone trials and the prepulse–pulse trials and expressed as percent PPI [100 × (mean ASR amplitude on pulse-alone trials – mean ASR amplitude on prepulse–pulse trials)/mean ASR amplitude on pulse-alone trials]. Experiment 2. The aim of experiment 2 was to test whether chronic clozapine treatment before the onset of the behavioral symptoms induced by neonatal hypoxia (PD 120) prevents the development of these symptoms. To minimize factors, the animals of this experiment (n = 54) were not cross-fostered after neonatal hypoxia or normoxia (experiment 1 showed that this factor has no influence on the hypoxia effect) and only male rats were used. Behavioral testing was identical to experiment 1, except that our tests on motor activity were carried out in a cage (55 cm × 32 cm × 19 cm) with one infrared sensor delivering an arbitrary measure of motor activity (“counts”) being used. Unfortunately, this setup did not allow us to measure rearing behavior. 3.3. Brain Investigations
Brains were removed, frozen in liquid nitrogen-cooled isopentane at –80◦ C, and stored for 3 months. For cryostat sections, brains were processed at –20◦ C. Deep-frozen brains were brought to –12◦ C the day before cutting. Cryostat temperature was maintained at –16◦ C. Adjacent coronal 15 μm sections (receptor autoradiography) and 20 μm sections (in situ hybridization) were thaw-mounted on superfrost plus microscopic slides (Roth, Germany), dried, and stored at –20◦ C for less than 5 days (receptor binding). Sections for in situ hybridization were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde, dehydrated in ethanol, and stored at –20◦ C for several weeks. For the microarray experiments, slices of 120 μm thickness were used to excise respective brain regions using a micro-punching tool. The resultant cylindrical tissue blocks were collected in –78◦ C test tubes (on dry ice) and stored at –80◦ C until further processing. Brain regions were delineated according to the rat brain atlas of Paxinos and Watson (63). We investigated the frontal
102
Schmitt et al.
(motor cortex), infralimbic and medial prefrontal cortex (anterior cingulate cortex) (Bregma 3.20 to 2.20 mm), and temporal cortex; hippocampal subregions CA1, CA2, CA3; and dentate gyrus (Bregma –3.60 to –4.30 mm), nucleus accumbens, as well as striatum (caudate and putamen) (Bregma 1.70 to –0.70 mm). 3.3.1. Receptor Autoradiography
Using quantitative in vitro receptor autoradiography, binding to NMDA receptors was performed with specific ligands such as glycine, spermidine, and MK-801 according to the method of Zilles et al. (64, 65). To remove endogenous ligands such as glutamate and glycine, preincubation of the glass-mounted cryostat sections was carried out with 50 mM Tris–HCl buffer (pH 7.5) for a total of 15 min (3×5 min) at 4◦ C. Binding assays were performed as triplicates in the same magnesium- and zinc-free buffer at 22◦ C for 60 min in the presence of 30 μM glycine and 50 μM spermidine for opening of receptor channels and 5 nM [3 H]-MK801 (dizocilpine maleate). In two adjacent sections per region, non-specific binding was determined by adding 10–4 M (+) MK-801 to the incubation solution. Typically, non-specific binding of less than 5% of total binding was found under these conditions. Non-specific binding was subtracted from total binding to obtain specific binding. Tritium-labeled sections were exposed on [3 H]-sensitive R , Amersham, Germany) for 4 weeks. Autorafilm (Hyperfilm diographs were digitized using an image analysis system (Inter Active Systems, Interfocus, Germany) and a video camera (Nikon, Japan). The gray value images of the coexposed [3 H] plastic standards (Amersham, Germany) were used to compute a calibration curve by non-linear, least-squares fitting, which defined the relationship between gray values and concentration of radioactivity. The plastic standards were calibrated to tissue standards prepared from homogenized brain tissue with known protein content in order to express binding site densities in fmol/mg protein. Concentrations of radioactivity were multiplied by (KD +c)/c to obtain Bmax values (KD = equilibration dissociation constants of the ligand binding kinetics, 5 nM for [3 H]-MK-801; c = incubation concentration of labeled ligand).
3.3.2. In Situ Hybridizations
By in situ hybridization and quantitative real-time PCR, we measured mRNA transcripts (Fig. 4.3). To obtain the probes for in situ hybridization, RNA was prepared from postnatal day 2 rat cerebral cortex using a commercial kit (RNeasy, Qiagen, Germany), and cDNA was synthesized with Superscript II reverse transcriptase (Life Technologies, Germany) and oligo-dT primers (PerkinElmer, Germany). After PCR amplification (Promega, Germany) using specific primer sequences, the PCR product was cloned (Sure Clone Ligation Kit, Pharmacia, Germany) into the
Hypoxic Rat Model for Obstetric Complications in Schizophrenia
103
Fig. 4.3. Base pairs are attached to the DNA strand and this messenger RNA represents the information of a gene. After transport to the cytoplasm it is translated into the protein.
EcoRV site of pBluescript-SK. Sequence identity of the insert was confirmed by sequencing. Asymmetrically digested plasmids served as templates for in vitro transcription with T3 or T7 RNA polymerase (MBI Fermentas, Germany), respectively, to obtain antisense and sense cRNA probes. Efficacy of incorporation of radioactively labeled 35 S-dUTP was measured and hybridizations of duplicate brain sections with antisense and sense probes in concentrations of 107 cpm/ml were performed for 16 h at 55◦ C in the presence of 50% formamide, 20 mM Tris, pH 7.5, 1x Denhardt’s solution, 1.25 mM EDTA pH 8.0, 100 mM DTT, 10% dextran sulfate, 2x SSC, 0.1% SDS, and 1 mg/ml yeast RNA hybridization buffer. After stepwise washings in 2x/1.5x/0.2x SSC dilutions, RNase A digest (10 μg/ml RNase A in 1.5x SSC), and dehydration, three slices (antisense) and one slice (sense) per region and animal were exposed to X-ray films (Biomax MR1) and coexposed with 14 C plastic standards for 4–6 days to obtain radioactivity concentration in Bq/mg brain tissue. Autoradiographs were digitized using the image analysis system mentioned above (Fig. 4.4). 3.3.3. RNA Isolation and Reverse Transcription
One milliliter of Trizol was added to excised tissue and the tissue was homogenized by 30 strokes of trituration through a 27 gauge needle attached to a 1 ml syringe. After addition of 200 μl chloroform and thorough mixing, the solution was transferred to Phase-Lock tubes (Eppendorf) for phase separation. Aqueous supernatants were purified and concentrated by RNeasy mini columns (Qiagen). Quality control was performed by OD measurements in a NanoDrop ND-1000 spectrophotometer (260/280 nm >1.8) and by electrophoretic separation
104
Schmitt et al.
Fig. 4.4. 35 S-stained rat brain sections and their analysis after exposure to sensitive films (in situ hybridization) allows the measurement of expression of a single gene (for example, NR1) in several delineated brain regions.
using an Agilent 2100 Bioanalyzer (R.I.N. values >7.0). Reverse transcription of purified total RNA was carried out using oligodT primers with T7 promoter sequence and Superscript II (Invitrogen). 3.3.4. In Vitro Transcription and Labeling
Reverse-transcribed cDNAs were treated with RNase and with DNA polymerase to obtain double-stranded DNA. Purified DNA was used for amplification reactions using RNAMaxx High Yield Transcription Kit (Stratagene) with T7 polymerase. Typically, this in vitro transcription resulted in 40- to 50-fold amplification of mRNA/cDNA. Subsequently, cRNA was reverse-transcribed using aminoallyl dUTP for reaction with cyanine 3 or cyanine 5 NHS esters. Labeled DNA was purified and dried in a speed-vac.
3.3.5. Preparation of DNA (“Glutamate”) Chips
The chips represent a hypothesis-driven selection of 340 cDNA sequences of genes related to glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission and synaptic vesicle proteins (M. Zink, unpublished data), supplemented by clones purchased from RZPD (Berlin) (Fig. 4.5). The cDNA inserts of bacterial clones were PCR-amplified directly from aliquots of bacterial cultures grown overnight (LB medium, containing antibiotic selection). After electrophoretic quality control, PCR mixtures were dried, resuspended in betaine-containing spotting buffer (66), and spotted on aminosilane slides (Nexterion, PeqLab) using a MicroGrid II Arrayer. Each DNA was double spotted, and each array was spotted three times on the same slide. Control DNAs were salmon sperm DNA, human COT-1 DNA, four Arabidopsis thaliana DNAs (for spike-ins), spotting buffer, water, and a series of DNAs derived from artificial genes (library of random external controls, LOREC) (67). Altogether, a chip contained 4608 spots, including markers at the array corners to facilitate finding the correct
Hypoxic Rat Model for Obstetric Complications in Schizophrenia
105
Fig. 4.5. After conversion into cDNA, hybridization of fluorescence-stained samples on the microarray chip allows the simultaneous measurement of the expression of thousands of genes.
orientation after scanning. After spotting, DNAs were UV crosslinked with a radiation of 250 mJ/cm2 . Then they were baked at 80◦ C for 2 h and stored at RT. 3.3.6. Hybridization and Scanning of Slides
Spotted slides were washed in 1% SDS solution and in distilled water. Then they were immersed in 95◦ C distilled water and immediately in 55◦ C prehybridization solution, where they were left for 45 min. Then they were washed again in distilled water and in isopropanol for 20 s each. Finally, they were dried by a stream of nitrogen or air. For hybridizations, SlideHyb Glass Array Hybridization buffer #1 (Ambion) was used throughout. Hybridizations were done overnight at 56◦ C in a water bath with shaking. All subsequent washing steps at increasing stringency were performed in the dark. Scanning was carried out in a Packard ScanArray 5000 scanner. Each slide was scanned twice at laser strengths of 90 and 100% to obtain a larger spectrum of fluorescence intensities.
3.3.7. Data Processing
Data quality was assessed by a number of procedures, including visual inspection of intensities, MA plots, and correlation analysis of replicates. Within-array normalization was performed according to locally weighted scatterplot smoothing (LOESS) method (68). Only rank-invariant probes were used for model generation during this procedure (69). For between-array normalization, scaling (68) was used. After assessing normal distribution of replicate spots using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, replicates
106
Schmitt et al.
were pooled by their arithmetic means. To identify differentially expressed genes, two-tailed t tests and the RankProduct (RP) (70) method were used. The false discovery rate (FDR) method was employed to avoid accumulation of alpha-error. Corrected p values 5 h in the tissue culture incubator. Wash three times with sterile water and let air-dry in the tissue culture hood. 22. Discard the media from the flask containing the astrocytes and wash in PBS (no Ca2+ no Mg2+ ). If microglia are present, cap the flask tightly and tap it against a hard surface (such as the bench of the tissue culture hood) to release the microglia. Wash again with D-PBS and then incubate at 37◦ C in 3 ml of 0.25% trypsin for approximately 10 min. You may need to bang the flask against your hand to release the astrocytes (do not use a pipette) and transfer to a 15-ml conical tube. Wash the flask with another 5 ml astrocyte plating media, add this to the previous cells, and spin down as before. Gently resuspend cells in 5 ml of astrocyte growth media with a widemouthed, 5-ml pipette and dilute at a ratio of 2 ml of cells into 48 ml of astrocyte growth media. 23. Plate in 12-well poly-D-lysine-coated plates (1 ml/well). 24. Feed by exchanging half of the media once a week only if media become acidic (yellowish). Astrocytes prepared in this manner are used as feeder layers when preparing two-step or dissociated primary neuronal cultures. When used as feeder layers for two-step primary neuronal cultures, astrocytes should be allowed to grow to complete confluency in growth media. In general, we use astrocytes plates that have been confluent for about 3 weeks before seeding primary neurons on top of them (see Note 3). For using as feeder layers for dissociated neuronal cultures, astrocytes should be transferred to MS/N2.1 media when they have reached about 50–60% confluency (see Note 4). These feeder layers should be transferred to MS/N2.1 a week in advance of use. 3.2. Dissection and Preparation of Primary Cortical Neurons
The procedure described below will render two 12-well plates of one-step cultures, and two 12-well plates of dissociated neuronal cultures. If you want to scale up, follow the same procedure up to disaggregating the cells. Then dilute in the corresponding volume
138
Behrens
of neuron plating media such that the final dilution is 1.25 cortices per 10 ml. 3.2.1. One-Step Cultures
These are the cultures of choice when performing electrophysiological recordings or single-neuron transcriptional analysis (42): 1. Dissect cortices of two E15 embryos in DM as described for newborn pups in the preparation of astrocytes, making sure you remove the hippocampus. 2. Collect all four cortices into a 15-ml sterile conical tube and let them settle in the bottom of the tube. Aspirate off the media and add 5 ml of neuron growth media. 3. Using a widemouthed, 5-ml pipette attached to a pipettor, place the mouth of the pipette against the bottom of the conical tube and gently triturate the tissue until no aggregates are observed (be careful to avoid air getting into the pipette and making bubbles). It usually takes around 5–8 times filling the pipette up and down to the 4-ml mark to disaggregate the tissue. Add to 27 ml of neuron plating media in a 50-ml conical tube. 4. Mix and plate 1 ml in each well of two 12-well plates containing poly-L-lysine-coated coverslips that have been treated for 5 days in neuron plating media. Alternatively, for testing of sera, or for biochemical studies, you can plate 0.4 ml in each well of a 24-well Primaria plate. Save the leftover cells for the preparation of dissociated neuronal cultures as described below. 5. Place the plates in a tissue culture incubator (5% CO2 ) and monitor astrocyte growth and neuronal differentiation. The astrocyte monolayer should be confluent in about 12 days. Do not feed the cells during the first 2 weeks after plating. The plating media is sufficiently rich to sustain growth and differentiation of these low-density cultures. When the astrocyte layer is confluent (usually between 10–14 days in vitro), add cytosine arabinoside (Ara-C) to a final concentration of 10 μM. 6. After 48 h of Ara-C addition, exchange 0.5 ml of media with neuron maintenance media. In general, these low-density cultures will need to be fed with neuron maintenance media once a week.
3.2.2. Dissociated Neuronal Cultures
These types of cultures are especially suitable for experiments of intracellular signaling and confocal microscopy analyses: 1. Add neuron growth media to the ∼6 ml leftover cells from the one-step culture preparation to a final volume of 7.5 ml. 2. Add 300 μl of the above neuronal suspension onto each poly-L-lysine-coated coverslip that has been incubated in
Studying Schizophrenia in a Dish
139
plating media for at least 5 days. Do not exchange media. Incubate overnight in tissue culture incubator. 3. The next day, using sterile forceps and a sterile syringe needle with its tip bent, invert the “feeted” coverslips onto 12-well astrocyte plates that have been grown to ∼50–60% confluency and incubated in MS/N2.1 for 1 week. Do not exchange the MS/N2.1 in the astrocyte plates. 4. Add Ara-C (5 μM final concentration) to prevent astrocyte growth on the coverslip. 5. Monitor the development of neurons once a week and feed with MS/N2.1 only if media becomes yellowish (see Note 5 on monitoring the development of neurons). 3.2.3. Immunocytochemistry Methods to Study Parvalbumin Interneurons in Culture
3.2.3.1. Fixation and Blocking
Due to the low number of PV interneurons, both in vivo and in culture, analysis of protein expression patterns as well as signal transduction mechanisms can be studied only by immunocytochemical methods. Thus, the availability of suitable antibodies is a must when studying these interneurons. Following is the protocol we have used over the last 10 years that results in reliable and reproducible fixation of PV interneurons in culture. 1. Wash coverslips twice with PBS to remove growth or treatment media, by carefully aspirating half of the media and adding 0.5 ml of sterile PBS. Aspiration must be performed with low vacuum and the addition of PBS is best performed with a widemouthed pipette (not automatic) against the wall of the well to prevent the formation of a vortex in the center that will lift up the neurons. Add 1 ml of ice-cold 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. This is performed sequentially for each coverslip. Each well should be washed and fixed separately. 2. Incubate at room temperature for 30 min. 3. Wash carefully with PBS three times. Once the neurons are fixed, the fixative as well as the washes can be completely aspirated. However, vortexes can still occur, so the same rules apply as for when fixing. 4. Permeabilization, when needed, is performed by incubation in 1 ml of 0.25% Triton X-100 in PBS for exactly 10 min. 5. Aspirate the Triton solution and wash once, again gently, with PBS. 6. Block in 10% normal serum in PBS (see Note 6 on choice of host) for at least 24 h at 4◦ C. If a preservative is added to the blocking solution, such as 0.02% sodium azide, the coverslips can be kept at 4◦ C for prolonged periods.
140
Behrens
Fig. 6.2. Cultured PV interneurons show different morphologies. Primary cortical neuronal cultures were developed in vitro and immunostained for parvalbumin expression during the fourth week in culture. In general, PV interneurons in culture have three or more aspiny primary dendrites and extensive branching.
3.2.3.2. Immunocytochemistry
PV interneurons in culture can be easily identified by immunodetection of parvalbumin expression (Fig. 6.2). In the following, we will describe the immunocytochemistry procedure we have used for simultaneous detection of parvalbumin and GAD67 in cultured neurons, followed by an example where we have quantified the changes in their levels of expression using exposure to NMDA receptor antagonists as an in vitro model to study the molecular effects of these propsychotic drugs on PV interneurons: 1. Aspirate blocking solution and add 1 ml of primary antibody solution containing 2% normal goat serum, anti-parvalbumin antibody (rabbit polyclonal 1:4000 from Swant), and anti-GAD67 (mouse monoclonal from Chemicon). 2. Incubate in a humidified 37◦ C incubator for 2 h. 3. Aspirate antibody solution and wash three times, 10 min each, with PBS using a rocker. 4. Aspirate the last wash and add 1 ml of secondary antibody solution containing 2% normal goat serum, anti-mouse
Studying Schizophrenia in a Dish
141
Alexa Fluor 488 (1:1000 dilution), and anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 568 (1:1000 dilution) conjugated secondary antibodies. 5. Incubate protected from light for 45–60 min. 6. Aspirate secondary antibody solution and wash as in step 4 avoiding light exposure. 7. Place a small drop of aqueous fluorescence mount media (such as FluoroMount-G) on a slide. Gently lift a coverslip from its well and place it at an angle (cell layer facing the mounting media) on top of the drop. If the coverslips have wax feet, eliminate these before mounting (see Note 7). 8. Aspirate the excess of mounting media on the edges of the coverslip and seal using nail hardener (not nail polish). 9. Let dry protected from light for 24 h before imaging. 3.2.4. Prolonged Treatment of Three-Week-Old Primary Cortical Cultures with NMDA-R Antagonists Reduces the Expression of Parvalbumin and GAD67 in PV Interneurons
Dissociated or one-step neurons are treated with ketamine (0.5 μM) for 24 h by adding 10 μl of a 100× stock solution of ketamine (Ketaset) made in PBS and returning the plates to the tissue culture incubator. After 24 h, the coverslips containing the neurons are lifted from the wells using a bent syringe needle and forceps, washed by immersion in PBS, and transferred (cells up) into the wells of a 12-well plate containing icecold 4% paraformaldehyde. Immunocytochemistry is performed as described above. After immunostaining, the slides containing the coverslips are imaged with a 40× objective on a confocal microscope set to detect emission of the fluorophores conjugated to the secondary antibodies. We normally use sequential imaging to prevent fluorescence bleed-through and take z-stage images across 3.2 μm in depth every 0.2 μm. The PMTs for the two fluorophores are then set manually such that the fluorescence in control PV interneurons is about 75% of the maximum saturation when the images across the 3.2 μm are collapsed. This prevents the burnout of images when the fluorescence level is too high. The low number of PV interneurons in the primary cultures (∼5%) results in having to scan the coverslip to find them. In our experience, the total number of PV interneurons in one coverslip varies between 40 and 70 for dissociated and one-step cultures, respectively. Each PV interneuron is imaged through 3.2 μm starting from the side of the neurons attached to the coverslip, which allows maintaining consistency in the region that will be analyzed for fluorescence intensity. Because GAD67 tends to aggregate in the somatic region, its co-expression with parvalbumin is imaged in the neuronal soma (Fig. 6.3). PV interneurons in culture have different morphologies (summarized in Fig. 6.2) and their processes tend to spread long distances from their somas. However, PV-synaptic contacts are
142
Behrens
Fig. 6.3. GAD67 immunoreactivity aggregates in the somatic region around the nucleus. This image depicts the typical double immunostaining for GAD67 and parvalbumin observed in PV interneurons. GAD67 aggregation in the somatic region around the nucleus can be observed in the left panel. Parvalbumin immunostaining, on the other hand, is observed throughout the cell including the nucleus (right panel ). For this reason it is important to avoid the nuclear region when demarcating the region of interest (dotted lines in images) to be quantified for the two fluorophores.
always perisomatic (Fig. 6.3). Parvalbumin expression, in difference from that of GAD67, can be nuclear. This is important to take into account when performing the image analysis, as described below. 3.2.4.1. Image Analysis of GAD67 and Parvalbumin Expression
We have used MetaMorph to perform image analysis, but ImageJ (NIH) can also be used. Using either software, the region of interest is delineated manually such that it contains the somatic, but not the nuclear, region of the cell (Fig. 6.3). Each neuron is delineated this way and the fluorescence intensity in the region is measured for both fluorophores. The mean ± standard deviation fluorescence per cell is then calculated across all neurons in the coverslips that were treated in the same manner (we usually perform this in duplicate coverslips obtained from the same dissection and repeat this across three different dissections (six coverslips in total) to take into account differences in culture conditions and gestational age). NMDA-R antagonist exposures produce a pronounced decrease in parvalbumin and GAD67 expression in PV interneurons (Fig. 6.4 and also see (28, 42, 56)).
4. Notes 1. In our experience, dissection of the brain is simplified if we use pups at or near the second postnatal day. At this stage, their skulls are still soft, which makes the extraction of the
Studying Schizophrenia in a Dish
143
Fig. 6.4. Prolonged treatment with ketamine leads to the loss of GAD67 and parvalbumin immunoreactivity in PV interneurons. Primary cortical-dissociated neurons were treated with ketamine (0.5 mM) for 24 h after which they were fixed in paraformaldehyde and immunostained for the simultaneous detection of GAD67 and parvalbumin expression. The ketamine treatment strongly reduced GAD67 and parvalbumin expression in PV interneurons (arrows in bottom panels) while preserving GAD67 expression in non-PV interneurons (asterisk in left bottom panel ).
brain much easier than in later days. Also, elimination of the meninges is easier at this postnatal age. 2. The use of endotoxin-free sera together with the elimination of meninges helps reduce microglia. It is important to monitor the appearance of these cells in the astrocyte cultures. Should they appear, tap the flask against the bench to make them float and exchange the media completely. 3. Two-step cultures are used in biochemical studies, in studies of neurodegeneration, or when performing nucleic acid analyses. When preparing these cultures it is important that the astrocyte monolayer has reached confluency several weeks in advance of seeding the embryonic neurons. Astrocyte proliferation becomes contact inhibited when they reach confluency and cells acquire a mature phenotype that is not reactive to media changes. If they have not reached this stage when neurons are seeded on top of them, since
144
Behrens
AraC is added to prevent proliferation of astrocyte precursors coming from the neuronal preparation, they become highly reactive, thus preventing the attachment and development of neurons. 4. In the case of dissociated cultures, the proliferative phase is halted by the prolonged incubation in MS/N2.1 and the astrocytes do not react to the addition of AraC. The reason why these types of neuronal cultures require a lower density of astrocytes is probably related to the maintenance of a critical ratio of neurons to astrocytes for optimal development of neurons. 5. It is important that anybody working with primary neurons becomes familiar with the development of processes during in vitro conditions. Just monitoring this will permit correction of media conditions early on. Under the light microscope, you will observe small processes starting to appear about 4 h after plating the neurons. These processes grow to several times the soma size by the end of the first week in vitro. A critical time in the culture process occurs between 9 and 14 days in vitro when NMDA receptors acquire a mature phenotype. It is during this time period when one gets to know whether the astrocytes are healthy and able to transport enough glutamate. If this is not the case, cultures that looked beautiful during the first week will die by necrosis overnight. Should this happen, there is a >95% chance that the problem is in the astrocyte feeder layer. In the following weeks, the chance of death by anoxia increases in the center of coverslips held on wax feet. This is very difficult to avoid, since lifting up the coverslips tends to damage the astrocyte layer underneath, which also produces the death of the neurons. To bypass this problem, we now put the wax feet toward the center of the coverslip such that while keeping the separation with the astrocyte layer, the wax dots will be in the region that becomes more anoxic. 6. Although it may sound redundant, it is not said enough: make sure you block with a serum that is compatible with the host on which your secondary antibodies are made! The most commonly used secondary antibodies are made in goat, thus we normally block in 10% normal goat serum (NGS). However, depending on the hosts of your primary antibodies, you may need a different set of secondary antibodies that are made in a different host. In general, when the primary antibodies are made in rat, mouse, or rabbit, we use NGS as the blocking sera. When one of the primary antibodies is made in goat, we use normal horse or donkey serum. 7. Removing the wax feet from the coverslips before mounting can become an issue, since in the process, the neurons
Studying Schizophrenia in a Dish
145
can be dislodged. With the aid of a closed #3 forceps, poke gently the center of the wax dot and make a slight movement sideways; the dot will stick to the tip of the forceps. If the dot brakes, using the same forceps, gently pick up the pieces such that they do not “dance around” the coverslip. Yes, you do have to take all the wax out before mounting the coverslip. References 1. Ayhan, Y., Sawa, A., Ross, C.A., and Pletnikov, M.V. (2009). Animal models of gene– environment interactions in schizophrenia. Behav Brain Res 204, 274–281 2. Javitt, D.C. (2007). Glutamate and schizophrenia: phencyclidine, N-methyl-Daspartate receptors, and dopamine–glutamate interactions. Int Rev Neurobiol 78, 69–108 3. Lewis, D.A., Hashimoto, T., and Volk, D.W. (2005). Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 6, 312–324 4. Mouri, A., Noda, Y., Enomoto, T., and Nabeshima, T. (2007). Phencyclidine animal models of schizophrenia: approaches from abnormality of glutamatergic neurotransmission and neurodevelopment. Neurochem Int 51, 173–184 5. Beasley, C.L., and Reynolds, G.P. (1997). Parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons are reduced in the prefrontal cortex of schizophrenics. Schizophr Res 24, 349–355 6. Benes, F.M., and Berretta, S. (2001). GABAergic interneurons: implications for understanding schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 25, 1–27 7. Hashimoto, T., Volk, D.W., Eggan, S.M., Mirnics, K., Pierri, J.N., Sun, Z., Sampson, A.R., and Lewis, D.A. (2003). Gene expression deficits in a subclass of GABA neurons in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with schizophrenia. J Neurosci 23, 6315–6326 8. Ammassari-Teule, M., Sgobio, C., Biamonte, F., Marrone, C., Mercuri, N.B., and Keller, F. (2009). Reelin haploinsufficiency reduces the density of PV+ neurons in circumscribed regions of the striatum and selectively alters striatal-based behaviors. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 204, 511–521 9. Do, K.Q., Cabungcal, J.H., Frank, A., Steullet, P., and Cuenod, M. (2009). Redox dysregulation, neurodevelopment, and schizophrenia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 19, 220–230 10. Fisahn, A., Neddens, J., Yan, L., and Buonanno, A. (2009). Neuregulin-1 modulates hippocampal gamma oscillations: implica-
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
tions for schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex 19, 612–618 Hikida, T., Jaaro-Peled, H., Seshadri, S., Oishi, K., Hookway, C., Kong, S., Wu, D., Xue, R., Andrade, M., Tankou, S., Mori, S., Gallagher, M., Ishizuka, K., Pletnikov, M., Kida, S., and Sawa, A. (2007). Dominant-negative DISC1 transgenic mice display schizophrenia-associated phenotypes detected by measures translatable to humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104, 14501–14506 Cardin, J.A., Carlen, M., Meletis, K., Knoblich, U., Zhang, F., Deisseroth, K., Tsai, L.H., and Moore, C.I. (2009). Driving fast-spiking cells induces gamma rhythm and controls sensory responses. Nature 459, 663–667 Doischer, D., Hosp, J.A., Yanagawa, Y., Obata, K., Jonas, P., Vida, I., and Bartos, M. (2008). Postnatal differentiation of basket cells from slow to fast signaling devices. J Neurosci 28, 12956–12968 Sohal, V.S., Zhang, F., Yizhar, O., and Deisseroth, K. (2009). Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature 459, 698–702 Alexander, G.E., and Goldman, P.S. (1978). Functional development of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: an analysis utilizing reversible cryogenic depression. Brain Res 143, 233–249 Rao, S.G., Williams, G.V., and GoldmanRakic, P.S. (2000). Destruction and creation of spatial tuning by disinhibition: GABA(A) blockade of prefrontal cortical neurons engaged by working memory. J Neurosci 20, 485–494 Wilson, F.A., O’Scalaidhe, S.P., and Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1994). Functional synergism between putative gammaaminobutyrate-containing neurons and pyramidal neurons in prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91, 4009–4013 Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1999). The physiological approach: functional architecture of working memory and disordered cogni-
146
19.
20.
21.
22.
23.
24.
25.
26.
27.
28.
Behrens tion in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 46, 650–661 Kawaguchi, Y. (2001). Distinct firing patterns of neuronal subtypes in cortical synchronized activities. J Neurosci 21, 7261–7272 Markram, H., Toledo-Rodriguez, M., Wang, Y., Gupta, A., Silberberg, G., and Wu, C. (2004). Interneurons of the neocortical inhibitory system. Nat Rev Neurosci 5, 793–807 Gonzalez-Burgos, G., and Lewis, D.A. (2008). GABA neurons and the mechanisms of network oscillations: implications for understanding cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 34, 944–961 Uhlhaas, P.J., Haenschel, C., Nikolic, D., and Singer, W. (2008). The role of oscillations and synchrony in cortical networks and their putative relevance for the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 34, 927–943 Jentsch, J.D., and Roth, R.H. (1999). The neuropsychopharmacology of phencyclidine: from NMDA receptor hypofunction to the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 20, 201–225 Le Pen, G., Grottick, A.J., Higgins, G.A., and Moreau, J.L. (2003). Phencyclidine exacerbates attentional deficits in a neurodevelopmental rat model of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 1799–1809 Stefani, M.R., and Moghaddam, B. (2002). Effects of repeated treatment with amphetamine or phencyclidine on working memory in the rat. Behav Brain Res 134, 267–274 Krystal, J.H., Perry, E.B., Jr., Gueorguieva, R., Belger, A., Madonick, S.H., AbiDargham, A., Cooper, T.B., Macdougall, L., Abi-Saab, W., and D’Souza, D.C. (2005). Comparative and interactive human psychopharmacologic effects of ketamine and amphetamine: implications for glutamatergic and dopaminergic model psychoses and cognitive function. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62, 985–994 Stoet, G., and Snyder, L.H. (2006). Effects of the NMDA antagonist ketamine on taskswitching performance: evidence for specific impairments of executive control. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 1675–1681 Behrens, M.M., Ali, S.S., Dao, D.N., Lucero, J., Shekhtman, G., Quick, K.L., and Dugan, L.L. (2007). Ketamine-induced loss of phenotype of fast-spiking interneurons is mediated by NADPH-oxidase. Science 318, 1645–1647
29. Cochran, S.M., Kennedy, M., McKerchar, C.E., Steward, L.J., Pratt, J.A., and Morris, B.J. (2003). Induction of metabolic hypofunction and neurochemical deficits after chronic intermittent exposure to phencyclidine: differential modulation by antipsychotic drugs. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 265–275 30. Morrow, B.A., Elsworth, J.D., and Roth, R.H. (2007). Repeated phencyclidine in monkeys results in loss of parvalbumincontaining axo-axonic projections in the prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 192, 283–290 31. Zhang, Y., Behrens, M.M., and Lisman, J.E. (2008). Prolonged exposure to NMDAR antagonist suppresses inhibitory synaptic transmission in prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 100, 959–965 32. Jonas, P., Bischofberger, J., Fricker, D., and Miles, R. (2004). Interneuron Diversity series: fast in, fast out – temporal and spatial signal processing in hippocampal interneurons. Trends Neurosci 27, 30–40 33. Bergles, D.E., Doze, V.A., Madison, D.V., and Smith, S.J. (1996). Excitatory actions of norepinephrine on multiple classes of hippocampal CA1 interneurons. J Neurosci 16, 572–585 34. Gorelova, N., Seamans, J.K., and Yang, C.R. (2002). Mechanisms of dopamine activation of fast-spiking interneurons that exert inhibition in rat prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol 88, 3150–3166 35. Jones, S., and Yakel, J.L. (1997). Functional nicotinic ACh receptors on interneurones in the rat hippocampus. J Physiol 504 (Pt 3), 603–610 36. Gulyas, A.I., Megias, M., Emri, Z., and Freund, T.F. (1999). Total number and ratio of excitatory and inhibitory synapses converging onto single interneurons of different types in the CA1 area of the rat hippocampus. J Neurosci 19, 10082–10097 37. Hull, C., and Scanziani, M. (2007). It’s about time for thalamocortical circuits. Nat Neurosci 10, 400–402 38. Daw, M.I., Ashby, M.C., and Isaac, J.T. (2007). Coordinated developmental recruitment of latent fast spiking interneurons in layer IV barrel cortex. Nat Neurosci 10, 453–461 39. de Lecea, L., del Rio, J.A., and Soriano, E. (1995). Developmental expression of parvalbumin mRNA in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus of the rat. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 32, 1–13 40. Chattopadhyaya, B., Di Cristo, G., Wu, C.Z., Knott, G., Kuhlman, S., Fu, Y., Palmiter,
Studying Schizophrenia in a Dish
41.
42.
43.
44.
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
R.D., and Huang, Z.J. (2007). GAD67mediated GABA synthesis and signaling regulate inhibitory synaptic innervation in the visual cortex. Neuron 54, 889–903 Di Cristo, G., Wu, C., Chattopadhyaya, B., Ango, F., Knott, G., Welker, E., Svoboda, K., and Huang, Z.J. (2004). Subcellular domain-restricted GABAergic innervation in primary visual cortex in the absence of sensory and thalamic inputs. Nat Neurosci 7, 1184–1186 Kinney, J.W., Davis, C.N., Tabarean, I., Conti, B., Bartfai, T., and Behrens, M.M. (2006). A specific role for NR2A-containing NMDA receptors in the maintenance of parvalbumin and GAD67 immunoreactivity in cultured interneurons. J Neurosci 26, 1604–1615 Dichter, M.A. (1978). Rat cortical neurons in cell culture: culture methods, cell morphology, electrophysiology, and synapse formation. Brain Res 149, 279–293 Dichter, M.A. (1980). Physiological identification of GABA as the inhibitory transmitter for mammalian cortical neurons in cell culture. Brain Res 190, 111–121 Kato-Negishi, M., Muramoto, K., Kawahara, M., Kuroda, Y., and Ichikawa, M. (2004). Developmental changes of GABAergic synapses formed between primary cultured cortical neurons. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 152, 99–108 Lesuisse, C., and Martin, L.J. (2002). Longterm culture of mouse cortical neurons as a model for neuronal development, aging, and death. J Neurobiol 51, 9–23 Zona, C., Palma, E., Brancati, A., and Avoli, M. (1994). Age-dependent appearance of synaptic currents in rat neocortical neurons in culture. Synapse 18, 1–6 Jovanovic, J.N., Thomas, P., Kittler, J.T., Smart, T.G., and Moss, S.J. (2004). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor modulates fast synaptic inhibition by regulating GABA(A) receptor phosphorylation, activity, and cell-surface stability. J Neurosci 24, 522–530 Muir, J.K., Lobner, D., Monyer, H., and Choi, D.W. (1996). GABAA receptor activation attenuates excitotoxicity but exacerbates oxygen-glucose deprivation-induced neuronal injury in vitro. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 16, 1211–1218 Behrens, M.M., Strasser, U., Koh, J.Y., Gwag, B.J., and Choi, D.W. (1999). Prevention of neuronal apoptosis by phorbol ester-induced activation of protein kinase C: blockade of p38 mitogen-activated protein kinase. Neuroscience 94, 917–927
147
51. Behrens, M.M., Strasser, U., Lobner, D., and Dugan, L.L. (1999). Neurotrophinmediated potentiation of neuronal injury. Microsc Res Tech 45, 276–284 52. Dugan, L.L., Bruno, V.G., Amagasu, S.M., and Giffard, R.G. (1995). Glia modulate the response of murine cortical neurons to excitotoxicity: Glia exacerbate AMPA neurotoxicity. J Neurosci 15, 4545–4555 53. Heidinger, V., Manzerra, P., Wang, X.Q., Strasser, U., Yu, S.P., Choi, D.W., and Behrens, M.M. (2002). Metabotropic glutamate receptor 1-induced upregulation of NMDA receptor current: mediation through the Pyk2/Src-family kinase pathway in cortical neurons. J Neurosci 22, 5452–5461 54. Kim, M.J., Dunah, A.W., Wang, Y.T., and Sheng, M. (2005). Differential roles of NR2A- and NR2B-containing NMDA receptors in Ras-ERK signaling and AMPA receptor trafficking. Neuron 46, 745–760 55. Yu, S.P., Canzoniero, L.M., and Choi, D.W. (2001). Ion homeostasis and apoptosis. Curr Opin Cell Biol 13, 405–411 56. Behrens, M.M., Ali, S.S., and Dugan, L.L. (2008). Interleukin-6 mediates the increase in NADPH-oxidase in the ketamine model of schizophrenia. J Neurosci 28, 13957–13966 57. Cauli, B., Audinat, E., Lambolez, B., Angulo, M.C., Ropert, N., Tsuzuki, K., Hestrin, S., and Rossier, J. (1997). Molecular and physiological diversity of cortical nonpyramidal cells. J Neurosci 17, 3894–3906 58. Xi, D., Keeler, B., Zhang, W., Houle, J.D., and Gao, W.J. (2009). NMDA receptor subunit expression in GABAergic interneurons in the prefrontal cortex: application of laser microdissection technique. J Neurosci Methods 176, 172–181 59. Xia, S., Cai, Z.Y., Thi, L.L., Kim-Han, J.S., Dugan, L.L., Covey, D.F., and Rothman, S.M. (2002). The estrogen receptor is not essential for all estrogen neuroprotection: new evidence from a new analog. Neurobiol Dis 9, 282–293 60. Fejgin, K., Palsson, E., Wass, C., Svensson, L., and Klamer, D. (2008). Nitric oxide signaling in the medial prefrontal cortex is involved in the biochemical and behavioral effects of phencyclidine. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 1874–1883 61. Zuo, D.Y., Wu, Y.L., Yao, W.X., Cao, Y., Wu, C.F., and Tanaka, M. (2007). Effect of MK-801 and ketamine on hydroxyl radical generation in the posterior cingulate and retrosplenial cortex of free-moving mice, as determined by in vivo microdialysis. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 86, 1–7
148
Behrens
62. Papadia, S., Soriano, F.X., Leveille, F., Martel, M.A., Dakin, K.A., Hansen, H.H., Kaindl, A., Sifringer, M., Fowler, J., Stefovska, V., McKenzie, G., Craigon, M., Corriveau, R., Ghazal, P., Horsburgh, K., Yankner, B.A., Wyllie, D.J., Ikonomidou, C., and Hardingham, G.E. (2008). Synaptic NMDA receptor activity boosts intrinsic antioxidant defenses. Nat Neurosci 11, 476–487 63. Muller, N., Riedel, M., Gruber, R., Ackenheil, M., and Schwarz, M.J. (2000). The immune system and schizophrenia. An integrative view. Ann N Y Acad Sci 917, 456–467 64. Ganguli, R., Yang, Z., Shurin, G., Chengappa, K.N., Brar, J.S., Gubbi, A.V., and Rabin, B.S. (1994). Serum interleukin-6 concentration in schizophrenia: elevation associated with duration of illness. Psychiatry Res 51, 1–10 65. Naudin, J., Mege, J.L., Azorin, J.M., and Dassa, D. (1996). Elevated circulating levels of IL-6 in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 20, 269–273 66. Nunes, S.O., Borelli, S.D., Matsuo, T., Watanabe, M.A., and Itano, E.N. (2005). The association of the HLA in patients with
67.
68.
69.
70.
schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and in their biological relatives. Schizophr Res 76, 195–198 Kudoh, A., Takase, H., Takahira, Y., Katagai, H., and Takazawa, T. (2003). Postoperative confusion in schizophrenic patients is affected by interleukin-6. J Clin Anesth 15, 455–462 Lin, A., Kenis, G., Bignotti, S., Tura, G.J., De Jong, R., Bosmans, E., Pioli, R., Altamura, C., Scharpe, S., and Maes, M. (1998). The inflammatory response system in treatment-resistant schizophrenia: increased serum interleukin-6. Schizophr Res 32, 9–15 Zhang, X.Y., Zhou, D.F., Zhang, P.Y., Wu, G.Y., Cao, L.Y., and Shen, Y.C. (2002). Elevated interleukin-2, interleukin6 and interleukin-8 serum levels in neuroleptic-free schizophrenia: association with psychopathology. Schizophr Res 57, 247–258 Goslin, K., Asmussen, H., and Banker, G. (1998). Rat hippocampal neurons in low-density culture. In Goslin, K., and Banker, G. (ed.), Culturing nerve cells, 2nd edition (London: The MIT Press), pp. 339–370
Chapter 7 Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia Pascal Steullet, Jan-Harry Cabungcal, Anita Kulak, Michel Cuenod, Françoise Schenk, and Kim Q. Do Abstract Evidence of altered antioxidant systems and signs of elevated oxidative stress are reported in peripheral tissue and brain of schizophrenic patients, including low levels of glutathione (GSH), a major thiol antioxidant and redox buffer. Functional and genetic data indicate that an impaired regulation of GSH synthesis is a vulnerability factor for the disease. Impaired GSH synthesis from a genetic origin combined with environmental risk factors generating oxidative stress (e.g., malnutrition, exposure to toxins, maternal infection and diabetes, obstetrical complications, and psychological stress) could lead to redox dysregulation. This could subsequently perturb normal brain development and maturation with delayed functional consequences emerging in early adulthood. Depending on the nature and the time of occurrence of the environmental insults, the structural and functional delayed consequences could vary, giving rise to various endophenotypes. The use of animal models of GSH deficit represents a valuable approach to investigate how interactions between genetic and environmental factors lead to the emergence of pathologies found in the disease. Moreover, these models of GSH can be useful to investigate links between schizophrenia and comorbid somatic disorders, as dysregulation of the GSH system and elevated oxidative stress are also found in cardiovascular diseases and diabetes. This chapter reviews pharmacological and genetic rodent models of GSH synthesis dysregulation used to address some of the aforementioned issues. Up to date, these models revealed that GSH deficits lead to morphological, physiological, and behavioral alterations that are quite analogous to pathologies observed in patients. This includes hypofunction of NMDA receptors, alteration of dopamine neurotransmission, anomalies in parvalbumin-immunoreactive fast-spiking interneurons, and reduced myelination. In addition, a GSH deficit affects the brain in a region-specific manner, the anterior cingulate cortex and the ventral hippocampus being the most vulnerable regions investigated. Interestingly, a GSH deficit during a limited period of postnatal development is sufficient to have long-lasting consequences on the integrity of PV–IR interneurons in the anterior cingulate cortex and impairs cognitive functions in adulthood. Finally, these animal models of GSH deficit display behavioral impairments that could be related to schizophrenia. Altogether, current data strongly support a contributing role of a redox dysregulation on the development of pathologies associated with the illness and demonstrate the usefulness of these models to better understand the biological mechanisms leading to schizophrenia. Key words: Glutathione, oxidative stress, schizophrenia, parvalbumin, NMDA receptors, dopamine, γ-oscillations, behavior, development, anterior cingulate cortex, ventral hippocampus. P. O’Donnell (ed.), Animal Models of Schizophrenia and Related Disorders, Neuromethods 59, DOI 10.1007/978-1-61779-157-4_7, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
149
150
Steullet et al.
1. Introduction The present review is focused on animal models with a glutathione (GSH) deficit used to investigate the contribution of redox dysregulation to the pathology of schizophrenia. Redox dysregulation is the consequence of an imbalance between an overproduction of reactive oxygen species (ROS) and reactive nitrogen species (RNS) on one side, and a deficiency of enzymatic and non-enzymatic antioxidants on the other side. This leads to oxidative and nitrosative stress-inducing macromolecular damage such as lipid peroxidation, protein oxidation (protein carbonyls), and DNA damages (1). Redox dysregulation can also alter redoxsensitive processes and signaling (2) such as cell cycle regulation and differentiation (3–5), receptor activation (e.g., NMDA receptor) (6), and signal transduction and transcription (e.g., via Nrf-2, NF-κB). ROS include superoxide O2 .− , hydrogen peroxide H2 O2 , hydroxyl radical . OH, and peroxyl radical ROO, while RNS include nitric oxide NO and the highly toxic peroxynitrite ONOO− . The defense systems against oxidative and nitrosative stress are multiple, consisting of enzymes such as superoxide dismutases and catalase, and many other enzymes involved in the redox cycle of GSH, thioredoxin, and glutaredoxin systems. In addition, non-enzymatic antioxidants such as GSH, ascorbic acid (vitamin C), α-tocopherol (vitamin E), carotenoids, and flavonoids act in concert to neutralize ROS and RNS produced in the different cellular compartments. GSH (γ-glutamylcysteine–glycine, Fig. 7.1a) is present in millimolar range in the cytosol, nuclei, and mitochondria. It is the major thiol antioxidant and redox buffer of the cell. It can be found in reduced and oxidized form (glutathione disulphide, GSSG) and constitutes a major storage of cysteine. GSH is a cofactor of several enzymes, which detoxify hydrogen and lipid peroxides through glutathione peroxidases (GPX, Fig. 7.1b, c) and environmental toxins through glutathione transferases (GST, Fig. 7.1a, c) (7). GSH is also able to scavenge directly hydroxyl radicals and singlet oxygen; it participates in the glutaredoxin system and regenerates the antioxidants, ascorbic acid and vitamin E, back to their active forms (Fig. 7.1b). The capacity of GSH to regenerate these antioxidants is linked with the redox state of the glutathione disulphide–glutathione couple (GSSG/GSH) (for recent reviews, see (1, 2, 7, 8)). In addition, the thiol redox state determined by the glutathione disulphide–glutathione couple plays a central role in the modification of cysteine residues of proteins, providing mechanisms to regulate protein function (2) (Fig. 7.1d). In brain, which utilizes 20% of the oxygen
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
151
consumed by the body leading to high amount of ROS during oxidative phosphorylation, GSH plays a key role in protection against oxidative stress. Moreover, in brain, GSH metabolism depends on intact function of glial cells such as astrocytes, which provide neurons with cysteine, the limiting precursor of GSH (9). In peripheral tissues of schizophrenic patients, impaired antioxidant defense systems and increased lipid peroxidation have been reported (10, 11). Reductions of plasma antioxidants such as bilirubin and uric acid were found in both first episode (12) and chronic patients (13). Abnormal antioxidant enzyme activity of superoxide dismutase (SOD), GPX, and catalase (CAT) was observed in plasma and/or blood cells of patients (14– 17). However, large discrepancies were reported (see (18)). Impaired antioxidant systems could lead to oxidative stress and ROS-mediated injury as supported by increased lipid peroxidation products, decreased levels of membrane polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs), and decreased membrane phospholipids in blood cells (19, 20) and fibroblasts (21). It has been suggested that peripheral membrane anomalies correlate with abnormal central phospholipid metabolism in first episode and chronic schizophrenic patients (22–24). Altered redox systems have also been observed in brains of subjects suffering from schizophrenia. Decreased levels of GSH in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and in medial prefrontal cortex (PFC) have been measured in schizophrenic patients (25). A microarray and proteomic study showed anomalies of mitochondrial function and oxidative stress pathways in post-mortem brains of patients (26). In addition, lower GPX and glutathione reductase activities (27), elevated Mn-SOD protein levels and activity (28, 29), and elevated Cu- and Zn-SOD activity (29) were found in post-mortem brains of patients. However, it was not clear whether these observations indicated an intrinsic impairment of some antioxidant systems or whether these were the consequences of other dysfunctions and/or environmental insults (i.e., toxic compounds, infections) leading to an excess of ROS production. We recently provided genetic and functional evidence for an intrinsically impaired function of the glutamate cysteine ligase (GCL, Fig. 7.1a), the rate-limiting enzyme of GSH synthesis (30). Polymorphisms of GAG trinucleotide repeat (GAG-TNR) of the catalytic subunit of GCL (GCLC) were associated with the illness in two independent cohorts. Compared to skin fibroblasts of “low-risk” GCLC genotypes, fibroblasts of “high-risk” GCLC genotypes, present in 35–40% of patients, had lower GCL protein expression, GCL activity, and GSH levels under oxidative stress. This demonstrated that GAG-TNR variants are associated with dysfunctional regulation of GSH synthesis in a sub-population of patients. Furthermore, “high-risk” genotype patients had
152
Steullet et al.
A
Redox-dependent signaling - regulation of protein function (e.g., NMDAR) - regulation of DNA binding
BSO Glu
(-)
GCLC
γ -GluCys
GCLM
Cys
D
Gly
GCL
GSH
GSS
(γ -GluCysGly)
C
GCLM (-/-)
B Antioxidant & Protection against macromolecular damage: - lipid peroxidation - protein oxidation (protein carbonyls) - DNA oxidation and damage
Detoxification
B
C
H2O2
GSH
LOOH
Ep
NADP +
vit E radical
dehydroascorbate
GSH
GSSG
GST GR
GPX
GS-Ep
H 2O LOH vit E ascorbate
in
GSSG
out
NADPH GS-Ep
D P-S
(3 )
GSH
GSSG
P-SSG
P-SH P-SH
SH
GSSG/GSH
(1)
(4)
P-SNO
P-S S
P1- S
P1- SH P2- SH
S
(2 )
P2
Fig. 7.1. Synthesis and role of glutathione (GSH). (a) GSH synthesis requires two ATP-dependent enzymatic steps. The first and rate-limiting enzyme is GCL that binds glutamate (Glu) and cysteine (Cys) to form γ-glutamylcysteine (γ-GluCys). GCL consists of two subunits: a heavy catalytic subunit (GCLC) and a light modifier subunit (GCLM). The second enzyme is GSS, which adds glycine (Gly) onto γ-GluCys to form GSH. Here is also depicted the action site of the pharmacological (BSO) and genetic tool (knockout of GCLM) used to induce a GSH deficit. Roles of GSH include antioxidant action and protection against macromolecular damage (b), detoxification (c), and redox-dependent regulation of protein function (d) and DNA binding (2). (b) Antioxidant action of GSH. Hydrogen peroxide (H2 O2 ) and lipid hydroxyperoxide (LOOH) are reduced by GSH via GPX, producing GSSG, which in turn can be reduced back to GSH via GR. Regeneration of ascorbic acid and vitamin E from their oxidized forms (dehydroascorbate and vitamin E radical, respectively) is achieved via the same GPX-dependent mechanism. Failure of this system may result in irreversible macromolecular damage such as lipid peroxidation, formation of protein carbonyls, and DNA damage. (c) Detoxifying action of GSH. Toxic electrophile compounds (Ep), including diethyl maleate and cyclohexene-1-one, are detoxified by formation of adducts with GSH (GSEp) via the action of GST, and subsequently expelled from the cells via multidrug-resistance proteins (MRPs depicted as circles). This results in GSH depletion. 4-Hydroxynonenal, a product from lipid peroxidation process, can also be rendered
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
153
lower plasma GSH levels and higher oxidized cysteine levels than “low-risk” patients, pointing to a generalized redox dysregulation (31). This is consistent with the decreased GSH levels reported in schizophrenia (25, 27). The drug-naïve status of the patients in the CSF study (25) suggested that the deficit in GSH was not a consequence of treatment. However, GAG-TNR polymorphism was also associated with bipolar patients, but not with major depression (Gysin et al., unpublished), supporting the concept of a psychosis continuum (32) and that various genetic anomalies are common to several forms of psychosis. The implication of redox dysregulation in both schizophrenia and bipolar disease is consistent with the observation that supplementation of N-acetyl-cysteine, a cysteine prodrug, improves both schizophrenic and bipolar patients (33–35). Altogether, this suggests that a dysfunctional regulation of GSH synthesis and a subsequent deficit of GSH in brain and peripheral tissue can be a vulnerability factor to schizophrenia, but also possibly to bipolar disease. Furthermore, other susceptibility genes for schizophrenia may also induce an oxidative state. For instance, functional polymorphisms of PRODH, encoding proline oxidase, that are positively associated with schizophrenia display increased enzyme activity (36), leading to subsequent decreased proline levels. Proline has antioxidant properties and protects the intracellular GSH pool and the GSSG/GSH redox status (37). Consequently, low levels of proline could cause redox dysregulation, which could be particularly severe when combined with impaired GSH synthesis. It is quite striking that many environmental risk factors for schizophrenia (malnutrition, exposure to toxins, maternal infection and diabetes, obstetrical complications, maternal or early life stress, or later insults like brain trauma and stress during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood) result in increased ROS generation, lipid, protein, and DNA oxidation, and decreased GSH levels and antioxidant defense system (see (38) for review). Some of these environmental insults also lead to increased inflammation, emphasizing the tight link between inflammation and oxidative stress (39, 40). Any of the aforementioned environmental insults could worsen the fragile redox equilibrium and, depending on the phase of brain development when
Fig. 7.1. (continued) into a non-toxic glutathiyl adduct via a GST-dependent reaction. (d) Redox regulation of protein function. The GSSG/GSH redox couple determines in great part the redox state of cysteine residues on proteins. Reversible oxidation and modification of cysteine residues are important mechanisms that control protein function and interaction between proteins. Oxidation of cysteine residues can lead to formation of disulfide bonds (1) within a same protein (P1) or (2) between two proteins (P1 and P2). The GSSG/GSH redox couple also modulates (3) S-glutathionylation and (4) S-nitrosylation that are known to control protein function. NMDA receptor function is for instance modulated by redoxdependent modifications of cysteine residues located on the NR2A subunit (6). GCL, glutamate cysteine ligase; GPX, glutathione peroxidase; GR, glutathione reductase; GSS, glutathione synthetase; GST, glutathione transferase.
154
Steullet et al.
they occur, could perturb normal brain maturation with delayed functional consequences in early adulthood. Thus, a genetic defect that leads to dysregulation of GSH synthesis, combined with environmental insults or other genetic factors generating oxidative stress, could be a critical factor contributing to the emergence of schizophrenia (38). Since the brain is highly vulnerable to oxidative damage because of its high oxygen consumption, its high content of oxidizable polyunsaturated fatty acids, and the presence of redoxactive metals (Cu and Fe), a GSH deficit could be particularly damaging to the neuronal function. Oxidative stress-induced cellular damage is also involved in the pathogenesis of various neurodegenerative diseases such as Parkinson’s disease (PD), Alzheimer’s disease (ALZ), and Huntington’s disease (HD). However, in contrast to schizophrenia where a genetic origin of GSH synthesis could affect brain development, GSH depletion and ROS/RNS increase in neurodegenerative diseases appear later in life and are probably downstream consequences of other primary causes (1). In the following, we review animal models of GSH deficit that have been used to determine the consequences of redox dysregulation and to assess their involvement in the pathology of schizophrenia. The chapter is focused on pharmacological and genetic approaches that induce a GSH deficit in rodents via a downregulation of GCL, the key synthesizing enzyme of GSH. In rodent models, GSH deficit was induced (1) semi-chronically during postnatal development, (2) acutely during adulthood (pharmacological treatment with L-buthionine-SR-sulfoximine, L-BSO), or (3) chronically throughout the life [genetic model GCLM (−/−) mice] (Fig. 7.2). These models were used to address the questions below. First, what are the short- and long-term consequences of a GSH deficit restricted to a period of the development? This can test the contribution of a redox dysregulation in the neurodevelopmental aspect of schizophrenia. Neuroanatomical, neurochemical, neurophysiological, and psychopathological data converge to suggest that interactions between susceptibility genes and environmental insults during pre- and perinatal development cause defects in neuronal integrity and connectivity, setting off a cascade of events that extend into adult life and lead to the emergence of the psychotic symptoms. In this context, a genetic and/or environmentally derived GSH deficit could represent an important susceptibility factor (see (38) for review). Second, what are the effects of an acute and transient GSH deficit during adulthood? GSH deficits have been observed in adult patients (25). This approach is useful to distinguish the consequences of an acute GSH deficit during adulthood from those caused by a GSH deficit during development.
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
155
Semi-chronic during postnatal development (non-mutant or ODS rats)
A
BSO GSH deficit GBR12909
Acute during late postnatal development (non-mutant rats)
B
Acute or semichronic at adulthood (non-mutant rats or mice)
C
Chronic throughout life (GCLM (-/-) mice)
D
GSH deficit
fetal
PN PN weaning day 5 day 16
adolescence
adulthood
Fig. 7.2. Schema of the different types of induced GSH deficits that are presented in the review. Horizontal bars delineate the time of the different BSO treatments and the approximate corresponding periods during which a brain GSH deficit is observed. Dotted horizontal bars indicate the period of GBR12909 treatment causing increased extracellular dopamine levels in brain regions rich in dopamine innervations. Arrows indicate times of morphological, physiological, or behavioral analyses. Dotted vertical line indicates birth.
Finally, what are the effects of a chronic GSH deficit throughout life? This is expected to combine effects of a GSH deficit during development (including during fetal development) and later in life. However, a chronic GSH deficiency may in addition lead to more profound adaptations of the entire system to compensate for the redox dysregulation.
2. Pharmacological Models of GSH Deficit
Two main types of pharmacological agents are available to induce a GSH deficit: electrophile compounds and specific inhibitors of GCL. Electrophile compounds (i.e., diethyl maleate and 2-cyclohexen-1-one) cause very rapid and reversible depletion of GSH through a detoxifying process. These compounds conjugate with GSH directly or via the GSH S-transferases (41). The resulting conjugates are then expelled from the cells through pumps called multidrug-resistance proteins (MRPs), leading to rapid decrease in intracellular GSH (Fig. 7.1c). These GSH-depleting
156
Steullet et al.
agents cross the blood–brain barrier and are able to efficiently decrease brain GSH levels within hours, leading to increased protein carbonyls (42, 43). However, they are quite toxic, not very specific for GSH depletion (at least diethyl maleate), and cannot be used for long-term GSH depletion. In our view, they are not appropriate pharmacological tools to model GSH deficit in the context of schizophrenia but are adequate for toxicological studies. However, two studies describe the effects of these GSH-depleting agents on rodent behavior. Adult rats and mice with acute GSH depletion induced by 2-cyclohexene-1-one (for a few hours) showed impaired short-term spatial memory in a Y maze (44). Rats with an acute GSH deficit induced by diethyl maleate (for a few hours) had impaired acquisition of spatial reference memory in a water maze and displayed normal acquisition in an avoidance test (45). Impaired short-term spatial memory in 2-cyclohexene-1-one-treated animals was also reported for other models of GSH deficit (see following paragraphs). However, impairments in spatial learning and memory in a water maze have not been observed in other models of GSH deficit than diethyl maleate-treated rats, suggesting that some effects of diethyl maleate may be independent of GSH depletion. More specific pharmacological tools are inhibitors of GCL. The most specific inhibitor is DL-buthionine-(SR)-sulfoximine (BSO) (see Fig. 7.1a). It represents a relevant model of GSH deficit, as observed in schizophrenia. Only the L-buthionine(SR)-sulfoximine (L-BSO) is a mechanism-based inhibitor subject to ATP-dependent, enzyme-catalyzed phosphorylation by GCL to form L-buthionine-(SR)-sulfoximine phosphate which tightly binds to the active site of GCL (46–48). L-BSO is only weakly metabolized as 90% is expelled from the body in its original form. L -BSO is considered to be an irreversible inhibitor. But after a washout period, GSH levels in cells/tissue slowly recover via turnover of GCLC and possibly very slow reactivation (48). The main limitation for the use of L-BSO in vivo is its poor ability to cross the blood–brain barrier after weaning age (49, 50). Therefore, only a transitory GSH deficit during postnatal development can be achieved by subcutaneous (s.c.) injections of L-BSO. BSO s.c. injection in pre-weanling animals (Fig. 7.2a) can be used to investigate the effects of a transitory GSH deficit on postnatal development and its long-term effects in adulthood when GSH levels have returned back to normal. Indeed, GSH levels normalize a few days after the last BSO treatment (51, 52). On the other hand, acute GSH deficit in adults, via intracerebroventricular (i.c.v.) administrations of L-BSO (Fig. 7.2c), can help understand the effects of an acute or a semi-chronic GSH deficit in adulthood, independent of any developmental contribution. In rats and mice, a GSH deficit causes upregulation of the synthesis of the antioxidant ascorbic acid (53). Such
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
157
compensation does not exist in humans, as we are not able to synthesize ascorbic acid. In order to use a rodent model that resembles more closely the conditions found in humans, GSH deficit was induced in osteogenic-disorder Shionogi (ODS) rats (54), which are deficient in L-gulonolactone oxidase, the key enzyme in the biosynthesis of ascorbic acid (55). In this rat strain, a GSH deficit does not lead to a compensatory increase in ascorbic acid. Animals with a GSH deficit can be more vulnerable to any insults that cause oxidative stress. For instance, enhanced dopamine release, as observed during the encounter of a stressful situation (56), could be deleterious and exacerbate redox dysregulation during conditions of GSH deficit. Indeed, catecholamines such as dopamine produce ROS (57) that need to be neutralized by antioxidant systems. These catecholamines can also react with cysteine residues of GSH and proteins to form conjugates, leading to decrease of endogenous GSH (58–60). To test the effect of enhanced dopamine release during a transient GSH deficit during development, a specific inhibitor of the dopamine re-uptake transporter 1-(2-(bis-(4-fluorophenyl)methoxy)ethyl)4-(3-phenylpropyl)piperazine dihydrochloride (GBR12909) was used to elevate extracellular dopamine levels locally in brain regions rich in dopamine innervations (61). 2.1. BSO-Induced Transient Postnatal GSH Deficit 2.1.1. Methods 2.1.1.1. Animals
2.1.1.2. BSO Treatment
BSO-induced GSH deficits (Fig. 7.2a, b) were studied in nonmutant rats (OFA, Wistars) and in osteogenic-disorder Shionogi (ODS) rats. A GSH deficit leads to compensatory increase in ascorbic acid synthesis in non-mutant rats, but not in ODS rats. ODS rats were obtained from Clea Japan, Inc. (Tokyo, Japan). Because ODS rats cannot synthesize their own ascorbic acid, they require freshly dissolved ascorbic acid (Sigma-Aldrich Chemie GmbH, Buchs, Switzerland) in their drinking water (1 g/l) to prevent vitamin C deficiency (62). ODS rats treated with BSO were in a state of GSH deficiency that was not compensated by ascorbic acid synthesis, but they were not in a state of combined GSH and vitamin C deficiency. A transient postnatal GSH deficit [from postnatal (PN) days 5–24] by about 50% was achieved in brain of rodents through daily subcutaneous (s.c.) injection of L-buthionine(S,R)-sulfoximine (L-BSO; 3.8 mmol/kg animal corresponding to 0.842 mg/g animal) (51, 63). L-BSO (>99% purity) in powdered form can be purchased from Acros Organics (Geel, Belgium) and stored at 4◦ C. BSO was dissolved in a phosphate buffer solution (PBS) consisting of 25 mM NaH2 PO4 , 65 mM Na2 HPO4, and 0.9% NaCl at pH 7.4. Other investigators used 0.9% physiological saline (NaCl alone) to dissolve BSO (64). Solubility of BSO was facilitated by sonication and/or heating the solution to about 45◦ C for 10–15 min. According to the
158
Steullet et al.
producer, BSO solutions can be stored at –20◦ C for up to 3 months. Typically for a rat, 100 μl of a freshly made BSO solution (2–4 mM) was injected daily in the morning. Prior to injection, each animal was weighed in order to adjust the BSO concentration to inject (0.842 mg/g animal). This protocol can be adapted to mice. 2.1.1.3. Enhanced Extracellular Dopamine Levels by GBR12909 Treatment
The specific inhibitor of the dopamine re-uptake transporter, 1-(2-(bis-(4-fluorophenyl)methoxy)ethyl)-4-(3-phenylpropyl)piperazine dihydrochloride (GBR12909), was used to elevate extracellular dopamine levels locally in brain regions rich in dopamine innervations. GBR12909 dihydrochloride (purity >98%) was initially purchased from Tocris Cookson Ltd (Bristol, UK) and stored dessicated at RT. Recently, however, GBR12909 is produced by Biotrend AG (Zurich, Switzerland), on behalf of Tocris. GBR12909 (GBR) was freshly dissolved in PBS (pH 7.4) and subsequently injected s.c. (5 mg/kg animal, in 100 μl) to each rat every other day alone or in combination with BSO (51, 65). Prior to injection, each animal was weighed in order to adjust the GBR12909 concentration to inject (5 mg/kg animal). This protocol can be adapted to mice.
2.1.2. Results
While Slivka et al. (50) reported ∼80% GSH depletion in brain of preweaned mice after four daily injections (s.c.) of BSO, a single daily injection (s.c.) of similar BSO doses in rats from PN days 5–16 resulted in only 40–50% diminution of brain GSH (63). Comparable brain GSH deficit was also observed when BSO was daily administrated to 15-day-old rats for only 1–2 days (66); GSH levels returned back to normal within 2–3 days after the end of the treatment (51). BSO-induced postnatal GSH deficit did not affect the activity of the redox-related enzymes CAT, SOD, GPX, and GR (63). BSO treatment during postnatal development induced similar GSH deficit in ODS and non-mutant rats (63). However, lipid peroxidation was more prominent in ODS rats, particularly in the diencephalon and pons/medulla region (63). Generally, the effects of BSO treatment were stronger in ODS than in any of the non-mutant rats. After 6 days of BSO treatment, ODS rats showed lower body weight than their untreated counterparts (65), but this body weight deficit slowly disappeared after the end of the treatment. Finally, BSO induced more severe cataracts in ODS than in non-mutant rats (65). GSH is indeed an essential antioxidant vital for the maintenance of the transparency of the lenses (67), which are highly vulnerable to oxidative stress. Cataractogenesis due to BSO treatment during postnatal development was largely prevented by melatonin, which has antioxidant properties (68). Altogether, this indicates that the inability to
2.1.2.1. Effects on Brain GSH Levels, Oxidative Stress, and Physical Parameters
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
159
synthesize ascorbic acid (like in humans) is an additional vulnerability factor to a GSH deficiency. In some experiments, rats with a transient GSH deficit during postnatal development were further challenged by a co-treatment with the dopamine uptake inhibitor, GBR12909 (GBR). GBR causes excess extracellular dopamine, which could further disturb the delicate redox balance. Overall, GBR+BSO-treated rats were not notably different from BSO-treated rats in terms of GSH deficit, lipid peroxidation, body weight, and cataract occurrence (51, 65). However, the additional GBR treatment exacerbated some adverse effects of GSH deficit on the development of cortical circuitry and cognitive functions (see below). 2.1.2.2. Effects During the Development of Cortical Circuits
The neuronal circuitry of the PFC is altered in schizophrenic patients (for review, see (69)). This includes reduced spines on pyramidal neurons and reduced expression of parvalbumin (PV), GAD67, and GAT1 in fast-spiking interneurons. ODS rats were used to examine whether a transient BSO-induced GSH deficit during postnatal development (with or without GBR-induced elevated extracellular dopamine) could alter cortical circuits in the PFC. In these experiments, rats were sacrificed after the end of the postnatal treatment (at PN day 16 or 24) and brains were processed for either immunohistochemistry (for interneuron investigations) or Golgi preparations (for dendritic spine quantification). BSO treatment alone affected parvalbumin-immunoreactive (PV-IR) interneurons in the anterior cingulate cortex as demonstrated by a reduced number of PV-IR processes radiating from the somata (70). This effect was more prominent in rats treated with BSO+GBR. Moreover, a GSH deficit combined with elevated extracellular dopamine during postnatal development led to a drastic reduction in the number of small PV-IR profiles (corresponding to PV-IR synaptic boutons and dendritic and/or axonal arborization) in superficial (II–III) and deeper (V–VI) layers of the anterior cingulate cortex, but not in the somatosensory cortex (70). However, there was no loss of PV-IR interneurons as the density of large PV-IR (cell bodies) was not altered. The effect of a GSH deficit on PV-IR interneurons was quite specific seeing that expression of other calcium-binding proteins (i.e., calbindin and calretinin) was not significantly altered by BSO or BSO+GBR treatments. In addition, BSO+GBR treatment during postnatal development (PN days 5–24) also altered the morphology of the dendritic spines on apical and basal dendrites of pyramidal neurons of the layer III of the anterior cingulate cortex (Gheorghita et al., unpublished). Altogether, the use of BSO treatment in rats during their postnatal development, particularly when combined with elevated extracellular dopamine, affects cortical circuitry in the prefrontal cortex (i.e., PV fast-spiking interneurons and dendritic arborization of pyramidal neurons). These observations are
160
Steullet et al.
analogous to anomalies found in post-mortem brains of schizophrenic patients (71–73). 2.1.2.3. Long-Term Effects on Cortical Circuits
Another important question is whether anomalies in PV-IR interneurons caused by a transitory GSH deficit during postnatal development persist throughout adulthood, as GSH levels normalize after the end of the treatment. This has not yet been fully examined. Preliminary observations indicate that BSO or BSO+GBR treatments during postnatal development lead to reduced number of PV-IR interneurons in the anterior cingulate cortex of adult ODS rats (PN day 90) (Cabungcal et al., unpublished). This effect was less severe after BSO treatment alone. Thus, the alteration of PV-IR interneurons by a transitory GSH deficit during development rendered these interneurons more vulnerable during adulthood. This suggests that a transient GSH deficit during postnatal development is sufficient to affect permanently PV-IR interneurons in the anterior cingulate cortex, including a possible cell loss. This long-term effect on cortical circuitry and possibly other regions could be related to the observed long-term cognitive impairments described below.
2.1.2.4. Long-Term Effects on Behavior
A general investigation of the neurological status of the rats up to PN day 50 indicated no obvious effect of BSO treatment. Forelimb muscle strength, novelty reaction, escape, and startle reactions were not affected by BSO treatment with or without GBR (51). A transitory GSH deficit during postnatal development impaired neither spontaneous alternation task performance nor exploratory behavior in an open field (51). The main effect in juvenile ODS rats but not in non-mutant Wistar rats (PN days 25–31) was a significant impaired performance in the spatial water maze task that was observed soon after BSO treatment cessation (74). However, this impairment was no more present in older ODS rats (75). Nevertheless, significant cognitive deficits appeared during the third month of postnatal life. Adult ODS but not non-mutant rats with a transient GSH deficit during postnatal development showed impaired object recognition. This deficit was detected when interval delay was 30 min but not 15 min, suggesting a specific retention impairment. Interestingly, compared to control subjects, the object recognition capacity of schizophrenic patients is also impaired (76–79). The object recognition impairment after BSO treatment emerged earlier in male rats (PN day 65) than in females (PN day 94) and was more severe in males. At PN day 94, males were impaired after BSO treatment (± GBR), while females were impaired only after BSO+GBR treatment (65). This could be due to the protective and antioxidant effects of estrogens (see (80)). Interestingly in humans, first psychotic symptoms occur earlier in men than in women.
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
161
BSO-treated rats trained at a later adult age (above 5 months) in classical spatial tasks expressed a severe impairment in a homing board task, but not in a reference and a working memory paradigm in water maze (74, 75, 81). On a homing board, both adult ODS and Wistar rats treated with BSO during postnatal development were highly inaccurate in discriminating the escape hole position based on distributed visual and olfactory cues (74). Their accuracy was restored only when a single salient cue marked the trained position. In a radial maze, adult BSO-treated rats did more working memory errors and were markedly disturbed by the addition of eight different olfactory cues, a procedure that normally enhances the performance of control rats (74). This suggests that BSO-treated rats have difficulties integrating multisensory cues. The fact, that performance of adult BSO-treated rats was intact in water maze but impaired in homing board, suggests that either the heavy path integration requirement during successive locomotion bouts on a solid floor or the presence of multiple olfactory cues, or a combination of both factors, enhanced task difficulty in the homing board for the BSO-treated rats. Interestingly, schizophrenic patients show impaired multi-sensory processing (82). The data also revealed that the effects of a GSH deficit were more severe in ODS rats unable to synthesize ascorbic acid or when combined with an additional stress factor (GBR treatment). Altogether, these studies demonstrate that a transitory GSH deficit during postnatal development has long-term behavioral consequences, affecting particularly the processing and integration of multi-sensory cues. 2.2. BSO-Induced Acute GSH Deficit in Adulthood 2.2.1. Methods
GSH deficit in adult rodents was achieved via administration of BSO into the lateral ventricle (Fig. 7.2c). Unilateral injection appears to be sufficient to induce similar GSH deficit contraand ipsilaterally to the injection site (52). In adult rats, a dose of 3.2 mg BSO/animal (dissolved in 30 μl of physiological saline, pH 7.4) was routinely administrated every 24 or 48 h for the longest treatments (12 days) (83, 84). A single BSO administration corresponds to approximately 0.01 mg/g rat. In mice, daily doses of about 0.02 mg/g mouse (dissolved in 3–5 μl of physiological saline, pH 7.4) were used (52, 64). Below is the description of the intracerebroventricular injection protocol used by Jacobsen et al. (52) on mice, which is easily adaptable to rats or other rodents. Mice were anesthetized using a saline solution (10 ml/kg; i.p.) of ketamine (100 mg/kg) and xylazine (10 mg/kg) and placed in a stereotaxic frame. A guide cannula (cat. no. C313GS-5; Plastics One, Roanoke, VA, USA) was implanted into the lateral ventricle (AP −0.3 mm; ML 0.9 mm; DV 2.0 mm) and fixed in place with two anchor screws (CMA, North Chelmsford, MA, USA) and dental cement. The guide cannula was protected with a dummy cannula (cat. no. C313DC;
162
Steullet et al.
Plastics One) until used. Mice recovered for at least 1 week before injection and were treated with antibiotics (1.2 mg sulfamethoxazole/ml and 0.24 mg trimethoprim/ml) and analgesics (acetylsalicylic acid; 1 mg/ml) in the drinking water for the first 4 days after surgery and thereafter with only antibiotics. The internal cannula (cat. no. C313I; Plastics One) was inserted into the guide; L -BSO (0.5 mg/mouse; 5 μl) or saline was injected (1 μl/min, 5 min) using a A-99 Razel syringe pump (Stamford, CT, USA). The internal cannula was left in place for 10 min after injection to allow for dispersion away from the injection site. 2.2.2. Results 2.2.2.1. Effects on Brain GSH Levels, Oxidative Stress, and Physical Parameters
2.2.2.2. Effects on Behavior
Intracerebroventricular injections of BSO in adult rodents caused a 40–80% decrease in brain GSH levels. All brain structures, even the most distant from the site of injection, were equally affected (52). However, Shukitt-Hale et al. (84) found that after 12 days of treatment, GSH deficit varied in brain regions, with striatum being the most affected and cerebellum the least. Because of the limited number of studies available, it is difficult to know to what extent GSH deficit varies with the animal model, the dose of BSO, and the duration of the treatment. BSO appears to induce faster GSH depletion in mice (52, 64) than in rats (83), but this difference might well be due to the difference in the BSO concentrations used. Maximum GSH deficiency was reached after 48 h in rats (83, 84) and 24 h in mice (52). The degree of GSH deficit remained stable for 12 days following BSO administration every other day. At the end of BSO treatment, brain GSH levels slowly recovered and normalized almost completely after 72 h (52). This decrease in GSH was accompanied by increased protein oxidation (64). Long-term BSO treatment (12 days) resulted in loss of body weight in rats but did not cause major pathology and did not affect locomotor coordination and performance (84). However, some rats with a GSH deficit exhibited slight and occasional tremors/convulsions, usually in the first few days of BSO treatment (84). Furthermore, Abe et al. (85) showed that BSOinduced GSH deficit in adult mice exacerbated the convulsive action of pentylenetetrazol. This suggests that a GSH deficit can promote epileptic conditions, possibly by depressing the GABAergic system. BSO-induced GSH deficit in adult rodents did not affect locomotor activity (52), spatial reference learning, and spatial reference memory in a water maze (86). However, when GSH deficit was combined with intraventricular injection of dopamine, impairment of locomotion coordination and spatial learning and memory in water maze was observed (84, 86). Since dopamine alone or injected before BSO injection did not have any effect, the authors suggested that reactive compounds resulting from
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
163
dopamine autooxidation led to further redox dysregulation in rats with GSH deficit, thus causing impairment in locomotion coordination and spatial performance. Adult mice with a BSO-induced GSH deficit had reduced capacity in novel object recognition and in a spontaneous alternation task, suggesting impaired short-term memory (64). Interestingly, adult rats which had a transient GSH deficit during postnatal development also showed impaired novel object recognition but not spontaneous alternation (51, 65), suggesting that recognition of objects is particularly vulnerable to a GSH deficit, independent of its time of occurrence. Chronic GSH deficit in GCLM (−/−) mice also led to impaired object recognition (see below). Finally, a GSH deficit in adult mice led to altered psychostimulant-induced locomotor activity (52). Locomotor activity was only transiently increased by amphetamine in BSOtreated mice, while PCP-induced increase in locomotor activity was strongly enhanced in BSO-treated compared to control mice. Dopamine release induced by amphetamine was enhanced in nucleus accumbens but not in PFC of BSO-treated mice, while PCP-induced dopamine release in nucleus accumbens was not affected by a GSH deficit (52). The behavioral responses of the BSO-treated mice to amphetamine and PCP could not however be simply explained by the effects of GSH deficit on dopamine release induced by these psychostimulants. Responses to psychostimulants depend on several neurotransmission systems (i.e., dopaminergic, serotonergic, noradrenergic, and glutamatergic) and interactions between different brain regions (i.e., striatum, nucleus accumbens, PFC, and VTA). A GSH deficit or the resulting increase in ROS could affect many of the above neurotransmission systems in a complex manner. 2.2.2.3. Effects on Neurotransmission
Many proteins are redox sensitive and their functions modulated by the redox state of the cell. This includes NMDA receptors (6), GABAA receptors (87), IP3 receptors (88), ryanodine receptors (89), calcineurin (90), various calcium and potassium channels (91, 92), and dopamine and glutamate transporters (93, 94). Thus, a GSH deficit could potentially alter many aspects of neurotransmission. NMDA receptors. For instance, NMDA receptor-mediated field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) were weaker in hippocampal slices of BSO-treated rats compared to control rats (66). This hypofunction of NMDA receptors was partly due to an excessive oxidation of their extracellular redoxsensitive sites. In addition, a GSH deficit caused reduced NMDA receptor-dependent, long-term potentiation, decreased pairedpulse facilitation, and increased excitability of pyramidal neurons in CA1 (66).
164
Steullet et al.
Dopamine signaling. A BSO-induced GSH deficit also altered dopamine modulation of NMDA-mediated calcium responses in cultured cortical neurons (95). Dopamine decreased NMDA responses in BSO-treated neurons, while the same dopamine concentration (1 μM) enhanced NMDA responses in neurons with normal GSH. The effect of a GSH deficit on dopamine modulation of calcium responses was due to alteration of dopamine modulation of L-type calcium channels and could be prevented by sulpiride, an antagonist of D2-type receptors. This suggests that antipsychotics with D2 antagonist properties might prevent the alteration of dopamine modulation of calcium responses induced by a GSH deficit. Thus, a GSH deficit can lead to changes in neurotransmission and neuromodulation that are relevant to schizophrenia.
3. Genetic Models of GSH Deficit
3.1. GCLM Knockout (GCLM −/−) Mice
Both the catalytic (GCLC) and the modifier (GCLM) subunits of GCL, the key enzyme of GSH synthesis, have been associated with schizophrenia (30, 96). In particular, some polymorphisms of GCLC that display blunted oxidative stress-induced increase in GCL activity and GSH levels confer a higher risk to schizophrenia. Therefore, genetic animal models targeting GCL to disrupt GSH synthesis (97) are valuable models to investigate the effects of a GSH deficit in the context of schizophrenia. Knockout of the GCLC gene, however, is lethal at an early embryonic stage (5, 98), presumably because GSH synthesis is completely suppressed. By contrast, GCLM (−/−) mice are viable showing a decrease in GSH of 50–80% depending on the organs (99, 100). Association between the GCLM gene and schizophrenia (association with SNPs situated on the 5 and 3 regions) was found in two independent cohorts of patients (Swiss and Danish) and in a family-based study from NIMH cohort (96), but not in the Japanese population (101). In the Swiss and Danish cohorts, no clear causal variant within the coding regions of the GCLM gene was however identified (102). However, reduced expression of GCLM mRNA was observed in cultured fibroblasts of patients compared to control in the Swiss sample (96). Therefore, these findings do not rule out the possibility that DNA change, critical for GCLM function or expression, is located in a region that was not screened (e.g., intronic region). Thus, GCLM (−/−) mice (99) represent a valid genetic animal model to study the effects of a chronic and systemic GSH deficit (Fig. 7.2d). GCLM (−/−) mice show about 60% decrease in GSH across brain regions and throughout life (103). GCLM
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
165
(−/−) mice have no overt phenotype besides a slight reduction in body weight (99) and a reduced female fertility, and cultured cells from these mice are vulnerable to neurotoxicity and ROS (99, 100, 104). However, more detailed investigations revealed that GCLM (−/−) mice display brain morphological, physiological, and behavioral anomalies that could be related to schizophrenia and/or other psychiatric disorders. 3.1.1. Methods
GCLM (−/−) mice were kindly provided by Tim Dalton and Chen Ying from the Department of Environmental Health, Center for Environmental Genetics, University of Cincinnati Medical Center (USA). See (99) for a more detailed description of the generation of these mice. GCLM (−/−) mice were then backcrossed with C57Bl/6 J mice over >10 generations. GCLM (−/−) mice were compared with (+/+) littermates.
3.1.2. Results
GABAergic system. Compared to (+/+) mice, GCLM (−/−) mice showed selective and region-specific anomalies in the GABAergic system. Like in the BSO-treated rats, PV-IR interneurons in GCLM (−/−) mice were particularly affected. The developmental expression of PV was impaired in both the anterior cingulate and the somatosensory cortex at PN day 10, but normalized at PN day 20 (105). Additional stress induced by GBR treatment during postnatal development of GCLM (−/−) mice (from PN days 10–20) led to reduced number of PV-IR interneurons in the anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex until PN day 20 (105). PV-IR interneurons in the anterior cingulate cortex of GCLM (−/−) mice remained vulnerable after weaning, as GBR treatment between PN days 30–40 also caused a decrease in PV-IR interneurons in GCLM (−/−) but not (+/+) mice (Cabungcal, unpublished). Interestingly, elevated oxidative stress, as revealed by 8-oxo-dG (marker of DNA oxidation), was present in the anterior cingulate, but not in the somatosensory cortex of GCLM (−/−) mice at PN day 20 (105) (Cabungcal et al., unpublished). This coincides with the higher vulnerability of PVIR interneurons in the anterior cingulate compared to somatosensory cortex. In the hippocampus of GCLM (−/−) mice, PV-IR interneurons also showed a high vulnerability to oxidative stress. The number of PV-IR interneurons was normal in 20-day-old GCLM (−/−) mice, but was strongly reduced in the ventral but not dorsal hippocampus of 4–5-month-old GCLM (−/−) mice (103). The decrease in PV-IR interneurons was particularly selective for the ventral CA3 and dentate gyrus, and this impairment emerged after weaning age as oxidative stress increased or cumulated selectively in the ventral hippocampus (103). These observations confirm that PV-IR interneurons are particularly sensitive to a GSH deficit but their vulnerability depends
3.1.2.1. Morphological Anomalies
166
Steullet et al.
on brain regions and correlates with the level of oxidative stress. This also supports the notion that PV-IR fast-spiking interneurons are highly vulnerable to oxidative stress (106). Thus, signs of DNA oxidation and more severe impairment of PV-IR interneurons were observed in the anterior cingulate cortex and the ventral hippocampus, but not in the somatosensory cortex and the dorsal hippocampus (103; Cabungcal et al., unpublished). Interestingly, the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior hippocampus (analogous to the ventral hippocampus of rodents) are also two brain regions that are known to be affected in schizophrenia (107, 108). Myelin. Preliminary results suggest less myelination in the anterior cingulate cortex of GCLM (−/−) mice, as revealed by weaker MBP immunolabeling intensity and thinner MBP-IR profiles (105). Further investigations are currently under way to identify anomalies associated with myelin and oligodendrocytes. Indeed, redox state modulates proliferation and differentiation of oligodendrocyte precursors (109). Thus, mice with a chronic deficit in GSH show impaired fast-spiking PV-IR interneurons and altered myelination, both of which occur in schizophrenia (69, 110). 3.1.2.2. Reduction of γ-Oscillations
Fast-spiking PV-IR interneurons control the output of principal neurons and are necessary for the generation of γ neuronal synchrony that facilitates information processing and transfer within and between brain regions during cognitive tasks (111). Such γ-oscillations are reduced in schizophrenic patients during impaired cognitive tasks (112, 113), but also at resting state (114). This suggests that anomalies in synchronized neuronal activity, driven by PV-IR interneurons, are a core feature of the disorder. Therefore, we examined whether the reduced number of PV-IR interneurons in the ventral hippocampus of GCLM (−/−) mice was also associated with reduced γ-oscillations. We found that β/γ-oscillations induced by kainate in CA3 were significantly reduced in ventral but not dorsal hippocampal slices of GCLM (−/−) mice (103). These results indicate that a chronic deficiency in GSH affects PV-IR interneurons and the generation of β/γ-oscillations in the ventral but not dorsal hippocampus. Such anomaly of synchronized neuronal activity in specific brain regions is therefore expected to affect the behavior accordingly.
3.1.2.3. Behavioral Phenotype
A chronic GSH deficit did not affect in an unspecific and broad manner all behaviors investigated to date. Overall, the behavioral differences between GCLM (−/−) and (+/+) mice were quite subtle. Using an array of paradigms that require an intact hippocampus, we found a selective alteration in behaviors that rely on the ventral rather than the dorsal hippocampus. GCLM (−/−) mice showed intact spatial reference learning and memory in a water maze and intact performance
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
167
in a rewarded alteration task (103), indicating normal spatial learning and memory capacity and functionally intact dorsal hippocampus (115). By contrast, tasks that are modulated by the ventral hippocampus (115) were altered in GCLM (−/−) mice. Thus, GCLM (−/−) mice showed a lack of behavioral inhibition in stressful conditions. Compared to wild-type mice, GCLM (−/−) mice entered significantly more often the open arms of an elevated plus maze, performed significantly more transitions between the two compartments of a light/dark box, and spent more time in the anxiogenic light compartment (103). GCLM (−/−) mice also displayed reduced acquisition of delay fear conditioning and expression of contextual and auditory-cued fear, again suggesting a functional anomaly of the ventral but not dorsal hippocampus (103). The ventral hippocampus is indeed implicated in both contextual and auditory-cued delay fear conditioning, while the dorsal hippocampus is involved in trace fear conditioning and contextual delay fear conditioning, but not auditory-cued delay fear conditioning (116–118). Moreover, GCLM (−/−) mice showed novelty-induced hyperactivity and strong exploratory drive toward novel objects. Despite this increased object exploration, however, GCLM (−/−) mice displayed impaired object recognition memory. Interestingly, adult rodents with a BSO-induced GSH deficit during postnatal development or an acute GSH deficit at adulthood also had impaired object recognition abilities (51, 64, 65) but intact spatial reference learning and memory in a water maze ((86); Schenk et al., unpublished). Further characterization of the behavioral phenotype of GCLM (−/−) mice focusing on startle responses and PPI, responses to stress, and specific cognitive tasks requiring functional PFC is under current investigation. 3.2. Other Genetic Models of GSH Deficiency
As genetic and functional evidence of an implication of the GCLC in schizophrenia exists, it would be of interest to study animals with a genetic defect on GCLC. Knockout of GCLC gene is however lethal at early embryonic stage (5, 98). GCLC (+/−) mice, which are viable and display a moderate GSH deficiency (in the liver: 20 and 50% decrease of GSH and GCL activity, respectively (98); but no published data on brain GSH levels), could be another model worth investigating. Similarly, mice knockout for the excitatory amino acid carrier-1 (EAAC1) is also an interesting model of GSH deficiency. Observations of altered expression of EAAC1 (=EAAT3) are reported in post-mortem brains of schizophrenic patients (119, 120). EAAC1, which transports glutamate and cysteine (the limiting precursor for GSH synthesis), is specifically expressed in neurons. EAAC1 (−/−) mice have a neuronal deficiency in GSH, leading to increased susceptibility of neurons to oxidant injury (121). Cerebral cortex
168
Steullet et al.
and hippocampus of these mice show particularly elevated oxidative stress. EAAC1 (−/−) mice have also ventricular enlargement and brain atrophy, which progress with aging (121). The Na+ -independent glutamate–cysteine exchanger (xCT) plays a crucial role in astrocytes and developing neurons to provide them with cysteine, the limiting precursor of GSH. Interestingly, xCT has been implicated in cocaine relapse (122). Sut/sut mice (xCT loss-of-function mutants) could also represent a valuable model. Interestingly, these mice develop brain atrophy by early adulthood, exhibiting ventricular enlargement, thinning of the cortex, and shrinkage of the striatum (123), as observed in schizophrenia. 3.3. Models of GSH Deficit to Investigate Comorbidity with Somatic Disorders
Alterations of the GSH system in schizophrenia have been found in not only brains but also blood and skin fibroblasts (15, 25, 27, 30, 96, 124). Therefore, a genetically compromised GSH system can affect all tissues and could represent a vulnerability factor for not only psychiatric diseases but also several somatic disorders. Genetic polymorphisms of GCL genes have been associated with various pathologies, including cardiovascular diseases (125–127) which are more prevalent in schizophrenia (128, 129). Compared to the general population, patients with schizophrenia also suffer more often from type 2 diabetes mellitus and have higher frequency of impaired glucose tolerance (130). Thus, patients with schizophrenia and relatives have higher levels of baseline plasma levels of insulin compared to control subjects. But 2 h after administration of oral glucose, levels of both glucose and insulin are higher in patients and relatives (130), suggesting a blunted response to insulin. These observations suggest either shared environmental or genetic predisposition in schizophrenia and in the impaired glucose tolerance. Interestingly, a dysregulation of the GSH system could be one candidate as a common vulnerability factor. Indeed, elevated oxidative stress and low GSH levels were reported in patients suffering from diabetes (131, 132). Hyperglycemia and insulin deficiency can lead to decreased GSH levels via reduced expression of GCLC (see (7)). Alteration in the mutual interactions between the GSH system and the glucose metabolism might be central to the observed comorbidity between schizophrenia, and more generally to the metabolic syndrome in schizophrenia (133–136). Further investigations on the role of the GSH system in the comorbidity of schizophrenia with various somatic disorders are needed. In this context, GCLM (−/−) mice and other genetic models of GSH deficit could be used to address these aspects.
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
169
4. Summary Data collected (Table 7.1) using various animal models of GSH deficiency have revealed the following: • A GSH deficit affects the brain in a region-specific manner. The PFC (in particular the anterior cingulate cortex) and the ventral hippocampus are especially vulnerable, displaying elevated oxidative stress and impairment of fast-spiking PV-IR interneurons. • A GSH deficit renders the brain more vulnerable to high levels of catecholamine release, in particular dopamine. The most vulnerable brain regions to a GSH deficit appear to be regions that are densely innervated by catecholaminergic neurons. • A GSH deficit causes complex changes in dopaminergic transmission as evidenced by enhanced D2R-dependent signaling and enhanced amphetamine-induced dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens. • A GSH deficit causes hypofunction of NMDA receptors. • A GSH deficit during development disrupts cortical circuit in the anterior cingulate cortex, leading to permanent morphological anomalies (i.e., reduced number of PV-IR interneurons and altered myelination) and long-term behavioral impairment in object recognition and multi-sensory processing, but not in spatial memory and learning in water maze. • A chronic GSH deficit also causes a loss of PV-IR interneurons in the adult ventral hippocampus as oxidative stress increases selectively in this region after weaning age. Such impairment of PV-IR interneurons in the ventral hippocampus is accompanied by reduced β/γ-oscillations. • A chronic GSH deficit leads to impaired object recognition, inadequate response to emotion and stress-related situations with a lack of behavioral inhibition, and reduced responses to fear. Under acute GSH deficiency, adult animals display altered responses to psychostimulants and impairment in object recognition. Finally, a transitory GSH deficiency during postnatal development is sufficient to cause long-term behavioral effects, affecting mostly object recognition and multi-sensory processing. Overall, all these types of GSH deficit lead to impaired object recognition but tend to leave spatial abilities intact when tested in a water maze. Altogether, a redox dysregulation due to a compromised GSH synthesis leads to morphological, physiological, and
Non-mutant rats
BSO + GBR (PN days 5–24)
BSO and BSO + GBR
BSO + GBR
Morphology (in ACC)
Morphology (in ACC, not SSC)
Abnormal dendritic spines on pyramidal neurons of layer III at PN day 24
Reduced number of small PV-IR profiles at PN day 16 Reduced number of PV-IR interneurons at adult No significant change of calbindin and calretinin-IR at PN day 16
No significant change of PV-IR at PN day 16 Reduced number of PV-IR interneurons at adult
BSO
(65)
Cataract (partially reversible) (more severe in ODS rats)
Gheoghita et al., submitted
(70)
(70); Cabungcal, unpublished
(51, 65)
(51)
References
Normal neurological status at PN day 50
ODS rats
General health
BSO and BSO + GBR
Description of effects
In ODS rats only: loss of body weight at PN day 16 (reversible)
ODS and non-mutant rats
During postnatal development (PN days 5–16)
Types of effects
Method
ODS rats and non-mutant rats
Animal
GSH deficit
Table 7.1 Summary of the main effects of different induced GSH deficits on general health parameters, brain morphology, physiology and neurotransmission, and behavior in rodents. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; SSC, somatosensory cortex; HP, hippocampus
170 Steullet et al.
Acute at PN days 15–17
GSH deficit
Table 7.1 (continued)
Non-mutant rats
ODS and non-mutant rats
BSO
BSO
Physiology, neurotransmission (in HP)
NMDAR hypofunction Impaired LTP in CA1 Increased excitability of pyramidal neurons in CA1 Decreased paired-pulse facilitation
Impairment in homing board and radial maze when multiple sensory cues available
In ODS rats only: impaired object recognition (males and females) at ∼PN day 90
In ODS rats only: impaired object recognition (only in males) at ∼PN day 90
BSO and BSO + GBR
Behavior
Description of effects
In ODS rats only: transient impairment of performance in water maze in young rats (PN days 21–30) but not in adults
BSO
ODS rats and non-mutant rats
Types of effects
BSO
Method
Animal
(66)
(74)
(51, 65)
(74)
(51, 65)
(continued)
References
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia 171
BSO
General health
Loss of body weight Normal neurological status and locomotor coordination Slight and short tremors/convulsions
Non-mutant rats
Semi-chronic in adults (12 days)
Physiology, neurotransmission
Enhanced amphetamine-induced dopamine release in nucleus accumbens (not in PFC) No effect on PCP-induced dopamine release in nucleus accumbens and PFC
BSO
Impaired object recognition and performance in a spontaneous alternation task
WT mice
Behavior
Description of effects
(2 days)
BSO
Types of effects
Enhanced PCP-induced locomotor activity; altered pattern of amphetamine-induced locomotor activity
WT mice
Acute in adults (1 day)
Method
(2 days)
Animal
GSH deficit
Table 7.1 (continued)
(84)
(52)
(64)
References
172 Steullet et al.
Chronic
GSH deficit
Table 7.1 (continued)
GCLM (−/−)
GCLM (−/−)
GCLM (−/−) + GBR (PN days 30–40) GCLM (−/−)
Mice
Mice
Mice
BSO
Non-mutant rats
Mice
Method
Animal
Morphology (in ventral, not dorsal HP)
Morphology (in ACC, not SSC)
Behavior
Types of effects
Reduced number of PV-IR interneurons (defect emerges after weaning) Signs of oxidative stress (8-oxo-Dg) emerging after weaning
(105)
Sign of oxidative stress (8-oxo-Dg) at PN day 20 Reduced PV-IR interneurons and signs of oxidative stress at PN day 40
103
103
(continued)
Cabungcal, unpublished
(105); Cabungcal, unpublished
(105)
(86)
References
Altered myelination at PN day 20
Delayed expression of PV at PN days 10–20 (aggravated by GBR)
Normal spatial learning and memory in water maze (impairment when combined with dopamine brain injection)
Normal locomotor activity (impairment when combined with dopamine brain injection)
Description of effects
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia 173
GSH deficit
Table 7.1 (continued)
Method GCLM (−/−)
GCLM (−/−)
Animal
Mice
Mice
Behavior
Physiology, neurotransmission (in ventral, not dorsal HP)
Types of effects
Impaired object recognition
Novelty-induced hyperactivity
Reduced expression of contextual and auditory-cued fear
Reduced freezing during delay fear conditioning
More time in light chamber of a light/dark box
More time in open arm of elevated plus maze
Normal performance in rewarded alternation task
103
103
Reduced γ-oscillations in adults
Normal spatial learning and memory in water maze
References
Description of effects
174 Steullet et al.
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
175
behavioral changes in rodents that are for many analogous to pathologies observed in schizophrenia and possibly other psychiatric diseases (Table 7.2). Thus, a genetic GSH synthesis impairment represents a risk factor in schizophrenia. Redox dysregulation may constitute a “hub” where genetic and environmental vulnerability factors converge and their timing during neurodevelopment plays a decisive role on schizophrenia phenotypes. The described preclinical models that are based on pathophysiology and recent genetic evidence will allow to reveal underlying mechanisms and to test new developed drugs.
5. Technical Notes 5.1. Glutathione Measurements
There are different methods to measure GSH in tissue/cell extracts, other biological samples (i.e., blood), and even in vivo in individual cells or within a certain volume of tissue in live animals/humans. Description of non-invasive GSH measurements in brain tissue using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy methodologies is however beyond the scope of this review. GSH is found in high concentrations in the cytoplasm and organelles, such as mitochondria and nuclei. Free glutathione is found in either reduced (GSH) or oxidized (GSSG) state (Fig. 7.1); GSH represents in normal conditions >95% of the total amount of free glutathione. Quantification of both GSH and GSSG can be important in some studies as the thiol redox state of a cell depends on the relative concentration of the GSH and GSSG. In addition, GSH can bind to NO to form nitrosoglutathione (GSNO) and to proteins to form protein mixed disulfides (for reviews, see (2, 7); see Fig. 7.1). HPLC methods can be used to separate and quantify these various thiols. In this review however, we will only present colorimetric and fluorometric assays that are often based on enzymatic reactions and have the advantages compared to HPLC techniques to be simpler, require less instrumentation, and be less time consuming. The choice of the method will depend on whether one is interested in quantifying: the total thiols, or total free glutathione (GSH + GSSG), or both GSH and GSSG, or GSH bound to proteins. Thus, a careful reading of the technical spreadsheets of the various commercially available glutathione assay kits is necessary before making a choice. Below is a brief description of some of the most commonly used assays that are commercially available. Most assays require preliminary deproteination of the samples using various acids (sulfosalicylic acid, perchloric acid, or metaphosphoric acid), which prevents oxidation of GSH and its degradation by glutamyl
Physiology
Myelination
Dendritic spines
Morphology PV-IR
Consistent with the NMDA model of schizophrenia which assumes hypofunction of NMDA receptors ? (no data available) Reduced γ-oscillations in cortex (110;111)
Hypofunction of NMDA receptors
Impaired LTP in CA1 (HP)
Reduced kainate-induced γ-oscillations in slices of ventral HP
In PFC, HP: Deficit in myelin-associated mRNA and proteins; however MBP expression deficit not seen in all studies (see (110))
In dorsolateral PFC: Reduced number of dendritic spines in deep layer III (72)
In HP: Reduced number of PV-IR cell bodies (145) Consistent with the emergence of symptoms in adulthood
In ventral HP : Reduced number of PV-IR cell bodies in adult GCLM (−/−) mice Anomalies of PV-IR emerge after weaning age
In ACC : Abnormal dendritic spines on pyramidal neurons of layer III at PN day 24 In ACC : Reduced number of and size of MBP-IR profiles in adult GCLM (−/−) mice
In PFC: Reduced expression of PV in fast-spiking interneurons (69) Consistent with the neurodevelopment hypothesis of schizophrenia
Schizophrenia
In ACC: Reduced number of PV-IR cell bodies in adult GCLM (−/−) mice when challenged with a GBR treatment After a transient GSH deficit during development, reduced number of PV-IR cell bodies in adult rats that are unable to synthesize ascorbic acid
Models of GSH deficit
Table 7.2 Comparisons between the major effects of GSH deficit observed in rodent models and pathologies described in schizophrenic patients. The effects of GSH deficit on NMDAR function, dopamine signaling, parvalbumin-immunoreactive (PV-IR) fast-spiking interneurons, and myelination mimic morphological and functional anomalies that might be central to the disconnectivity in schizophrenia. On the other hand, the lack of behavioral inhibition in GCLM (−/−) mice may be more related to behavioral disinhibition observed in other disorders. Note that all morphological data on schizophrenia are from post-mortem brains
176 Steullet et al.
Behavior (in adults)
Impaired object recognition (78) Impairment in a hippocampal-dependent virtual Morris water task (147) PCP exacerbates symptoms in stabilized chronic patients (148) Amphetamine induces heterogeneous changes in psychotic behavior, which may be state dependent (149) Behavioral inhibition in patients (150)
Impaired object recognition
Intact spatial memory in water maze in adult rats and mice
Enhanced PCP-induced locomotion during acute GSH deficit in adult mice
Altered pattern of amphetamine-induced locomotion during acute GSH deficit in adult mice
Lack of behavioral inhibition in GCLM (−/−) mice
Enhanced amphetamine-induced dopamine release in striatum (146)
Enhanced amphetamine-induced dopamine release in nucleus accumbens
Consistent with the neurodevelopment hypothesis of schizophrenia and the emergence of symptoms in adulthood Impaired multi-sensory processing (82)
D2R-type antagonists are antipsychotics
Altered dopamine modulation of calcium responses (can be normalized by D2R-type antagonists)
A GSH deficit during postnatal development leads to behavioral impairments in adulthood GSH deficit during postnatal development causes impaired performance in homing board and radial maze when multiple sensory cues are available
Schizophrenia
Models of GSH deficit
Table 7.2 (continued)
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia 177
178
Steullet et al.
transpeptidase. It is usually recommended however to perform measurements as soon as possible, but some assay kits mention that samples following appropriate preparation can be stored for 1 month at –80◦ C before measurements. 5.1.1. Colorimetric Glutathione Assay Kits
The most commonly used assay is a colorimetric method originally described by Tietze (137, 138). Assay kits using this method are available from numerous commercial providers (i.e., Luminos, Ann Arbor, MI, USA; Cayman Chemical, Ann Arbor, MI, USA; Oxis International, Inc., Beverly Hills, CA, USA; and Biovision Incorporated, Mountain View, CA, USA). This method measures the total free glutathione, but can be also adapted to measure GSH and GSSG. In this method, GSH reacts with DTNB (5,5 dithio-bi-2-nitrobenzoic acid or Ellman’s reagent) to produce a yellow compound, 2-nitro-5-thiobenzoic acid (TNB) and GSSG. In the presence of GR and non-limiting amount of NADPH, the generated GSSG is reduced back to GSH, which can react again with DTNB to produce more TNB. The rate of TNB production, measured by absorbance at 405 or 412 nm, is directly proportional to the concentration of total free glutathione. To measure GSSG and therefore to deduct GSH, one has to first preincubate an aliquot of the sample with 2-vinylpyridine (2-VP; 2–3 μl/100 μl sample), which will slowly derivatize and scavenge free GSH (reaction time, about 60 min). After subsequent adjunction of DTNB, GR, and NADPH, the rate of TNB production will then be proportional to GSSG present in the sample. In some assay kits, another thiol-scavenging reagent (1-methyl2-vinylpyridinium trifluoromethanesulfonate) is provided to scavenge more rapidly and efficiently GSH than does 2-VP (i.e., kit from Oxis International, Inc.). Another colorimetric method commercially available (Calbiochem Merck, Darmstadt, Germany; Oxis International inc.) allows measurement within the same well of total thiols and GSH. This assay does not depend on enzymatic reactions. A patented reagent (4-chloro-1-methyl-7-trifluuromethylquinolinium methylsulfate) is first added to the sample where it reacts with all thiols to produce thiolethers that can be quantified at an absorbance of 356 nm. The second step consists of adding 30% NaOH to convert the thiolether obtained specifically with GSH (not with other thiols) into a chromophoric thione that has a maximal absorbance at 400 nm.
5.1.2. Fluorometric Glutathione Assay Kits
Several fluorometric assays can also be used. One of them (from Biovision Incorporated) allows detection of GSH, GSSG, and total glutathione individually using three aliquots of the same sample. In this assay, o-phthalaldehyde (OPA) reacts with GSH (not GSSG), generating fluorescence (Ex/Em: 340/420) that is proportional to GSH concentration. Adding a reducing agent,
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
179
which converts GSSG to GSH prior to OPA incubation, will allow determination of total free glutathione (GSH + GSSG). To measure GSSG specifically, a GSH quencher is first added to remove GSH and then a reducing agent is added to destroy excess of quencher and to convert GSSG to GSH. Subsequent adjunction of OPA will allow quantification of GSH formed from GSSG. Another fluorometric assay (from Luminos) uses a nonfluorescent molecule, ThioStarTM , that covalently binds to the GSH to yield a highly fluorescent product (Ex/Em: 390/510). In the same aliquot, total free glutathione (GSH + GSSG) can also be subsequently quantified following addition of a reaction mixture that converts all GSSG into GSH. Finally, it is worth mentioning a last assay (i.e., from SigmaAldrich, St Louis, MO, USA; Chemicon International Millipore, Billerica, MA, USA) that utilizes a thiol probe (monochlorobimane, MCB), which becomes fluorescent (Ex/Em: 380/460) when bound to thiols. The formation of these adducts is very slow. However, in the presence of GST, the specific formation of fluorescent adduct between MCB and GSH is much faster, resulting in a fluorescence intensity which is closely correlated with the amount of GSH in the sample. This last method can also be adapted to determine GSH levels in living cells. Cells are permeable to MCB and the incorporated MCB rapidly forms adducts with free GSH via the constitutive GST of the cells. The fluorescence measured within the cells with epifluorescent or confocal microscopes is therefore correlated with the amount of GS–MCB adduct. However, the rate of formation of this adduct also depends on the enzymatic activity of GST within the cell. In our hands, maximal fluorescence is reached after 3–5 min in astrocytes but can take more than 60 min in neurons. Furthermore the time course of GS–MCB formation can change with the age of cells (139). GS–MCB adduct can be expelled from the cell via multidrug-resistance proteins (MRPs); this efflux is particularly prominent in astrocytes (140). Therefore, this method should be considered as semiquantitative and should be adapted for each type of cells and experiments (duration of MCB incubation and MCB concentration, use of inhibitor of MRPs). Thus, we would recommend to first characterize the time course of the fluorescence increase. The method is also inadequate to measure GSH levels when GST activity change is expected. For more information on this technique, see (139–142). Using a similar approach, Molecular Probes (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA) proposes a new free thiol detection reagent (ThiolTrackerTM Violet) in order to measure intracellular GSH levels in alive or fixed cells.
180
Steullet et al.
5.2. Use of Brain Slices for Physiological Measurements
The use of brain slices is a valuable in vitro approach to study neurotransmission, neuromodulation, and synaptic plasticity. However, when interested in the effect of a GSH deficiency, one should be cautious regarding the effect of dissecting and slicing procedures on tissue GSH levels. Since strong activation of neuronal tissue produces GSH release (143, 144), the preparation of brain slices could lead to subsequent depletion of GSH. In our hands, the following protocol of hippocampal slice preparation using 2- to 3-week-old rats led to only 10% decrease of total GSH after slicing, but GSH levels remained then stable in an interface recording chamber for at least 6 h (66). Brain was quickly removed and placed in an ice-cold modified artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACF for slicing) that was aerated with 5% CO2 /O2 . The ACF for slicing contained the following: 118 mM NaCl, 2 mM KCl, 4 mM MgCl2 , 0.5 mM CaCl2 , 1.2 mM NaH2 PO4 , 25 mM NaHCO3 , and 10 mM glucose. Brain was divided into two hemispheres. One hemisphere was kept at −80◦ C for subsequent determination of GSH content (control GSH values before slice preparation) and the other was mounted onto a vibroslicer (Campden Instruments, Loughborough, UK) to make 350-μm-thick slices in aerated ice-cold ACF for slicing. Slices were transferred into an interface chamber superfused with aerated ACF for slicing (∼2 ml/min) at RT. After 15 min, temperature was slowly increased to 30±2◦ C and ACF for slicing replaced by normal aerated ACF (composition: 118 mM NaCl, 4 mM KCl, 2 mM MgCl2 , 2 mM CaCl2 , 1.2 mM NaH2 PO4 , 25 mM NaHCO3 , and 10 mM glucose). Electrophysiological experiments were undertaken after slices had been in normal ACF for at least 60 min. This protocol was adequate for 2- to 3-week-old rats. However, for some unknown reasons, GSH depletion induced by slicing tended to increase with animal age (Steullet, unpublished). In 4–6-month-old mice, slicing could lead to >30% GSH depletion. However, after the initial GSH depletion following slicing, levels of GSH remained stable over many hours. Up to date, all our attempts to prevent or limit such GSH depletion in slices of adult mice have failed. Improved slicing protocol is still needed to investigate the effects of GSH depletion on most aspects of neurotransmission using slices of adult rodents. Therefore, in the absence of a better protocol, which would spare GSH in brain slices of adult rodents, many effects of GSH might be underestimated or even not observed.
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
181
Acknowledgments We would like to acknowledge all collaborators who contributed significantly to the development and study of some of the models described in this review. This includes in particular Beatrix Benz, Vincent Castagné, Ying Chen, Adeline Cottier, Timothy P. Dalton, Fulvia Gheorghita, Gilbert Grima, Jean-Pierre Hornung, Rudolf Kraftsik, Suzie Lavoie, Delphine Preissmann. We are also grateful to Pierre Magistretti for his constant encouragement and Paul Herrling for his support in the initial phase of the project. Work supported by the Swiss Research Foundation (grants nos. 31-55924.98 and 31-116689 to K.Q. Do; grant no. 3100A0–105765 to F. Schenk) and the foundation “Loterie Romande.” References 1. Valko, M., Leibfritz, D., Moncol, J., Cronin, M. T., Mazur, M., and Telser, J. (2007) Free radicals and antioxidants in normal physiological functions and human disease. Int J Biochem Cell Biol 39, 44–84. 2. Jones, D. P. (2008) Radical-free biology of oxidative stress. Am J Physiol Cell Physiol 295, C849–C868. 3. Kwon, Y. W., Masutani, H., Nakamura, H., Ishii, Y., and Yodoi, J. (2003) Redox regulation of cell growth and cell death. Biol Chem 384, 991–996. 4. Menon, S. G. and Goswami, P. C. (2007) A redox cycle within the cell cycle: ring in the old with the new. Oncogene 26, 1101–1109. 5. Shi, Z. Z., Osei-Frimpong, J., Kala, G., Kala, S. V., Barrios, R. J., Habib, G. M., Lukin, D. J., Danney, C. M., Matzuk, M. M., and Lieberman, M. W. (2000) Glutathione synthesis is essential for mouse development but not for cell growth in culture. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97, 5101–5106. 6. Lipton, S. A., Choi, Y. B., Takahashi, H., Zhang, D., Li, W., Godzik, A., and Bankston, L. A. (2002) Cysteine regulation of protein function – as exemplified by NMDA-receptor modulation. Trends Neurosci 25, 474–480. 7. Lu, S. C. (2009) Regulation of glutathione synthesis. Mol Aspects Med 30, 42–59. 8. Forman, H. J., Zhang, H., and Rinna, A. (2009) Glutathione: overview of its protective roles, measurement, and biosynthesis. Mol Aspects Med 30, 1–12. 9. Dringen, R. (2000) Metabolism and functions of glutathione in brain. Prog Neurobiol 62, 649–671.
10. Herken, H., Uz, E., Ozyurt, H., Sogut, S., Virit, O., and Akyol, O. (2001) Evidence that the activities of erythrocyte free radical scavenging enzymes and the products of lipid peroxidation are increased in different forms of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 6, 66–73. 11. Khan, M. M., Evans, D. R., Gunna, V., Scheffer, R. E., Parikh, V. V., and Mahadik, S. P. (2002) Reduced erythrocyte membrane essential fatty acids and increased lipid peroxides in schizophrenia at the never-medicated first-episode of psychosis and after years of treatment with antipsychotics. Schizophr Res 58, 1–10. 12. Reddy, R., Keshavan, M., and Yao, J. K. (2003) Reduced plasma antioxidants in first-episode patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 62, 205–212. 13. Yao, J. K., Reddy, R., and van Kammen, D. P. (2000) Abnormal age-related changes of plasma antioxidant proteins in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 97, 137–151. 14. Mahadik, S. P. and Mukherjee, S. (1996). Free radical pathology and antioxidant defense in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Res 19, 1–17. 15. Raffa, M., Mechri, A., Othman, L. B., Fendri, C., Gaha, L., and Kerkeni, A. (2009) Decreased glutathione levels and antioxidant enzyme activities in untreated and treated schizophrenic patients. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.06.018. 16. Yao, J. K., Reddy, R. D., and van Kammen, Ds. P. (2001) Oxidative damage and schizophrenia: an overview of the evidence
182
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
23.
24.
25.
Steullet et al. and its therapeutic implications. CNS Drugs 15, 287–310. Zhang, X. Y., Tan, Y. L., Cao, L. Y., Wu, G. Y., Xu, Q., Shen, Y., and Zhou, D. F. (2006) Antioxidant enzymes and lipid peroxidation in different forms of schizophrenia treated with typical and atypical antipsychotics. Schizophr Res 81, 291–300. Do, K. Q., Bovet, P., Cabungcal, J. H., Conus, P., Gysin, R., Lavoie, S., Steullet, P., and Cuenod, M. (2009) Redox dysregulation in schizophrenia: genetic susceptibility and pathophysiological mechanisms. In Handbook of Neurochemistry and Molecular Neurobiology, Vol: Schizophrenia (Javitt, D. C., Kantrowitz, I. T., Lajtha, A., Eds.), Springer, New York, vol. 27, pp. 285–311. Keshavan, M. S., Mallinger, A. G., Pettegrew, J. W., and Dippold, C. (1993) Erythrocyte membrane phospholipids in psychotic patients. Psychiatry Res 49, 89–95. Reddy, R. D., Keshavan, M. S., and Yao, J. K. (2004) Reduced red blood cell membrane essential polyunsaturated fatty acids in first episode schizophrenia at neurolepticnaive baseline. Schizophr Bull 30, 901–911. Mahadik, S. P., Mukherjee, S., Correnti, E. E., Kelkar, H. S., Wakade, C. G., Costa, R. M., and Scheffer, R. (1994) Plasma membrane phospholipid and cholesterol distribution of skin fibroblasts from drug-naive patients at the onset of psychosis. Schizophr Res 13, 239–247. Keshavan, M. S., Stanley, J. A., Montrose, D. M., Minshew, N. J., and Pettegrew, J. W. (2003) Prefrontal membrane phospholipid metabolism of child and adolescent offspring at risk for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder: an in vivo 31P MRS study. Mol Psychiatry 8, 316–323. Pettegrew, J. W., Keshavan, M. S., Panchalingam, K., Strychor, S., Kaplan, D. B., Tretta, M. G., and Allen, M. (1991) Alterations in brain high-energy phosphate and membrane phospholipid metabolism in first-episode, drug-naive schizophrenics. A pilot study of the dorsal prefrontal cortex by in vivo phosphorus 31 nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 48, 563–568. Yao, J., Stanley, J. A., Reddy, R. D., Keshavan, M. S., and Pettegrew, J. W. (2002) Correlations between peripheral polyunsaturated fatty acid content and in vivo membrane phospholipid metabolites. Biol Psychiatry 52, 823–830. Do, K. Q., Trabesinger, A. H., KirstenKruger, M., Lauer, C. J., Dydak, U., Hell, D., Holsboer, F., Boesiger, P., and Cuenod,
26.
27. 28.
29.
30.
31.
32. 33.
34.
M. (2000) Schizophrenia: glutathione deficit in cerebrospinal fluid and prefrontal cortex in vivo. Eur J Neurosci 12, 3721–3728. Prabakaran, S., Swatton, J. E., Ryan, M. M., Huffaker, S. J., Huang, J. T., Griffin, J. L., Wayland, M., Freeman, T., Dudbridge, F., Lilley, K. S., Karp, N. A., Hester, S., Tkachev, D., Mimmack, M. L., Yolken, R. H., Webster, M. J., Torrey, E. F., and Bahn, S. (2004) Mitochondrial dysfunction in schizophrenia: evidence for compromised brain metabolism and oxidative stress. Mol Psychiatry 9, 684–697. Yao, J. K., Leonard, S., and Reddy, R. (2006) Altered glutathione redox state in schizophrenia. Dis Markers 22, 83–93. Loven, D. P., James, J. F., Biggs, L., and Little, K. Y. (1996) Increased manganesesuperoxide dismutase activity in postmortem brain from neuroleptic-treated psychotic patients. Biol Psychiatry 40, 230–232. Michel, T. M., Thome, J., Martin, D., Nara, K., Zwerina, S., Tatschner, T., Weijers, H. G., and Koutsilieri, E. (2004) Cu, Zn- and Mn-superoxide dismutase levels in brains of patients with schizophrenic psychosis. J Neural Transm 111, 1191–1201. Gysin, R., Kraftsik, R., Sandell, J., Bovet, P., Chappuis, C., Conus, P., Deppen, P., Preisig, M., Ruiz, V., Steullet, P., Tosic, M., Werge, T., Cuenod, M., and Do, K. Q. (2007) Impaired glutathione synthesis in schizophrenia: convergent genetic and functional evidence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104, 16621–16626. Gysin, R., Krafsik, R., Boulat, O., Conus, P., Comte-Krieger, E., Polari, A., Steullet, P., Preisig, M.,Teichmann, T., Cuenod, M., and Do, K. Q. (2010, Oct 30) Genetic dysregulation of glutathione synthesis predicts alteration of plasma thiol redox status in schizophrenia. Antioxid Redox Signal [Epub ahead of print]. Crow, T. J. (1986) The continuum of psychosis and its implication for the structure of the gene. Br J Psychiatry 149, 419–429. Berk, M., Copolov, D. L., Dean, O., Lu, K., Jeavons, S., Schapkaitz, I., AndersonHunt, M., and Bush, A. I. (2008) N-acetyl cysteine for depressive symptoms in bipolar disorder – a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry 64, 468–475. Berk, M., Copolov, D., Dean, O., Lu, K., Jeavons, S., Schapkaitz, I., Anderson-Hunt, M., Judd, F., Katz, F., Katz, P., OrdingJespersen, S., Little, J., Conus, P., Cuenod, M., Do, K. Q., and Bush, A. I. (2008) N-acetyl cysteine as a glutathione precursor
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
41.
42.
43.
for schizophrenia – a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry 64, 361–368. Lavoie, S., Murray, M. M., Deppen, P., Knyazeva, M. G., Berk, M., Boulat, O., Bovet, P., Bush, A. I., Conus, P., Copolov, D., Fornari, E., Meuli, R., Solida, A., Vianin, P., Cuenod, M., Buclin, T., and Do, K. Q. (2008) Glutathione precursor, N-acetylcysteine, improves mismatch negativity in schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 2187–2199. Kempf, L., Nicodemus, K. K., Kolachana, B., Vakkalanka, R., Verchinski, B. A., Egan, M. F., Straub, R. E., Mattay, V. A., Callicott, J. H., Weinberger, D. R., and MeyerLindenberg, A. (2008) Functional polymorphisms in PRODH are associated with risk and protection for schizophrenia and frontostriatal structure and function. PLoS Genet 4, e1000252. Krishnan, N., Dickman, M. B., and Becker, D. F. (2008) Proline modulates the intracellular redox environment and protects mammalian cells against oxidative stress. Free Radic Biol Med 44, 671–681. Do, K. Q., Cabungcal, J. H., Frank, A., Steullet, P., and Cuenod, M. (2009) Redox dysregulation, neurodevelopment, and schizophrenia. Curr Opin Neurobiol 19, 220–230. Garcia-Bueno, B., Caso, J. R., and Leza, J. C. (2008) Stress as a neuroinflammatory condition in brain: damaging and protective mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32, 1136– 1151. Paintlia, M. K., Paintlia, A. S., Khan, M., Singh, I., and Singh, A. K. (2008) Modulation of peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor-alpha activity by N-acetyl cysteine attenuates inhibition of oligodendrocyte development in lipopolysaccharide stimulated mixed glial cultures. J Neurochem 105, 956–970. Wedner, H. J., Simchowitz, L., Stenson, W. F., and Fischman, C. M. (1981) Inhibition of human polymorphonuclear leukocyte function by 2-cyclohexene-1-one. A role for glutathione in cell activation. J Clin Invest 68, 535–543. Bizzozero, O. A., Ziegler, J. L., De, J. G., and Bolognani, F. (2006) Acute depletion of reduced glutathione causes extensive carbonylation of rat brain proteins. J Neurosci Res 83, 656–667. Gupta, A., Gupta, A., Datta, M., and Shukla, G. S. (2000) Cerebral antioxidant status and free radical generation following glutathione
44.
45.
46.
47.
48. 49.
50.
51.
52.
53.
54. 55.
183
depletion and subsequent recovery. Mol Cell Biochem 209, 55–61. Dean, O., Bush, A. I., Berk, M., Copolov, D. L., and van den Buuse, M. (2009) Glutathione depletion in the brain disrupts short-term spatial memory in the Y-maze in rats and mice. Behav Brain Res 198, 258– 262. Cruz-Aguado, R., Almaguer-Melian, W., Diaz, C. M., Lorigados, L., and Bergado, J. (2001) Behavioral and biochemical effects of glutathione depletion in the rat brain. Brain Res Bull 55, 327–333. Campbell, E. B., Hayward, M. L., and Griffith, O. W. (1991) Analytical and preparative separation of the diastereomers of L -buthionine (SR)-sulfoximine, a potent inhibitor of glutathione biosynthesis. Anal Biochem 194, 268–277. Griffith, O. W. and Meister, A. (1979) Potent and specific inhibition of glutathione synthesis by buthionine sulfoximine (S-n-butyl homocysteine sulfoximine). J Biol Chem 254, 7558–7560. Griffith, O. W. (1999) Biologic and pharmacologic regulation of mammalian glutathione synthesis. Free Radic Biol Med 27, 922–935. Reyes, E., Ott, S., Robinson, B., and Contreras, R. (1995) The effect of in utero administration of buthionine sulfoximine on rat development. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 50, 491–497. Slivka, A., Spina, M. B., Calvin, H. I., and Cohen, G. (1988) Depletion of brain glutathione in preweanling mice by Lbuthionine sulfoximine. J Neurochem 50, 1391–1393. Castagne, V., Rougemont, M., Cuenod, M., and Do, K. Q. (2004) Low brain glutathione and ascorbic acid associated with dopamine uptake inhibition during rat’s development induce long-term cognitive deficit: relevance to schizophrenia. Neurobiol Dis 15, 93–105. Jacobsen, J. P., Rodriguiz, R. M., Mork, A., and Wetsel, W. C. (2005) Monoaminergic dysregulation in glutathione-deficient mice: possible relevance to schizophrenia? Neuroscience 132, 1055–1072. Martensson, J. and Meister, A. (1992) Glutathione deficiency increases hepatic ascorbic acid synthesis in adult mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89, 11566–11568. Nishikimi, M. and Yagi, K. (1996) Biochemistry and molecular biology of ascorbic acid biosynthesis. Subcell Biochem 25, 17–39. Ellender, G. and Gazelakis, T. (1996) Growth and bone remodelling in a scorbutic rat model. Aust Dent J 41, 97–106.
184
Steullet et al.
56. Finlay, J. M. and Zigmond, M. J. (1997) The effects of stress on central dopaminergic neurons: possible clinical implications. Neurochem Res 22, 1387–1394. 57. Cadet, J. L. and Brannock, C. (1998) Free radicals and the pathobiology of brain dopamine systems. Neurochem Int 32, 117–131. 58. Hirrlinger, J., Schulz, J. B., and Dringen, R. (2002) Effects of dopamine on the glutathione metabolism of cultured astroglial cells: implications for Parkinson’s disease. J Neurochem 82, 458–467. 59. Rabinovic, A. D. and Hastings, T. G. (1998) Role of endogenous glutathione in the oxidation of dopamine. J Neurochem 71, 2071– 2078. 60. Grima, G., Benz, B., Parpura, V., Cuenod, M., and Do, K.Q. (2003) Dopamineinduced oxidative stress in neurons with glutathione deficit: implication for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 62, 213–224. 61. Chen, N. and Reith, M. E. (2000) Structure and function of the dopamine transporter. Eur J Pharmacol 405, 329–339. 62. Chan, S. W. and Reade, P. C. (1996) Determination of the L-ascorbic acid requirements in Wistar osteogenic disorder Shionogi rats for prolonged carcinogenesis experiments. Lab Anim 30, 337–346. 63. Rougemont, M., Do, K. Q., and Castagne, V. (2002) New model of glutathione deficit during development: effect on lipid peroxidation in the rat brain. J Neurosci Res 70, 774–783. 64. Mamiya, T., Kise, M., and Morikawa, K. (2008) Ferulic acid attenuated cognitive deficits and increase in carbonyl proteins induced by buthionine-sulfoximine in mice. Neurosci Lett 430, 115–118. 65. Castagne, V., Cuenod, M., and Do, K. Q. (2004) An animal model with relevance to schizophrenia: sex-dependent cognitive deficits in osteogenic disorder-Shionogi rats induced by glutathione synthesis and dopamine uptake inhibition during development. Neuroscience 123, 821–834. 66. Steullet, P., Neijt, H. C., Cuenod, M., and Do, K. Q. (2006) Synaptic plasticity impairment and hypofunction of NMDA receptors induced by glutathione deficit: relevance to schizophrenia. Neuroscience 137, 807–819. 67. Giblin, F. J. (2000) Glutathione: a vital lens antioxidant. J Ocul Pharmacol Ther 16, 121–135. 68. Li, Z. R., Reiter, R. J., Fujimori, O., Oh, C. S., and Duan, Y. P. (1997) Cataractogenesis and lipid peroxidation in newborn rats treated with buthionine sulfoximine: preven-
69.
70.
71.
72.
73.
74.
75.
76.
77.
78.
79.
tive actions of melatonin. J Pineal Res 22, 117–123. Lewis, D. A. and Gonzalez-Burgos, G. (2008) Neuroplasticity of neocortical circuits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 141–165. Cabungcal, J. H., Nicolas, D., Kraftsik, R., Cuenod, M., Do, K. Q., and Hornung, J. P. (2006) Glutathione deficit during development induces anomalies in the rat anterior cingulate GABAergic neurons: relevance to schizophrenia. Neurobiol Dis 22, 624–637. Garey, L. J., Ong, W. Y., Patel, T. S., Kanani, M., Davis, A., Mortimer, A. M., Barnes, T. R., and Hirsch, S. R. (1998) Reduced dendritic spine density on cerebral cortical pyramidal neurons in schizophrenia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 65, 446–453. Glantz, L. A. and Lewis, D. A. (2000) Decreased dendritic spine density on prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 57, 65–73. Kolluri, N., Sun, Z., Sampson, A. R., and Lewis, D. A. (2005) Lamina-specific reductions in dendritic spine density in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 162, 1200–1202. Cabungcal, J. H., Preissmann, D., Delseth, C., Cuenod, M., Do, K. Q., and Schenk, F. (2007) Transitory glutathione deficit during brain development induces cognitive impairment in juvenile and adult rats: relevance to schizophrenia. Neurobiol Dis 26, 634–645. Preissmann, D. (2009) A la recherche de symptômes de désorientation dépendant d’un déficit d’intégration multisensorielle chez l’animal: un éclairage sur les mécanismes fondamentaux de la schizophrénie. Thesis, University of Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland. Danion, J. M., Rizzo, L., and Bruant, A. (1999) Functional mechanisms underlying impaired recognition memory and conscious awareness in patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56, 639–644. Heckers, S., Curran, T., Goff, D., Rauch, S. L., Fischman, A. J., Alpert, N. M., and Schacter, D. L. (2000) Abnormalities in the thalamus and prefrontal cortex during episodic object recognition in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 48, 651–657. Laws, K. R., Leeson, V. C., and McKenna, P. J. (2006) Domain-specific deficits in schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 11, 537–556. Tek, C., Gold, J., Blaxton, T., Wilk, C., McMahon, R. P., and Buchanan, R. W. (2002) Visual perceptual and working
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia
80.
81.
82.
83.
84.
85.
86.
87.
88.
89.
90.
185
memory impairments in schizophrenia. Arch 91. Campbell, D. L., Stamler, J. S., and Strauss, H. C. (1996) Redox modulation of LGen Psychiatry 59, 146–153. type calcium channels in ferret ventricular Prokai, L. and Simpkins, J. W. (2007) myocytes. Dual mechanism regulation by Structure–nongenomic neuroprotection relanitric oxide and S-nitrosothiols. J Gen Phystionship of estrogens and estrogen-derived iol 108, 277–293. compounds. Pharmacol Ther 114, 1–12. Preissmann, D., Cocchi, C., Cabungcal, J. 92. DiChiara, T. J. and Reinhart, P. H. (1997) H., and Schenk, F. (2008) Animal modRedox modulation of hslo Ca2+ -activated K+ els of schizophrenia: in search of a comchannels. J Neurosci 17, 4942–4955. mon key neurological syndrome in rats and 93. Berman, S. B., Zigmond, M. J., and humans. In Endophenotypes of Psychiatric Hastings, T. G. (1996) Modification of and Neurodegenerative Disorders in Animal dopamine transporter function: effect of Models (Granon, S., Ed.), Research Signreactive oxygen species and dopamine. J Neupost/Transworld Research Network, Kerala, rochem 67, 593–600. India, pp. 265–291. 94. Berman, S. B. and Hastings, T. G. (1997) Ross, L. A., Saint-Amour, D., Leavitt, V. M., Inhibition of glutamate transport in synapMolholm, S., Javitt, D. C., and Foxe, J. J. tosomes by dopamine oxidation and reac(2007) Impaired multisensory processing in tive oxygen species. J Neurochem 69, 1185– schizophrenia: deficits in the visual enhance1195. ment of speech comprehension under noisy 95. Steullet, P., Lavoie, S., Kraftsik, R., Guidi, environmental conditions. Schizophr Res 97, R., Gysin, R., Cuenod, M., and Do, K. Q. 173–183. (2008) A glutathione deficit alters dopamine Pileblad, E. and Magnusson, T. (1989) modulation of L-type calcium channels via Intracerebroventricular administration of D2 and ryanodine receptors in neurons. Free L -buthionine sulfoximine: a method for Radic Biol Med 44, 1042–1054. depleting brain glutathione. J Neurochem 53, 96. Tosic, M., Ott, J., Barral, S., Bovet, P., 1878–1882. Deppen, P., Gheorghita, F., Matthey, M. L., Shukitt-Hale, B., Denisova, N. A., Strain, J. Parnas, J., Preisig, M., Saraga, M., Solida, A., G., and Joseph, J. A. (1997) Psychomotor Timm, S., Wang, A. G., Werge, T., Cuenod, effects of dopamine infusion under decreased M., and Quang, D. K. (2006) Schizophrenia glutathione conditions. Free Radic Biol Med and oxidative stress: glutamate cysteine ligase 23, 412–418. modifier as a susceptibility gene. Am J Hum Abe, K., Nakanishi, K., and Saito, H. (2000) Genet 79, 586–592. The possible role of endogenous glutathione 97. Dalton, T. P., Chen, Y., Schneider, S. N., as an anticonvulsant in mice. Brain Res 854, Nebert, D. W., and Shertzer, H. G. (2004) 235–238. Genetically altered mice to evaluate gluShukitt-Hale, B., Erat, S. A., and Joseph, tathione homeostasis in health and disease. J. A. (1998) Spatial learning and memory Free Radic Biol Med 37, 1511–1526. deficits induced by dopamine administration 98. Dalton, T. P., Dieter, M. Z., Yang, Y., with decreased glutathione. Free Radic Biol Shertzer, H. G., and Nebert, D. W. (2000) Med 24, 1149–1158. Knockout of the mouse glutamate cysteine Amato, A., Connolly, C. N., Moss, S. J., and ligase catalytic subunit (Gclc) gene: embrySmart, T. G. (1999) Modulation of neuronal onic lethal when homozygous, and proand recombinant GABAA receptors by redox posed model for moderate glutathione defireagents. J Physiol 517 (Pt 1), 35–50. ciency when heterozygous. Biochem Biophys Joseph, S. K., Nakao, S. K., and SukumRes Commun 279, 324–329. vanich, S. (2006) Reactivity of free thiol 99. Yang, Y., Dieter, M. Z., Chen, Y., Shertzer, groups in type-I inositol trisphosphate recepH. G., Nebert, D. W., and Dalton, T. tors. Biochem J 393, 575–582. P. (2002) Initial characterization of the Hidalgo, C., Bull, R., Behrens, M. I., and glutamate-cysteine ligase modifier subunit Donoso, P. (2004) Redox regulation of RyRGclm(−/−) knockout mouse. Novel model system for a severely compromised oxidative mediated Ca2+ release in muscle and neustress response. J Biol Chem 277, 49446– rons. Biol Res 37, 539–552. 49452. Sommer, D., Coleman, S., Swanson, S. A., and Stemmer, P. M. (2002) Differential sus- 100. Giordano, G., White, C. C., McConnachie, L. A., Fernandez, C., Kavanagh, T. J., and ceptibilities of serine/threonine phosphatases Costa, L. G. (2006) Neurotoxicity of domoic to oxidative and nitrosative stress. Arch Acid in cerebellar granule neurons in a Biochem Biophys 404, 271–278.
186
101.
102.
103.
104.
105.
106.
107.
108.
Steullet et al. genetic model of glutathione deficiency. Mol Pharmacol 70, 2116–2126. Kishi, T., Ikeda, M., Kitajima, T., Yamanouchi, Y., Kinoshita, Y., Kawashima, K., Inada, T., Harano, M., Komiyama, T., Hori, T., Yamada, M., Iyo, M., Sora, I., Sekine, Y., Ozaki, N., Ujike, H., and Iwata, N. (2008) Glutamate cysteine ligase modifier (GCLM) subunit gene is not associated with methamphetamine-use disorder or schizophrenia in the Japanese population. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1139, 63–69. Butticaz, C., Werge, T., Beckmann, J. S., Cuenod, M., Do, K. Q., and Rivolta, C. (2009) Mutation screening of the glutamate cysteine ligase modifier (GCLM) gene in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatr Genet 19, 201–208. Steullet, P., Cabungcal, J. H., Kulak, A., Kraftsik, R., Chen, Y., Dalton, T. P., Cuenod, M., and Do, Q. K. (2010) Redox dysregulation affects the ventral but not dorsal hippocampus: impairment of parvalbumin interneurons, gamma oscillations, and related behaviors. J Neurosci 30, 2547–2558. Lavoie, S., Chen, Y., Dalton, T. P., Gysin, R., Cuenod, M., Steullet, P., and Do, K. Q. (2009) Curcumin, quercetin and tBHQ modulate glutathione levels in astrocytes and neurons: importance of the glutamate cysteine ligase modifier subunit. J Neurochem 108, 1410–1422. Cabungcal, J. H., Frank, A., Steullet, P., Kraftsik, R., Hornung, J. P., Chen, Y., Dalton, T. P., Cuenod, M., and Do, K. Q. (2008) Redox Dysregulation Impairs Normal Development of Parvalbumin-Positive Interneurons and Neuronal Circuitry of the Prefrontal Cortex in an Animal Model with Relevance to Schizophrenia www.sfn.org/ am2008 – Neuroscience Meeting Planner. Society for Neuroscience, Washington, DC. Online. Behrens, M. M., Ali, S. S., Dao, D. N., Lucero, J., Shekhtman, G., Quick, K. L., and Dugan, L. L. (2007) Ketamine-induced loss of phenotype of fast-spiking interneurons is mediated by NADPH-oxidase. Science 318, 1645–1647. Baiano, M., David, A., Versace, A., Churchill, R., Balestrieri, M., and Brambilla, P. (2007) Anterior cingulate volumes in schizophrenia: a systematic review and a metaanalysis of MRI studies. Schizophr Res 93, 1–12. Goldman, M. B. and Mitchell, C. P. (2004) What is the functional significance of hippocampal pathology in schizophrenia? Schizophr Bull 30, 367–392.
109. Smith, J., Ladi, E., Mayer-Proschel, M., and Noble, M. (2000) Redox state is a central modulator of the balance between selfrenewal and differentiation in a dividing glial precursor cell. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97, 10032–10037. 110. Karoutzou, G., Emrich, H. M., and Dietrich, D. E. (2008) The myelin-pathogenesis puzzle in schizophrenia: a literature review. Mol Psychiatry 13, 245–260. 111. Sohal, V. S., Zhang, F., Yizhar, O., and Deisseroth, K. (2009) Parvalbumin neurons and gamma rhythms enhance cortical circuit performance. Nature 459, 698–702. 112. Cho, R. Y., Konecky, R. O., and Carter, C. S. (2006) Impairments in frontal cortical gamma synchrony and cognitive control in schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103, 19878–19883. 113. Uhlhaas, P. J., Haenschel, C., Nikolic, D., and Singer, W. (2008) The role of oscillations and synchrony in cortical networks and their putative relevance for the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 34, 927–943. 114. Knyazeva, M.G., Jalili, M., Meuli, R., Hasler, M., De Feo, O., and Do, K.Q. (2008) Alpha rhythm and hypofrontality in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 118, 188–199. 115. Bannerman, D. M., Rawlins, J. N., McHugh, S. B., Deacon, R. M., Yee, B. K., Bast, T., Zhang, W. N., Pothuizen, H. H., and Feldon, J. (2004) Regional dissociations within the hippocampus – memory and anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28, 273–283. 116. Bast, T., Zhang, W. N., and Feldon, J. (2001) The ventral hippocampus and fear conditioning in rats. Different anterograde amnesias of fear after tetrodotoxin inactivation and infusion of the GABA(A) agonist muscimol. Exp Brain Res 139, 39–52. 117. Esclassan, F., Coutureau, E., Di, S. G., and Marchand, A. R. (2009) Differential contribution of dorsal and ventral hippocampus to trace and delay fear conditioning. Hippocampus 19, 33–44. 118. Hunsaker, M. R. and Kesner, R. P. (2008) Dissociations across the dorsal-ventral axis of CA3 and CA1 for encoding and retrieval of contextual and auditory-cued fear. Neurobiol Learn Mem 89, 61–69. 119. Bauer, D., Gupta, D., Harotunian, V., Meador-Woodruff, J. H., and McCullumsmith, R. E. (2008) Abnormal expression of glutamate transporter and transporter interacting molecules in prefrontal cortex in elderly patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 104, 108–120.
Glutathione Deficit and Redox Dysregulation in Animal Models of Schizophrenia 120. Nudmamud-Thanoi, S., Piyabhan, P., Harte, M. K., Cahir, M., and Reynolds, G. P. (2007) Deficits of neuronal glutamatergic markers in the caudate nucleus in schizophrenia. J Neural Transm Suppl 72, 281–285. 121. Aoyama, K., Suh, S. W., Hamby, A. M., Liu, J., Chan, W. Y., Chen, Y., and Swanson, R. A. (2006) Neuronal glutathione deficiency and age-dependent neurodegeneration in the EAAC1 deficient mouse. Nat Neurosci 9, 119–126. 122. Baker, D. A., McFarland, K., Lake, R. W., Shen, H., Tang, X. C., Toda, S., and Kalivas, P. W. (2003) Neuroadaptations in cysteineglutamate exchange underlie cocaine relapse. Nat Neurosci 6, 743–749. 123. Shih, A. Y., Erb, H., Sun, X., Toda, S., Kalivas, P. W., and Murphy, T. H. (2006) Cysteine/glutamate exchange modulates glutathione supply for neuroprotection from oxidative stress and cell proliferation. J Neurosci 26, 10514–10523. 124. Ranjekar, P. K., Hinge, A., Hegde, M. V., Ghate, M., Kale, A., Sitasawad, S., Wagh, U. V., Debsikdar, V. B., and Mahadik, S. P. (2003) Decreased antioxidant enzymes and membrane essential polyunsaturated fatty acids in schizophrenic and bipolar mood disorder patients. Psychiatry Res 121, 109–122. 125. Campolo, J., Penco, S., Bianchi, E., Colombo, L., Parolini, M., Caruso, R., Sedda, V., Patrosso, M. C., Cighetti, G., Marocchi, A., and Parodi, O. (2007) Glutamate-cysteine ligase polymorphism, hypertension, and male sex are associated with cardiovascular events. Biochemical and genetic characterization of Italian subpopulation. Am Heart J 154, 1123–1129. 126. Koide, S., Kugiyama, K., Sugiyama, S., Nakamura, S., Fukushima, H., Honda, O., Yoshimura, M., and Ogawa, H. (2003) Association of polymorphism in glutamatecysteine ligase catalytic subunit gene with coronary vasomotor dysfunction and myocardial infarction. J Am Coll Cardiol 41, 539– 545. 127. Nakamura, S., Sugiyama, S., Fujioka, D., Kawabata, K., Ogawa, H., and Kugiyama, K. (2003) Polymorphism in glutamate-cysteine ligase modifier subunit gene is associated with impairment of nitric oxide-mediated coronary vasomotor function. Circulation 108, 1425–1427. 128. Curkendall, S. M., Mo, J., Glasser, D. B., Rose, S. M., and Jones, J. K. (2004) Cardiovascular disease in patients with schizophrenia in Saskatchewan, Canada. J Clin Psychiatry 65, 715–720.
187
129. Enger, C., Weatherby, L., Reynolds, R. F., Glasser, D. B., and Walker, A. M. (2004) Serious cardiovascular events and mortality among patients with schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 192, 19–27. 130. Spelman, L. M., Walsh, P. I., Sharifi, N., Collins, P., and Thakore, J. H. (2007) Impaired glucose tolerance in first-episode drug-naive patients with schizophrenia. Diabet Med 24, 481–485. 131. Arana, C., Cutando, A., Ferrera, M. J., Gomez-Moreno, G., Worf, C. V., Bolanos, M. J., Escames, G., and Acuna-Castroviejo, D. (2006) Parameters of oxidative stress in saliva from diabetic and parenteral drug addict patients. J Oral Pathol Med 35, 554–559. 132. Nwose, E. U., Jelinek, H. F., Richards, R. S., and Kerr, P. G. (2007) Erythrocyte oxidative stress in clinical management of diabetes and its cardiovascular complications. Br J Biomed Sci 64, 35–43. 133. Dickinson, D., Gold, J. M., Dickerson, F. B., Medoff, D., and Dixon, L. B. (2008) Evidence of exacerbated cognitive deficits in schizophrenic patients with comorbid diabetes. Psychosomatics 49, 123–131. 134. Fagiolini, A. and Goracci, A. (2009) The effects of undertreated chronic medical illnesses in patients with severe mental disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 70, Suppl 3, 22–29. 135. Henderson, D. C. (2005) Schizophrenia and comorbid metabolic disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 66 Suppl 6, 11–20. 136. Iacovides, A. and Siamouli, M. (2008) Comorbid mental and somatic disorders: an epidemiological perspective. Curr Opin Psychiatry 21, 417–421. 137. Tietze, F. (1969) Enzymic method for quantitative determination of nanogram amounts of total and oxidized glutathione: applications to mammalian blood and other tissues. Anal Biochem 27, 502–522. 138. Baker, M. A., Cerniglia, G. J., and Zaman, A. (1990) Microtiter plate assay for the measurement of glutathione and glutathione disulfide in large numbers of biological samples. Anal Biochem 190, 360–365. 139. Keelan, J., Allen, N. J., Antcliffe, D., Pal, S., and Duchen, M. R. (2001) Quantitative imaging of glutathione in hippocampal neurons and glia in culture using monochlorobimane. J Neurosci Res 66, 873–884. 140. Waak, J. and Dringen, R. (2006) Formation and rapid export of the monochlorobimaneglutathione conjugate in cultured rat astrocytes. Neurochem Res 31, 1409–1416. 141. Sun, X., Erb, H., and Murphy, T. H. (2005) Coordinate regulation of glutathione
188
142.
143.
144.
145.
146.
Steullet et al. metabolism in astrocytes by Nrf2. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 326, 371–377. Sun, X., Shih, A. Y., Johannssen, H. C., Erb, H., Li, P., and Murphy, T. H. (2006) Twophoton imaging of glutathione levels in intact brain indicates enhanced redox buffering in developing neurons and cells at the cerebrospinal fluid and blood–brain interface. J Biol Chem 281, 17420–17431. Wallin, C., Weber, S. G., and Sandberg, M. (1999) Glutathione efflux induced by NMDA and kainate: implications in neurotoxicity? J Neurochem 73, 1566–1572. Zangerle, L., Cuenod, M., Winterhalter, K. H., and Do, K. Q. (1992) Screening of thiol compounds: depolarization-induced release of glutathione and cysteine from rat brain slices. J Neurochem 59, 181–189. Zhang, Z. J. and Reynolds, G. P. (2002) A selective decrease in the relative density of parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the hippocampus in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 55, 1–10. Laruelle, M., Abi-Dargham, A., van Dyck, C. H., Gil, R., D’Souza, C. D., Erdos, J., McCance, E., Rosenblatt, W., Fingado, C., Zoghbi, S. S., Baldwin, R. M., Seibyl, J. P., Krystal, J. H., Charney, D. S., and Innis, R. B. (1996) Single photon emission computerized tomography imaging of
147.
148.
149.
150.
amphetamine- induced dopamine release in drug-free schizophrenic subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93, 9235–9240. Hanlon, F. M., Weisend, M. P., Hamilton, D. A., Jones, A. P., Thoma, R. J., Huang, M., Martin, K., Yeo, R. A., Miller, G. A., and Canive, J. M. (2006) Impairment on the hippocampal-dependent virtual Morris water task in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 87, 67–80. Cohen, B. D., Rosenbaum, G., Luby, E. D., and Gottlieb, J. S. (1962) Comparison of phencyclidine hydrochloride (Sernyl) with other drugs. Simulation of schizophrenic performance with phencyclidine hydrochloride (Sernyl), lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD25), and amobarbital (Amytal) sodium; II. Symbolic and sequential thinking. Arch Gen Psychiatry 6, 395–401. Laruelle, M. (2000) The role of endogenous sensitization in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia: implications from recent brain imaging studies. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 31, 371–384. RScholten, M. R., van, H. J., Aleman, A., and Kahn, R. S. (2006) Behavioral inhibition system (BIS), behavioral activation system (BAS) and schizophrenia: relationship with psychopathology and physiology. J Psychiatr Res 40, 638–645.
Chapter 8 Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models P. Alexander Arguello and Joseph A. Gogos Abstract Accumulating evidence indicates that the genetic architecture of psychiatric disorders does not strictly conform to the common disease/common allele hypothesis. The contribution of common genetic variants, while likely, may be fundamentally different from those of rare genetic variants. It is possible that common alleles do not increase disease risk per se but are disease modifiers sculpting the psychopathological landscape produced by rare alleles. Unlike common alleles, the statistical association of rare alleles is usually more robust and their functional effects more translatable into etiologically valid animal models. Although rare alleles may not be shared across individuals with the same diagnosis, the comparison of multiple animal models of rare risk alleles can identify common pathogenetic mechanisms. Thus, paradoxically, the cumulative evidence gathered from these animal models is currently poised to offer more insight into common psychiatric disorders than are models of common alleles. Key words: Risk alleles, rare genetic variants, neuregulin, DISC1, 22q11 microdeletions.
1. Introduction Genetic variation influences almost all phenotypes from the simple to the complex, including the risk for and manifestation of mental illnesses. In light of recent successes and failures, psychiatric genetics is currently at a crossroads concerning how to efficiently identify causal genetic factors and how to translate these findings into meaningful insights for disease prevention and treatment (1). Clearly, teasing apart the contribution of genetic variants to neural development and function, and ultimately disease risk, necessitates the use of model systems. Mutant animal models are thus becoming increasingly instrumental in basic psychiatric P. O’Donnell (ed.), Animal Models of Schizophrenia and Related Disorders, Neuromethods 59, DOI 10.1007/978-1-61779-157-4_8, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
189
190
Arguello and Gogos
research (2). In this chapter we summarize the current and future roles of animal models in this process with a particular focus on schizophrenia. Genetic manipulation in animal models is commonly used to investigate either a hypothesized disease state or a disease process, yet we discuss only the latter. A disease state is a more proximate cause of overt symptoms and, in the case of psychiatric disorders, reflects pathophysiological brain function, whereas a disease process is more distal in the causal chain and results from an initial etiological factor setting off a pathogenetic cascade [for a recent review of pathophysiological models, see Desbonnet et al. (3)]. Unlike models of disease states, models based on psychiatric genetics are not limited by incomplete knowledge of pathophysiology, are more closely linked with the initial pathogenic event, and hold more promise for identifying targets for early intervention. Before describing specific animal models, however, we first discuss recent findings from psychiatric genetics. This is critical because mutant animal models are only as reliable as the clinical data upon which they are based and choosing wisely among genetic findings is the first step in creating valid models. We argue that for both theoretical and empirical reasons, the current methods for identifying genetic risk variants determine the types of etiologically valid mutant models possible. Finally, we illustrate these points by comparing currently available mutant animal models and offer suggestions for future research.
2. Genetic Architecture of Psychiatric Disorders
Psychiatric disorders, like other common diseases, are multifactorial in nature with complex genetic etiologies. The genetic architecture underlying disease susceptibility is characterized by both the frequency and the penetrance of risk alleles (Fig. 8.1). The common disease-common allele (CDCA) hypothesis emphasizes the importance of relatively common alleles, each of small effect, acting together to increase disease risk. The common disease-rare allele (CDRA) hypothesis instead emphasizes the impact of individually rare yet highly penetrant alleles. It is likely that both common and rare alleles contribute to the risk of psychiatric disorders, although the relative impact of each remains unknown. In this section, we discuss the emerging evidence supporting both of these hypotheses in relation to schizophrenia and then address how these data can best guide the creation of mutant animal models.
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models
191
Fig. 8.1. Genetic architecture of common psychiatric disorders. Based on current findings, genetic variants that are relatively common in the population (>5%) have minimal impact on disease risk with most of these variants having low odds ratios (OR). In contrast, the rare genetic variants (5% minor allele frequency) in the population. These studies can focus on candidate genes based on a priori evidence or identify candidates in an unbiased manner using genome-wide association (GWA) studies (4). So far, based on the CDCA hypothesis, more than 1400 studies have investigated nearly 800 candidate genes for association with schizophrenia (www.szgene.org). Many of these are considered strong susceptibility genes, but none have unequivocal statistical support (5, 6). Combined, well-powered, GWA studies of psychiatric disorders have identified two candidate loci for bipolar disorder (10q21.2 and 12p13.3) (7), one candidate locus shared between bipolar disorder and schizophrenia (10q26.13) (8), and one locus for autism (15p14.1) (9). It is interesting to note that these loci do not include any top candidate genes. While there is some debate about the relative merits of candidate gene versus GWA approaches, both suffer from similar limitations. They both use a restricted set of polymorphic markers that may or may not be linked with other unseen variants. Given that the markers genotyped represent a small fraction of all common genetic variants, significantly associated markers in GWA studies are a priori unlikely to be the true causative variants (10). Moreover, these variants often occur in regulatory regions whose effects are currently unknown or lie distal to genic regions leaving open the question of which genes are affected. Thus, even if statistically unequivocal common risk alleles are found in the near future, these are likely far removed from the actual causative allele. Follow-up studies, including replication and fine mapping
192
Arguello and Gogos
studies, may help in identifying the causal alleles, but even then the functional impact of these may remain uncertain (11). 2.2. The Impact of Rare Genetic Variants
The rarity of families afflicted with psychiatric disorders in a strictly Mendelian fashion limited, until recently, acceptance that rare mutations play a significant etiological role. Taking into account variable penetrance and de novo occurrences, however, rare alleles may contribute substantially to both familial and sporadic cases (12–14). It is not surprising that the alleles with strongest statistical support for association with schizophrenia are rare, highly penetrant structural mutations. These include copy number mutations of 22q11.2 (14–16) and possibly 1q21.1, 5q13.3 (14, 16) as well as a balanced chromosomal translocation t(1;11)(q42.1;q14.3) (17). Moreover, recent surveys indicate that rare copy number mutations as a whole are over-represented in cases of schizophrenia and autism (13, 14, 18). These data suggest that rare alleles contribute substantially to the risk of psychiatric disorders. Unlike common variants, a rare allele may be population, family, or even individual specific. In the latter case, it is not possible to prove unequivocally that the mutation is pathogenic since individuals carry many private mutations. It is possible, however, to determine whether cumulative mutational load for a given locus or biological pathway is greater for affected versus unaffected individuals collectively (4). While current estimates indicate that only a minority of cases (≤1%) are attributable to any given rare, recurrent allele (14, 16), this is likely to be a lower limit. Improvements in the resolution and efficiency of genotyping platforms will increase detection of rare alleles and their association with disease. Moreover, common variants account for only a small proportion of familial risk for common diseases, leaving the possibility that rare alleles contribute significantly to the remaining risk (19). Nevertheless, it is possible that the penetrance or the expressivity of rare mutations is determined by common genetic variation (11).
2.3. Implications for Mutant Animal Models
In order for mutant animal models to uncover a genuine pathogenetic link between a candidate gene and disease risk, the association of the genetic variant must be unequivocal, its functional effect known, and the risk allele modeled accurately. None of these conditions are met for current candidate genes or variants identified through GWA studies and thus the prospect of creating valid animal models based on common alleles is strikingly low (2). Unlike association studies, assessments of rare variants identify specific, and very likely causative, disease-associated alleles. These rare alleles are far more likely to have deleterious functional consequences than are common alleles due to clear effects on gene products. As a result they have higher potential for revealing
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models
193
insights into disease etiology than do less penetrant common alleles (12). In fact, the few candidate genes with common alleles of clear functional effect (e.g., COMT rs4680 and BDNF rs6265 ) show the most inconsistent clinical associations (20, 21). Therefore, because the evidence of disease association for rare alleles is often more statistically sound and the effects of the mutation more transparent, it is possible to engineer genetically faithful, and thus etiologically valid, animal models.
3. Animal Models of DiseaseAssociated Loci
3.1. Neuregulin 1 (NRG1) Susceptibility Locus
Although the selection of candidate genes is controversial and their validation problematic, it is possible to dissect the functional roles of candidate genes and investigate the biological pathways in which they participate. This allows the generation of hypotheses regarding how these pathways may dysfunction and give rise to psychiatric disorders. When interpreting results from animal models of candidate genes, it is important to realize that the selection and the verification of candidate genes are not always independent of each other, making gene validation a potentially circular and self-bootstrapping process. Moreover, the fact that behavioral tests used to characterize models lack specificity raises the issue of whether a suite of behavioral changes can validate a mutant model in the absence of strong corroboration from human genetics. In this section we describe mutant animal models from four candidate genes, including three genes found in genomic loci showing genetic linkage with schizophrenia (NRG1, DTNBP1, and PPP3C). While linkage signals may reflect rare as well as common alleles, current technology allows only the efficient genotyping of common makers. Thus, association studies that follow-up linkage results assume a significant contribution of common variants within these loci. A significant linkage peak at 8p22 implicated Neuregulin 1 (NRG1) as a schizophrenia susceptibility locus. Despite a lack of robust statistical support for specific alleles, NRG1 and its receptor, ERBB4, are among the leading candidate susceptibility genes for schizophrenia (22, 23). NRG1 is a trophic factor that, through binding of receptor tyrosine kinases, is involved in cellular proliferation, migration, neurite outgrowth, synaptic transmission, and intracellular signaling (24). This high level of pleiotropy suggests that genetic variants affecting NRG1 signaling could influence disease risk through any number of biological pathways.
194
Arguello and Gogos
Several groups have created mutant animal models affecting various domains of the Nrg1 gene to assess their impact on behavior. The first group to describe an Nrg1 mutant mouse deleted the EGF-like domain of the encoded protein and showed that these mice displayed increased locomotor activity in multiple contexts but intact short-term memory (25). Stefansson et al. (22) analyzed mutant mice lacking the transmembrane domain of Nrg1 and found that they had increased locomotor activity in an open field and impaired sensorimotor gating as measured with PPI, but normal anxiety-related behaviors. These mice were further analyzed to show abnormal social interactions but normal short-term spatial memory (26, 27). Rimer et al. (28) targeted a subset of Nrg1 isoforms by deleting the Ig domain and found that these mice showed impaired latent inhibition, but normal locomotor activity and short-term memory. Other mutant animal models have focused on Nrg1 receptors. Similar to Nrg1 hypomorphs, homozygous ErbB4 knockout mice, but not ErbB2 or ErbB3 knockouts, showed increased locomotor activity and impaired PPI. Roy et al. (29) used a transgenic approach to express a dominant-negative ErbB4 receptor specifically in oligodendrocytes. In addition to decreased complexity of oligodendrocytes and reduced myelin thickness, these mice showed decreased locomotor activity, increased anxiety-related behaviors, abnormal social interactions, and hyperactive midbrain dopamine function. Barros et al. (30) crossed floxed ErbB2 and ErbB4 mice with CRE-expressing mice to knock out ErbB2/4mediated Nrg1 signaling within the CNS and showed that these mice had increased locomotor activity, increased aggressivity, and decreased PPI. Surprisingly, cortical development of these mice was normal. They did, however, show decreased spine maturation of both neocortical and hippocampal neurons. Savonenko et al. (31) investigated Nrg1 function by assessing knockout mice of one of its proteolytic processors, Bace1, which were previously used as an animal model of Alzheimer’s disease prevention (32). These mice showed increased locomotor activity in several contexts, impaired PPI, abnormal social interactions, impaired fear-associative memory, and impaired spatial short-term memory, but normal long-term spatial reference memory. Mutant mice also showed changes in dendritic complexity and spine density of hippocampal neurons. It is unclear, however, if these effects were due to Nrg1 or one of the other downstream substrates of Bace1. Overall, these studies show that disruptions in Nrg1 signaling affect locomotor activity and sensorimotor gating but have less reliable impact on cognitive function. Given that there is, as of yet, no clear disease-associated functional allele within the NRG1 locus, it is uncertain which, if any, of the current mutant animal models are relevant to schizophrenia.
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models
3.2. DystrobrevinBinding Protein 1 (DTNBP1) Susceptibility Locus
195
DTNBP1 was first identified as a candidate gene by fine mapping of a significant linkage signal at 6p22. Several subsequent association studies indicated that common variants within this locus may influence disease risk. As in the case of NRG1, however, the statistical support is equivocal (5, 6). DTNBP1 is widely expressed across tissues and organs, including the CNS, and is involved in the trafficking of lysosome-related organelles. Although disputed as a strong susceptibility locus, DTNBP1 remains a popular candidate gene and the availability of a naturally occurring null allele in a mouse strain makes it an attractive candidate to model. The sdy strain of mice carries a spontaneous Dtnbp1 null allele resulting from a ∼38-kb deletion that abolishes production of the dysbindin-1 protein (33). These mice show systemic abnormalities in vesicle trafficking to lysosomes, resulting in albinism and pulmonary fibrosis. Takao et al. (34) carried out a behavioral screen of homozygous sdy mice and found that mutant mice had decreased locomotor activity, impaired long-term spatial reference memory, and impaired acquisition of a short-term memory task, but normal anxiety-related behaviors and intact neurological functions. Bhardwaj et al. (35) also characterized sdy mice and showed that homozygous mutant mice had decreased locomotor habituation and increased fear-associative memory. While both heterozygous and homozygous mutants showed impaired shortterm recognition memory and a blunted locomotor response to acute amphetamine, only homozygous mice were hypersensitized to amphetamine after chronic treatment. Feng et al. (36) examined homozygous sdy mice and found they had normal locomotor activity and normal anxiety-related behaviors but impaired social interactions and long-term recognition memory. Cox et al. (37) backcrossed sdy mice into a C57BL/6 background and used wild-type littermate controls. Homozygous mutant mice showed increased locomotor activity, decreased locomotor habituation, impaired spatial navigation, and impaired long-term spatial reference memory but less anxiety-related behaviors and improved motor learning. Given its role in vesicle trafficking, it is not surprising that dysbindin mutant mice show deficits in neurotransmitter release (38). These deficits as well as dopamine dysfunction in these animals (39) may be relevant to the putative role of DTBNP1 in schizophrenia. Although methodological differences, such as the use of littermate controls, may account for contradictory findings from sdy mice, further studies are needed to confirm dysbindin’s role in the CNS and define its neurobiological functions. Like NRG1, however, the lack of clear disease-associated, functional alleles within the DTBNP1 locus makes dysbindin’s role in disease risk unclear. Thus, even though there is evidence of reduced dysbindin expression in schizophrenic brain tissue (40), this makes the relevance of sdy mice to schizophrenia pathogenesis uncertain.
196
Arguello and Gogos
3.3. Other Susceptibility Loci
4. Animal Models of DiseaseAssociated Alleles
Near the 8p22 linkage peak that implicated NRG1 as a candidate gene resides PPP3CC, which encodes for a calcineurin catalytic subunit. There is some support that common variants within this locus are associated with schizophrenia (41). Mice with a conditional knockout of calcineurin’s regulatory subunit provide additional support because they display behavioral deficits similar to animal models of other schizophrenia candidate genes. These deficits include increased locomotor activity, decreased PPI, impaired short-term spatial memory, and impaired reversal learning (42, 43). Variants within the regulatory subunit gene itself, however, have not been associated with schizophrenia. Nevertheless, taken together, genetic and animal model findings point to a potentially important role of this pathway in disease susceptibility, possibly via integrating glutamatergic and dopaminergic signaling as well as modulating synaptic vesicle recycling (44, 45). AKT1, a functional candidate gene, provides another relevant example. While AKT1 was not identified from a linkage peak, AKT1 protein levels and phosphorylation of its substrate GSK3β were reduced in brains and lymphocytes of individuals with schizophrenia (46). AKT1 it is an attractive candidate primarily because of its functional role in dopaminergic signaling (47, 48). Similar to other candidate genes, the support for common variants within this locus increasing disease risk is mixed (www.szgene.org). Nevertheless, mice deficient in Akt1 show a greater sensitivity to the disrupting effects of amphetamine on PPI (46) and have altered dendritic complexity in medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and impaired working memory performance under various pharmacological challenges. Whether any other changes produced by Akt1 deficiency (48) are also relevant to disease risk remains to be determined.
The increasing ability to detect rare genetic lesions strongly associated with psychiatric disorders provides a unique opportunity for generating etiologically valid mutant animal models. In this section we discuss animal models of the strongest inherited and de novo genetic risk factors for psychiatric disorders known: a familial truncation of the DISC1 gene and sporadic copy number mutations of 22q11.2, respectively. Although these lesions are individually rare, the ability to model them accurately allows identification of the exact cellular pathways and neural circuits likely involved in disease pathogenesis.
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models
4.1. The Disruptedin-Schizophrenia 1 (DISC1) Susceptibility Locus
197
DISC1 was identified through a balance chromosomal translocation t(1;11)(q42.1;q14.3) segregating with schizophrenia and mood disorders in a large Scottish pedigree. Like NRG1, DISC1 is a highly pleiotropic gene with effects on cellular proliferation, migration, dendritogenesis, synaptogenesis, and intracellular signaling (49). Association studies suggest that common variants within the DISC1 locus may influence the risk of psychotic and mood disorders in general (50). Like other association studies, however, these studies of DISC1 have not produced any statistically unequivocal risk alleles. This means the well-defined mutation in the Scottish family is currently the only disease-associated allele within the DISC1 locus capable of being modeled validly in animals. While the biology of DISC1 is being interrogated with many analytical tools, such as shRNA (51–53), these are not meant to recapitulate the integrated effects of a genetic lesion. Thus, the relevance of these findings to schizophrenia pathogenesis remains uncertain and, in fact, has produced opposing results from animal models carrying germline mutations (54). We therefore mainly discuss DISC1 mutant models that have attempted to recapitulate the Scottish mutation. We summarize the main findings from these models with a focus on the different methodological approaches used by investigators. Koike et al. (55) were the first to describe a DISC1 animal model. They used a knock-in approach to introduce a truncating lesion in the endogenous murine Disc1 orthologue to resemble the putative effects of the translocation (Fig. 8.2, Disc1tm1Kara ). This abolishes expression of the major Disc1 isoforms and, despite an artificial polyadenylation signal, causes low expression of the truncated protein, indicating its instability under physiological conditions (54). These mutant mice showed no changes in locomotor activity or PPI (55). They did, however, show a unique profile of cognitive impairments. A battery of tests revealed no changes in fear-associative, object recognition, long-term spatial reference, or short-term spatial memories (54). Rather, both heterozygous and homozygous mutant mice exhibited consistent impairments in working memory tests (i.e., short-term memory tests with high demands on cognitive control) (54, 55). Interestingly, the cognitive deficits in mutant mice were in the context of minimal histopathological changes in mPFC, with more evident changes in the hippocampus including changes in the distribution and organization of young and mature dentate gyrus granule cells. While the cognitive profile of translocation carriers is unknown, the cognitive impairments in these mice may relate to the cognitive control deficits prominent in psychotic and mood disorders.
198
Arguello and Gogos
Fig. 8.2. Animal models of the DISC1 locus. (Top) Chromosomal location and genetic structure of the human DISC1 locus. The t(1;11) translocation break point occurs between exons 8 and 9 (arrow) with exons 9–13 (black) relocated to chromosome 11. Below it are the allele symbols and transgenic constructs used to model the functional effects of the translocation. These models interfere with endogenous mouse Disc1 function in a dominant-negative [Tg(Camk2a-DISC1)10Asaw and Tg(tetO/CMVDISC1∗ 1001Plet + Tg(CAmk2a-tTA)1Mmay] or transdominant-negative [Tg(camk2aESR1/DISC1∗ )2698.1Sva] manner. (Bottom) Syntenic chromosomal location and genetic structure of the mouse Disc1 locus and the corresponding break point location (arrow). Below it are the various mouse Disc1 alleles so far described. Disc1del arose from a spontaneous 25-bp deletion within exon 6 that introduces a premature stop codon in
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models
199
Other mouse models were designed under the assumption that the (1;11) translocation produces a truncated protein and thus overexpresses a truncated form of human DISC1. Hikida et al. (56) were the first to produce such a model and used a αCAMKII promoter to drive truncated DISC1 expression in forebrain principal neurons (Fig. 8.2, Tg(Camk2aDISC1)10Asaw). These transgenic animals showed increased lateral ventricular volume in adulthood, reduced numbers of parvalbumin-positive cells in the mPFC, abnormal locomotor activity, subtle deficits in PPI, impaired foraging, and increased immobility in the forced swim test (FST). There were no changes in measures of anxiety-related behaviors, long-term spatial reference memory, or short-term spatial memory as assessed in the Y maze. Pletnikov et al. (57) used the Tet-off double transgenic system to drive expression of truncated human DISC1 under the CMV promoter with tetracycline under the αCAMKII promoter (Fig. 8.2, Tg(tetO/CMV-DISC1∗ )1001Plet). These double transgenic mice showed increased lateral ventricular volume, abnormal locomotor activity, and increased aggression in males but no changes in measures of anxiety-related behaviors, long-term spatial reference memory, olfaction, or PPI. Females did, however, show deficits in long-term spatial reference memory. Unlike the previous studies that expressed an N-terminal fragment of DISC1, Li et al. (58) expressed a C-terminal fragment of DISC1 under the αCAMKII promoter using a single transgenic inducible and reversible system (Fig. 8.2, Tg(Camk2aESR1/DISC1∗ )2698.1Sva). This approach was designed to transiently disrupt Disc1 interactions occurring at the C terminus. Mice briefly exposed to this truncated DISC1 peptide at postnatal day 7 showed abnormal responses to the FST, spatial working memory deficits, and decreased sociability but no changes in locomotor activity or measures of anxiety-related behaviors. Interestingly, no deficits were apparent when the mutant DISC1 fragment was expressed during adulthood.
Fig. 8.2. (continued) exon seven (∗ ) of 129 and related strains of mice. Disc1tm1Kara was engineered to carry a premature stop codon at the end of exon 8 (∗ ) followed by a polyadenylation signal in an attempt to recapitulate the translocation, but since this was created in a 129S6/SveV background, the allele also carries the endogenous 25-bp deletion. Disc1Rgsc1390 and Disc1Rgsc1393 have missense mutations in exon 2 identified through a mutagenesis screen and mice carrying these alleles show divergent behavioral phenotypes. Although not meant to model the translocation and thus not discussed in the main text, these mutations indicate that allelic heterogeneity of the DISC1 locus may account for diverse psychopathologies. Tg(Disc1∗ -eGFP)unannotated is a transgenic allele constructed from an artificial bacterial chromosome consisting of exons 1–8 of the mouse Disc1 fused to enhanced green fluorescent protein (eGFP). Mice carrying two copies of this allele also have two wild-type, endogenous Disc1 copies.
200
Arguello and Gogos
It is important to note that the above transgenic models do not necessarily recapitulate the effect of the diseaseassociated allele found in the Scottish family. The ectopic expression of mutant DISC1 does not reflect Disc1 expression in terms of absolute levels or spatial and temporal patterns. In an attempt to circumvent these limitations, Shen et al. (59) engineered transgenic mice to express two copies of truncated mouse Disc1 using a BAC system, thus, presumably, preserving the temporal and spatial profiles of Disc1’s endogenous expression (Fig. 8.2, Tg(Disc1∗ -EGFP)unannotated). These mice showed increased lateral ventricular volume, decreased neuronal proliferation, thinner cortices, agenesis of the corpus callosum, reduced parvalbumin-positive cells in the PFC and the hippocampus, increased immobility in the FST, and abnormal latent inhibition. While presumably the ratio of mutant to normal Disc1 copies is similar in this model to that in translocation carriers, the absolute dosage is increased by twofold and altered function of N-terminal interacting partners is possible. Although this model has advantages over other transgenic approaches, further clinical characterization of translocation carriers (e.g., MRIs) is needed to determine how accurately this model reflects the human condition. 4.2. The 22q11 Susceptibility Locus
Recent studies of human genetic variation indicate that largescale genomic structural rearrangements are common in the population. One of the most common abnormalities is de novo microdeletion of the 22q11.2 locus. Carriers of this microdeletion are at inordinately high risk of developing schizophrenia with odds ratios exceeding ∼25-fold. Currently, 22q11.2 microdeletions represent the only confirmed recurrent mutation responsible for introducing sporadic cases of schizophrenia into the population. The most common deletion spans 3 Mb, but a nested 1.5 Mb deletion seems to be the schizophrenia critical region (15). Premorbid children with the microdeletion present with variable symptoms including impairments in attention, memory, and cognitive control, suggesting that these cognitive deficits reflect genetic liability to schizophrenia. Fortunately, the human 22q11.2 locus is conserved within the syntenic region of mouse chromosome 16 and harbors nearly all orthologues of the human genes (Fig. 8.3). This provides a unique opportunity to create mutant animal models with a high degree of etiological validity. Kimber et al. (60) were the first to behaviorally characterize a model of the 22q11.2 microdeletion. They engineered mice deficient in seven genes spanning ∼150 kb of the 1.5-Mb deletion syntenic region (Fig. 8.3, Del(16Zpf520Slc25a1)1Awb). Hemizygous mice showed increased PPI but normal locomotor activity, anxiety-related behaviors, fear-associative memory, and neurological function. Paylor et al. (61) characterize a 22q11.2 model spanning ∼1 Mb containing 18
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models
201
Fig. 8.3. Animal models of the 22q11.2 locus. (Top) Chromosomal location and genetic organization of the 22q11.2 locus. Each open circle represents one gene. This 1.5-Mb critical region is flanked by low copy repeat sequences (black boxes), making it prone to non-homologous recombination. PRODH-P and DGCR6-like are pseudogenes. (Bottom) Syntenic region of mouse chromosome 16 and the genetic organization of the corresponding orthologues. Single gene deletion models that have been analyzed with PPI are indicated in solid black circles. Below are the various multigene deletion models that have been characterized behaviorally labeled by their allele symbols. Note that only two models contain all the genes within the 1.5 Mb deletion associated with schizophrenia.
202
Arguello and Gogos
orthologues of the human genes within the 1.5 Mb deletion (Fig. 8.3, Del(16Es2el-Ufd1l)217Bld). They found decreased PPI and impaired fear-associative memory but no changes in measures of anxiety-related behaviors or neurological function. Long et al. (62) used mice with a hemizygous deletion spanning ∼1.3 Mb and containing all of the orthologous genes within the 1.5 Mb deletion (Fig. 8.3, Del(Dgcr2-Hira)1Rak) and found decreased PPI and neurological dysfunction but normal fear-associative memory. Finally, Stark et al. (63) also created mice with a hemizygous deletion syntenic to the human 1.5 Mb deletion (Fig. 8.3, Del(Dgcr2-Hira)2Aam). This deletion resulted in increased locomotor activity, decreased PPI, decreased fear-associative memory, and impaired acquisition of a working memory task. These behavioral changes were in the context of decreased dendritic complexity, reduced spine density and fewer excitatory inputs to CA1 hippocampal neurons. To identify which of the many genes within the 22q11.2 microdeletion may be responsible for the increased risk of schizophrenia, several groups have created single gene deletion models. Gogos et al. (64) described a knockdown model of Prodh, which encodes a mitochondrial enzyme that metabolizes L -proline. These mice showed decreased PPI, increased locomotor activity, hypersensitivity to amphetamine and impaired fearassociative memory but normal working memory (64, 65). Interestingly, transcriptional profiling revealed an increased expression of Comt, a gene within the 22q11.2 microdeletion that catabolizes dopamine. This seemed to be a compensatory response to underlying dopamine dysfunction as pharmacological inhibition of Comt exacerbated the sensitivity to amphetamine and produced working memory deficits in Prodh-deficient mice (65). This animal model suggests that the risk for schizophrenia in 22q11.2 deletion carriers is mediated, at least in part, by an inability to deploy a genetic feedback loop residing within the deletion. Two subsequent clinical studies of human carriers showed an interaction between proline levels and low COMT activity thus providing additional support for this hypothesis (66, 67). Several other orthologues of genes within the microdeletion seem to contribute to the phenotypic profile of the 22q11.2 deletion model. Mukai et al. (68) and Stark et al. (63) showed that mice deficient in Zdhhc8, a palmitoyltransferase, and Dgcr8, a microRNA processor, each account for distinct aspects of the reduced dendritic complexity and spine density of hippocampal neurons from complete deletion mice. Furthermore, Dgcr8 deficiency accounts for the reduced microRNA biogenesis observed in these mice and on its own causes working memory deficits (63). Other groups have shown that several genes within the 1.5-Mb region differentially affect PPI. While heterozygous deficiency of Gscl, Dgcr8, and Tbx1 decreases PPI (62, 63, 69), Rtn4r,
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models
203
Zdhhc8, Comt, and Sept5 deficiencies have no effect (69–72). It is interesting that deletion mice described by Kimber et al. (60) have increased PPI, even though Gscl deficiency itself decreases PPI, thereby suggesting that other genes within this region have opposing influences. Although PPI is not diagnostically specific or predictive of clinical outcome, it is a relatively robust assay and thus underscores the difficulty in teasing apart genotype– phenotype relationships from models of large structural mutations. Most importantly, and despite disparate results due to the various genes deleted within the 22q11.2 locus, mouse models of the 22q11.2 microdeletion have identified specific pathways affecting hippocampal neuronal connectivity and neuromodulation that, in combination, may be critical for the dramatic increase in schizophrenia rates seen in 22q11.2 deletion carriers. Moreover, studies from these models will serve as guidelines for dissecting out the contribution of specific genes within other contiguous gene deletion syndromes that may be associated with psychiatric disorders in the future.
5. Integrating Human Genetics and Mutant Models
5.1. Identifying Disease Pathways
Unlike most common diseases, psychiatric disorders do not have clear etiologies or diagnostic tests. Instead they are syndromes, constellations of commonly co-occurring symptoms that are classified by outward behavioral disturbances. This presents a unique challenge to psychiatric research not evident for other common diseases. In this last section we describe the difficulties in integrating human genetics with animal models in order to validate genetic findings and identify pathogenetic pathways. One major advantage of GWA studies is their unbiased approach to identifying candidate genes. They are not limited by hypotheses about etiology or pathophysiology and can identify previously unsuspected biological pathways. Indeed, this has happened for many common diseases (4). Identifying biological pathways, however, is not equivalent to deciphering disease mechanisms. This is most evident in the case of pleiotropic genes involved in multiple biological pathways across development and adulthood. It is essential to determine how and when specific alterations in these pathways underlie disease pathogenesis. This is a major obstacle for mutant models of genes without clear functional risk alleles because psychiatric disorders lack biological markers, or even behavioral tests, that can validate the model (73).
204
Arguello and Gogos
In order to circumvent these limitations, findings from multiple, etiologically valid animal models are needed. It is likely that no single mutant animal model will provide clear etiological insight. For any individual genetic variant, whether common or rare, only a subset of its downstream functional alterations may be relevant to disease etiology (74). Taken collectively, however, various mutant animal models may identify a final common pathway, leading to psychopathology. The exact level of this convergence, whether at the molecular, cellular, or systems level, remains to be determined. Fortunately, as genotyping technologies advance, so will the ability to create valid animal models and the ability to integrate their phenotypes into a clearer picture of disease pathogenesis. 5.2. Challenges for Moving Beyond Single Genes
The challenge of unraveling biological pathways into disease mechanisms is compounded when interactions between different risk alleles and environmental factors are taken into account. For example, a candidate gene affecting one biological pathway may point to other genes within the same pathway as potential candidates. Without, however, corroborating human genetic findings implicating specific functional risk alleles in those genes, this added layer of complexity will likely create false leads and dead ends. The same applies to modeling gene–gene and gene– environment interactions and will be especially problematic when using animal models of genetic loci instead of specific risk alleles. It remains an open empirical question if risk factors, whether environmental or genetic, will interact with only disease-relevant pathways or also have non-specific effects. Moreover, simply looking for changes that are exaggerated in an animal model of multiple risk factors may be misleading and miss relevant mechanisms. This is because interacting factors may either worsen a pathogenetic process or mitigate a compensatory response (2). In both cases, this would lead to a more severe pathophysiology. Thus, caution is warranted when adding layers of higher order interactions atop shaky foundations of unreliable mutant models as this may increase the difficulty of unearthing relevant disease mechanisms.
Acknowledgments Work in the authors’ laboratory is supported by grants from NIMH, NARSAD, and the Simons Foundation. We thank M. Karayiorgou, B. Levy, and L. Drew for critical readings of the manuscript.
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models
205
References 1. Abbott, A. (2008) Psychiatric genetics: the brains of the family, Nature 454, 154–157. 2. Arguello, P. A., and Gogos, J. A. (2006) Modeling madness in mice: one piece at a time, Neuron 52, 179–196. 3. Desbonnet, L., Waddington, J. L., and O’Tuathaigh, C. M. (2009) Mutant models for genes associated with schizophrenia, Biochem Soc Trans 37, 308–312. 4. Altshuler, D., Daly, M. J., and Lander, E. S. (2008) Genetic mapping in human disease, Science 322, 881–888. 5. Need, A. C., Ge, D., Weale, M. E., Maia, J., Feng, S., Heinzen, E. L., Shianna, K. V., Yoon, W., Kasperaviciute, D., Gennarelli, M., Strittmatter, W. J., Bonvicini, C., Rossi, G., Jayathilake, K., Cola, P. A., McEvoy, J. P., Keefe, R. S., Fisher, E. M., St Jean, P. L., Giegling, I., Hartmann, A. M., Moller, H. J., Ruppert, A., Fraser, G., Crombie, C., Middleton, L. T., St Clair, D., Roses, A. D., Muglia, P., Francks, C., Rujescu, D., Meltzer, H. Y., and Goldstein, D. B. (2009) A genome-wide investigation of SNPs and CNVs in schizophrenia, PLoS Genet 5, e1000373. 6. Sanders, A. R., Duan, J., Levinson, D. F., Shi, J., He, D., Hou, C., Burrell, G. J., Rice, J. P., Nertney, D. A., Olincy, A., Rozic, P., Vinogradov, S., Buccola, N. G., Mowry, B. J., Freedman, R., Amin, F., Black, D. W., Silverman, J. M., Byerley, W. F., Crowe, R. R., Cloninger, C. R., Martinez, M., and Gejman, P. V. (2008) No significant association of 14 candidate genes with schizophrenia in a large European ancestry sample: implications for psychiatric genetics, Am J Psychiatry 165, 497–506. 7. Ferreira, M. A., O’Donovan, M. C., Meng, Y. A., Jones, I. R., Ruderfer, D. M., Jones, L., Fan, J., Kirov, G., Perlis, R. H., Green, E. K., Smoller, J. W., Grozeva, D., Stone, J., Nikolov, I., Chambert, K., Hamshere, M. L., Nimgaonkar, V. L., Moskvina, V., Thase, M. E., Caesar, S., Sachs, G. S., Franklin, J., Gordon-Smith, K., Ardlie, K. G., Gabriel, S. B., Fraser, C., Blumenstiel, B., Defelice, M., Breen, G., Gill, M., Morris, D. W., Elkin, A., Muir, W. J., McGhee, K. A., Williamson, R., MacIntyre, D. J., MacLean, A. W., St, C. D., Robinson, M., Van Beck, M., Pereira, A. C., Kandaswamy, R., McQuillin, A., Collier, D. A., Bass, N. J., Young, A. H., Lawrence, J., Ferrier, I. N., Anjorin, A., Farmer, A., Curtis, D., Scolnick, E. M., McGuffin, P., Daly, M. J., Corvin, A. P., Holmans, P. A., Blackwood, D. H., Gurling, H. M., Owen, M. J.,
8.
9.
10.
11.
Purcell, S. M., Sklar, P., and Craddock, N. (2008) Collaborative genome-wide association analysis supports a role for ANK3 and CACNA1C in bipolar disorder, Nat Genet 40, 1056–1058. O’Donovan, M. C., Craddock, N., Norton, N., Williams, H., Peirce, T., Moskvina, V., Nikolov, I., Hamshere, M., Carroll, L., Georgieva, L., Dwyer, S., Holmans, P., Marchini, J. L., Spencer, C. C., Howie, B., Leung, H. T., Hartmann, A. M., Moller, H. J., Morris, D. W., Shi, Y., Feng, G., Hoffmann, P., Propping, P., Vasilescu, C., Maier, W., Rietschel, M., Zammit, S., Schumacher, J., Quinn, E. M., Schulze, T. G., Williams, N. M., Giegling, I., Iwata, N., Ikeda, M., Darvasi, A., Shifman, S., He, L., Duan, J., Sanders, A. R., Levinson, D. F., Gejman, P. V., Cichon, S., Nothen, M. M., Gill, M., Corvin, A., Rujescu, D., Kirov, G., Owen, M. J., Buccola, N. G., Mowry, B. J., Freedman, R., Amin, F., Black, D. W., Silverman, J. M., Byerley, W. F., and Cloninger, C. R. (2008) Identification of loci associated with schizophrenia by genome-wide association and follow-up, Nat Genet 40, 1053–1055. Wang, K., Zhang, H., Ma, D., Bucan, M., Glessner, J. T., Abrahams, B. S., Salyakina, D., Imielinski, M., Bradfield, J. P., Sleiman, P. M., Kim, C. E., Hou, C., Frackelton, E., Chiavacci, R., Takahashi, N., Sakurai, T., Rappaport, E., Lajonchere, C. M., Munson, J., Estes, A., Korvatska, O., Piven, J., Sonnenblick, L. I., Alvarez Retuerto, A. I., Herman, E. I., Dong, H., Hutman, T., Sigman, M., Ozonoff, S., Klin, A., Owley, T., Sweeney, J. A., Brune, C. W., Cantor, R. M., Bernier, R., Gilbert, J. R., Cuccaro, M. L., McMahon, W. M., Miller, J., State, M. W., Wassink, T. H., Coon, H., Levy, S. E., Schultz, R. T., Nurnberger, J. I., Haines, J. L., Sutcliffe, J. S., Cook, E. H., Minshew, N. J., Buxbaum, J. D., Dawson, G., Grant, S. F., Geschwind, D. H., Pericak-Vance, M. A., Schellenberg, G. D., and Hakonarson, H. (2009) Common genetic variants on 5p14.1 associate with autism spectrum disorders, Nature 459(7246), 528–533. Ioannidis, J. P., Thomas, G., and Daly, M. J. (2009) Validating, augmenting and refining genome-wide association signals, Nat Rev Genet 10, 318–329. Frazer, K. A., Murray, S. S., Schork, N. J., and Topol, E. J. (2009) Human genetic variation and its contribution to complex traits, Nat Rev Genet 10, 241–251.
206
Arguello and Gogos
12. Bodmer, W., and Bonilla, C. (2008) Common and rare variants in multifactorial susceptibility to common diseases, Nat Genet 40, 695–701. 13. Xu, B., Roos, J. L., Levy, S., van Rensburg, E. J., Gogos, J. A., and Karayiorgou, M. (2008) Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with sporadic schizophrenia, Nat Genet 40, 880–885. 14. ISC. (2008) Rare chromosomal deletions and duplications increase risk of schizophrenia, Nature 455, 237–241. 15. Karayiorgou, M., Morris, M. A., Morrow, B., Shprintzen, R. J., Goldberg, R., Borrow, J., Gos, A., Nestadt, G., Wolyniec, P. S., Lasseter, V. K., et al. (1995) Schizophrenia susceptibility associated with interstitial deletions of chromosome 22q11, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92, 7612–7616. 16. Stefansson, H., Rujescu, D., Cichon, S., Pietilainen, O. P., Ingason, A., Steinberg, S., Fossdal, R., Sigurdsson, E., Sigmundsson, T., Buizer-Voskamp, J. E., Hansen, T., Jakobsen, K. D., Muglia, P., Francks, C., Matthews, P. M., Gylfason, A., Halldorsson, B. V., Gudbjartsson, D., Thorgeirsson, T. E., Sigurdsson, A., Jonasdottir, A., Bjornsson, A., Mattiasdottir, S., Blondal, T., Haraldsson, M., Magnusdottir, B. B., Giegling, I., Moller, H. J., Hartmann, A., Shianna, K. V., Ge, D., Need, A. C., Crombie, C., Fraser, G., Walker, N., Lonnqvist, J., Suvisaari, J., Tuulio-Henriksson, A., Paunio, T., Toulopoulou, T., Bramon, E., Di Forti, M., Murray, R., Ruggeri, M., Vassos, E., Tosato, S., Walshe, M., Li, T., Vasilescu, C., Muhleisen, T. W., Wang, A. G., Ullum, H., Djurovic, S., Melle, I., Olesen, J., Kiemeney, L. A., Franke, B., Sabatti, C., Freimer, N. B., Gulcher, J. R., Thorsteinsdottir, U., Kong, A., Andreassen, O. A., Ophoff, R. A., Georgi, A., Rietschel, M., Werge, T., Petursson, H., Goldstein, D. B., Nothen, M. M., Peltonen, L., Collier, D. A., St Clair, D., and Stefansson, K. (2008) Large recurrent microdeletions associated with schizophrenia, Nature 455, 232–236. 17. St Clair, D., Blackwood, D., Muir, W., Carothers, A., Walker, M., Spowart, G., Gosden, C., and Evans, H. J. (1990) Association within a family of a balanced autosomal translocation with major mental illness, Lancet 336, 13–16. 18. Sebat, J., Lakshmi, B., Malhotra, D., Troge, J., Lese-Martin, C., Walsh, T., Yamrom, B., Yoon, S., Krasnitz, A., Kendall, J., Leotta, A., Pai, D., Zhang, R., Lee, Y. H., Hicks, J., Spence, S. J., Lee, A. T., Puura, K., Lehtimaki, T., Ledbetter, D., Gregersen, P. K.,
19. 20.
21.
22.
23.
24.
25.
26.
Bregman, J., Sutcliffe, J. S., Jobanputra, V., Chung, W., Warburton, D., King, M. C., Skuse, D., Geschwind, D. H., Gilliam, T. C., Ye, K., and Wigler, M. (2007) Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism, Science 316, 445–449. Goldstein, D. B. (2009) Common genetic variation and human traits, N Engl J Med 360, 1696–1698. Fan, J. B., Zhang, C. S., Gu, N. F., Li, X. W., Sun, W. W., Wang, H. Y., Feng, G. Y., St Clair, D., and He, L. (2005) Catechol-Omethyltransferase gene Val/Met functional polymorphism and risk of schizophrenia: a large-scale association study plus metaanalysis, Biol Psychiatry 57, 139–144. Kanazawa, T., Glatt, S. J., Kia-Keating, B., Yoneda, H., and Tsuang, M. T. (2007) Meta-analysis reveals no association of the Val66Met polymorphism of brain-derived neurotrophic factor with either schizophrenia or bipolar disorder, Psychiatr Genet 17, 165–170. Stefansson, H., Sigurdsson, E., Steinthorsdottir, V., Bjornsdottir, S., Sigmundsson, T., Ghosh, S., Brynjolfsson, J., Gunnarsdottir, S., Ivarsson, O., Chou, T. T., Hjaltason, O., Birgisdottir, B., Jonsson, H., Gudnadottir, V. G., Gudmundsdottir, E., Bjornsson, A., Ingvarsson, B., Ingason, A., Sigfusson, S., Hardardottir, H., Harvey, R. P., Lai, D., Zhou, M., Brunner, D., Mutel, V., Gonzalo, A., Lemke, G., Sainz, J., Johannesson, G., Andresson, T., Gudbjartsson, D., Manolescu, A., Frigge, M. L., Gurney, M. E., Kong, A., Gulcher, J. R., Petursson, H., and Stefansson, K. (2002) Neuregulin 1 and susceptibility to schizophrenia, Am J Hum Genet 71, 877–892. Law, A. J., Kleinman, J. E., Weinberger, D. R., Weickert, C. S. (2007) Diseaseassociated intronic variants in the ErbB4 gene are related to altered ErbB4 splice-variant expression in the brain in schizophrenia, Hum Mol Genet 16(2), 129–141. Mei, L., and Xiong, W. C. (2008) Neuregulin 1 in neural development, synaptic plasticity and schizophrenia, Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 437–452. Gerlai, R., Pisacane, P., and Erickson, S. (2000) Heregulin, but not ErbB2 or ErbB3, heterozygous mutant mice exhibit hyperactivity in multiple behavioral tasks, Behav Brain Res 109, 219–227. O’Tuathaigh, C. M., Babovic, D., O’Sullivan, G. J., Clifford, J. J., Tighe, O., Croke, D. T., Harvey, R., and Waddington, J. L. (2007) Phenotypic characterization of spatial cognition and social behavior in mice
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models
27.
28.
29.
30.
31.
32.
33.
with ‘knockout’ of the schizophrenia risk gene neuregulin 1, Neuroscience 147, 18–27. O’Tuathaigh, C. M., O’Connor, A. M., O’Sullivan, G. J., Lai, D., Harvey, R., Croke, D. T., and Waddington, J. L. (2008) Disruption to social dyadic interactions but not emotional/anxiety-related behaviour in mice with heterozygous ‘knockout’ of the schizophrenia risk gene neuregulin-1, Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32, 462–466. Rimer, M., Barrett, D. W., Maldonado, M. A., Vock, V. M., and Gonzalez-Lima, F. (2005) Neuregulin-1 immunoglobulin-like domain mutant mice: clozapine sensitivity and impaired latent inhibition, Neuroreport 16, 271–275. Roy, K., Murtie, J. C., El-Khodor, B. F., Edgar, N., Sardi, S. P., Hooks, B. M., Benoit-Marand, M., Chen, C., Moore, H., O’Donnell, P., Brunner, D., and Corfas, G. (2007) Loss of erbB signaling in oligodendrocytes alters myelin and dopaminergic function, a potential mechanism for neuropsychiatric disorders, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104, 8131–8136. Barros, C. S., Calabrese, B., Chamero, P., Roberts, A. J., Korzus, E., Lloyd, K., Stowers, L., Mayford, M., Halpain, S., and Muller, U. (2009) Impaired maturation of dendritic spines without disorganization of cortical cell layers in mice lacking NRG1/ErbB signaling in the central nervous system, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106, 4507–4512. Savonenko, A. V., Melnikova, T., Laird, F. M., Stewart, K. A., Price, D. L., and Wong, P. C. (2008) Alteration of BACE1-dependent NRG1/ErbB4 signaling and schizophrenialike phenotypes in BACE1-null mice, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105, 5585–5590. Ohno, M., Sametsky, E. A., Younkin, L. H., Oakley, H., Younkin, S. G., Citron, M., Vassar, R., and Disterhoft, J. F. (2004) BACE1 deficiency rescues memory deficits and cholinergic dysfunction in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease, Neuron 41, 27–33. Li, W., Zhang, Q., Oiso, N., Novak, E. K., Gautam, R., O’Brien, E. P., Tinsley, C. L., Blake, D. J., Spritz, R. A., Copeland, N. G., Jenkins, N. A., Amato, D., Roe, B. A., Starcevic, M., Dell’Angelica, E. C., Elliott, R. W., Mishra, V., Kingsmore, S. F., Paylor, R. E., and Swank, R. T. (2003) HermanskyPudlak syndrome type 7 (HPS-7) results from mutant dysbindin, a member of the biogenesis of lysosome-related organelles complex 1 (BLOC-1), Nat Genet 35, 84–89.
207
34. Takao, K., Toyama, K., Nakanishi, K., Hattori, S., Takamura, H., Takeda, M., Miyakawa, T., and Hashimoto, R. (2008) Impaired long-term memory retention and working memory in sdy mutant mice with a deletion in Dtnbp1, a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia, Mol Brain 1, 11. 35. Bhardwaj, S. K., Baharnoori, M., SharifAskari, B., Kamath, A., Williams, S., and Srivastava, L. K. (2009) Behavioral characterization of dysbindin-1 deficient sandy mice, Behav Brain Res 197, 435–441. 36. Feng, Y. Q., Zhou, Z. Y., He, X., Wang, H., Guo, X. L., Hao, C. J., Guo, Y., Zhen, X. C., and Li, W. (2008) Dysbindin deficiency in sandy mice causes reduction of snapin and displays behaviors related to schizophrenia, Schizophr Res 106, 218–228. 37. Cox, M. M., Tucker, A. M., Tang, J., Talbot, K., Richer, D. C., Yeh, L., and Arnold, S. E. (2009) Neurobehavioral abnormalities in the dysbindin-1 mutant, sandy, on a C57BL/6 J genetic background, Genes Brain Behav 8(4), 390–397. 38. Chen, X. W., Feng, Y. Q., Hao, C. J., Guo, X. L., He, X., Zhou, Z. Y., Guo, N., Huang, H. P., Xiong, W., Zheng, H., Zuo, P. L., Zhang, C. X., Li, W., and Zhou, Z. (2008) DTNBP1, a schizophrenia susceptibility gene, affects kinetics of transmitter release, J Cell Biol 181, 791–801. 39. Murotani, T., Ishizuka, T., Hattori, S., Hashimoto, R., Matsuzaki, S., and Yamatodani, A. (2007) High dopamine turnover in the brains of Sandy mice, Neurosci Lett 421, 47–51. 40. Weickert, C. S., Straub, R. E., McClintock, B. W., Matsumoto, M., Hashimoto, R., Hyde, T. M., Herman, M. M., Weinberger, D. R., and Kleinman, J. E. (2004) Human dysbindin (DTNBP1) gene expression in normal brain and in schizophrenic prefrontal cortex and midbrain, Arch Gen Psychiatry 61, 544–555. 41. Gerber, D. J., Hall, D., Miyakawa, T., Demars, S., Gogos, J. A., Karayiorgou, M., and Tonegawa, S. (2003) Evidence for association of schizophrenia with genetic variation in the 8p21.3 gene, PPP3CC, encoding the calcineurin gamma subunit, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100, 8993–8998. 42. Miyakawa, T., Leiter, L. M., Gerber, D. J., Gainetdinov, R. R., Sotnikova, T. D., Zeng, H., Caron, M. G., and Tonegawa, S. (2003) Conditional calcineurin knockout mice exhibit multiple abnormal behaviors related to schizophrenia, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100, 8987–8992.
208
Arguello and Gogos
43. Zeng, H., Chattarji, S., Barbarosie, M., Rondi-Reig, L., Philpot, B. D., Miyakawa, T., Bear, M. F., and Tonegawa, S. (2001) Forebrain-specific calcineurin knockout selectively impairs bidirectional synaptic plasticity and working/episodic-like memory, Cell 107, 617–629. 44. Greengard, P., Allen, P. B., and Nairn, A. C. (1999) Beyond the dopamine receptor: the DARPP-32/protein phosphatase-1 cascade, Neuron 23, 435–447. 45. Cousin, M. A., and Robinson, P. J. (2001) The dephosphins: dephosphorylation by calcineurin triggers synaptic vesicle endocytosis, Trends Neurosci 24, 659–665. 46. Emamian, E. S., Hall, D., Birnbaum, M. J., Karayiorgou, M., and Gogos, J. A. (2004) Convergent evidence for impaired AKT1GSK3beta signaling in schizophrenia, Nat Genet 36, 131–137. 47. Beaulieu, J. M., Sotnikova, T. D., Marion, S., Lefkowitz, R. J., Gainetdinov, R. R., and Caron, M. G. (2005) An Akt/betaarrestin 2/PP2A signaling complex mediates dopaminergic neurotransmission and behavior, Cell 122, 261–273. 48. Arguello, P. A., and Gogos, J. A. (2008) A signaling pathway AKTing up in schizophrenia, J Clin Invest 118, 2018–2021. 49. Chubb, J. E., Bradshaw, N. J., Soares, D. C., Porteous, D. J., and Millar, J. K. (2008) The DISC locus in psychiatric illness, Mol Psychiatry 13, 36–64. 50. Schumacher, J., Laje, G., Abou Jamra, R., Becker, T., Muhleisen, T. W., Vasilescu, C., Mattheisen, M., Herms, S., Hoffmann, P., Hillmer, A. M., Georgi, A., Herold, C., Schulze, T. G., Propping, P., Rietschel, M., McMahon, F. J., Nothen, M. M., and Cichon, S. (2009) The DISC locus and schizophrenia – Evidence from an association study in a central European sample and from a meta-analysis across different European populations, Hum Mol Genet 18(14), 2719–2727. 51. Duan, X., Chang, J. H., Ge, S., Faulkner, R. L., Kim, J. Y., Kitabatake, Y., Liu, X. B., Yang, C. H., Jordan, J. D., Ma, D. K., Liu, C. Y., Ganesan, S., Cheng, H. J., Ming, G. L., Lu, B., and Song, H. (2007) Disruptedin-schizophrenia 1 regulates integration of newly generated neurons in the adult brain, Cell 130, 1146–1158. 52. Kamiya, A., Kubo, K., Tomoda, T., Takaki, M., Youn, R., Ozeki, Y., Sawamura, N., Park, U., Kudo, C., Okawa, M., Ross, C. A., Hatten, M. E., Nakajima, K., and Sawa, A. (2005) A schizophrenia-associated mutation
53.
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
59.
of DISC1 perturbs cerebral cortex development, Nat Cell Biol 7, 1167–1178. Mao, Y., Ge, X., Frank, C. L., Madison, J. M., Koehler, A. N., Doud, M. K., Tassa, C., Berry, E. M., Soda, T., Singh, K. K., Biechele, T., Petryshen, T. L., Moon, R. T., Haggarty, S. J., and Tsai, L. H. (2009) Disrupted in schizophrenia 1 regulates neuronal progenitor proliferation via modulation of GSK3beta/beta-catenin signaling, Cell 136, 1017–1031. Kvajo, M., McKellar, H., Arguello, P. A., Drew, L. J., Moore, H., MacDermott, A. B., Karayiorgou, M., and Gogos, J. A. (2008) A mutation in mouse Disc1 that models a schizophrenia risk allele leads to specific alterations in neuronal architecture and cognition, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105, 7076–7081. Koike, H., Arguello, P. A., Kvajo, M., Karayiorgou, M., and Gogos, J. A. (2006) Disc1 is mutated in the 129S6/SvEv strain and modulates working memory in mice, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103, 3693–3697. Hikida, T., Jaaro-Peled, H., Seshadri, S., Oishi, K., Hookway, C., Kong, S., Wu, D., Xue, R., Andrade, M., Tankou, S., Mori, S., Gallagher, M., Ishizuka, K., Pletnikov, M., Kida, S., and Sawa, A. (2007) Dominant-negative DISC1 transgenic mice display schizophrenia-associated phenotypes detected by measures translatable to humans, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104, 14501–14506. Pletnikov, M. V., Ayhan, Y., Nikolskaia, O., Xu, Y., Ovanesov, M. V., Huang, H., Mori, S., Moran, T. H., and Ross, C. A. (2008) Inducible expression of mutant human DISC1 in mice is associated with brain and behavioral abnormalities reminiscent of schizophrenia, Mol Psychiatry 13, 173–186, 115. Li, W., Zhou, Y., Jentsch, J. D., Brown, R. A., Tian, X., Ehninger, D., Hennah, W., Peltonen, L., Lonnqvist, J., Huttunen, M. O., Kaprio, J., Trachtenberg, J. T., Silva, A. J., and Cannon, T. D. (2007) Specific developmental disruption of disrupted-in-schizophrenia-1 function results in schizophrenia-related phenotypes in mice, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104, 18280–18285. Shen, S., Lang, B., Nakamoto, C., Zhang, F., Pu, J., Kuan, S. L., Chatzi, C., He, S., Mackie, I., Brandon, N. J., Marquis, K. L., Day, M., Hurko, O., McCaig, C. D., Riedel, G., and St Clair, D. (2008) Schizophreniarelated neural and behavioral phenotypes in transgenic mice expressing truncated Disc1, J Neurosci 28, 10893–10904.
Psychiatric Genetics and the Generation of Mutant Animal Models 60. Kimber, W. L., Hsieh, P., Hirotsune, S., Yuva-Paylor, L., Sutherland, H. F., Chen, A., Ruiz-Lozano, P., HoogstratenMiller, S. L., Chien, K. R., Paylor, R., Scambler, P. J., and Wynshaw-Boris, A. (1999) Deletion of 150 kb in the minimal DiGeorge/velocardiofacial syndrome critical region in mouse, Hum Mol Genet 8, 2229–2237. 61. Paylor, R., McIlwain, K. L., McAninch, R., Nellis, A., Yuva-Paylor, L. A., Baldini, A., and Lindsay, E. A. (2001) Mice deleted for the DiGeorge/velocardiofacial syndrome region show abnormal sensorimotor gating and learning and memory impairments, Hum Mol Genet 10, 2645–2650. 62. Long, J. M., LaPorte, P., Merscher, S., Funke, B., Saint-Jore, B., Puech, A., Kucherlapati, R., Morrow, B. E., Skoultchi, A. I., and Wynshaw-Boris, A. (2006) Behavior of mice with mutations in the conserved region deleted in velocardiofacial/DiGeorge syndrome, Neurogenetics 7, 247–257. 63. Stark, K. L., Xu, B., Bagchi, A., Lai, W. S., Liu, H., Hsu, R., Wan, X., Pavlidis, P., Mills, A. A., Karayiorgou, M., and Gogos, J. A. (2008) Altered brain microRNA biogenesis contributes to phenotypic deficits in a 22q11-deletion mouse model, Nat Genet 40, 751–760. 64. Gogos, J. A., Santha, M., Takacs, Z., Beck, K. D., Luine, V., Lucas, L. R., Nadler, J. V., and Karayiorgou, M. (1999) The gene encoding proline dehydrogenase modulates sensorimotor gating in mice, Nat Genet 21, 434–439. 65. Paterlini, M., Zakharenko, S. S., Lai, W. S., Qin, J., Zhang, H., Mukai, J., Westphal, K. G., Olivier, B., Sulzer, D., Pavlidis, P., Siegelbaum, S. A., Karayiorgou, M., and Gogos, J. A. (2005) Transcriptional and behavioral interaction between 22q11.2 orthologs modulates schizophrenia-related phenotypes in mice, Nat Neurosci 8, 1586–1594. 66. Raux, G., Bumsel, E., Hecketsweiler, B., van Amelsvoort, T., Zinkstok, J., ManouvrierHanu, S., Fantini, C., Breviere, G. M., Di Rosa, G., Pustorino, G., Vogels, A., Swillen, A., Legallic, S., Bou, J., Opolczynski, G., Drouin-Garraud, V., Lemarchand, M., Philip, N., Gerard-Desplanches, A., Carlier, M., Philippe, A., Nolen, M. C., Heron, D., Sarda, P., Lacombe, D., Coizet, C., Alem-
67.
68.
69.
70.
71.
72.
73. 74.
209
bik, Y., Layet, V., Afenjar, A., Hannequin, D., Demily, C., Petit, M., Thibaut, F., Frebourg, T., and Campion, D. (2007) Involvement of hyperprolinemia in cognitive and psychiatric features of the 22q11 deletion syndrome, Hum Mol Genet 16, 83–91. Vorstman, J. A., Turetsky, B. I., SijmensMorcus, M. E., de Sain, M. G., Dorland, B., Sprong, M., Rappaport, E. F., Beemer, F. A., Emanuel, B. S., Kahn, R. S., van Engeland, H., and Kemner, C. (2009) Proline affects brain function in 22q11DS children with the low activity COMT 158 allele, Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 739–746. Mukai, J., Dhilla, A., Drew, L. J., Stark, K. L., Cao, L., MacDermott, A. B., Karayiorgou, M., and Gogos, J. A. (2008) Palmitoylationdependent neurodevelopmental deficits in a mouse model of 22q11 microdeletion, Nat Neurosci 11, 1302–1310. Paylor, R., Glaser, B., Mupo, A., Ataliotis, P., Spencer, C., Sobotka, A., Sparks, C., Choi, C. H., Oghalai, J., Curran, S., Murphy, K. C., Monks, S., Williams, N., O’Donovan, M. C., Owen, M. J., Scambler, P. J., and Lindsay, E. (2006) Tbx1 haploinsufficiency is linked to behavioral disorders in mice and humans: implications for 22q11 deletion syndrome, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103, 7729–7734. Mukai, J., Liu, H., Burt, R. A., Swor, D. E., Lai, W. S., Karayiorgou, M., and Gogos, J. A. (2004) Evidence that the gene encoding ZDHHC8 contributes to the risk of schizophrenia, Nat Genet 36, 725–731. Gogos, J. A., Morgan, M., Luine, V., Santha, M., Ogawa, S., Pfaff, D., and Karayiorgou, M. (1998) Catechol-O-methyltransferasedeficient mice exhibit sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine levels and behavior, Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95, 9991–9996. Hsu, R., Woodroffe, A., Lai, W. S., Cook, M. N., Mukai, J., Dunning, J. P., Swanson, D. J., Roos, J. L., Abecasis, G. R., Karayiorgou, M., and Gogos, J. A. (2007) Nogo receptor 1 (RTN4R) as a candidate gene for schizophrenia: analysis using human and mouse genetic approaches, PLoS One 2, e1234. Insel, T. R. (2007) From animal models to model animals, Biol Psychiatry 62, 1337– 1339. Todd, J. A. (2006) Statistical false positive or true disease pathway? Nat Genet 38, 731–733.
Chapter 9 DISC1 Mouse Models Yavuz Ayhan, Hanna Jaaro-Peled, Akira Sawa, and Mikhail V. Pletnikov Abstract Disrupted-in-Schizophrenia 1 (DISC1) is a strong candidate gene for schizophrenia and major mental disorders. After its discovery in the Scottish chromosomal translocation, DISC1 has gained considerable attention in neuropsychiatric research. Recent studies have implicated DISC1 in fundamental processes of neurodevelopment and adulthood neuroplasticity. To get more insights into the functions of DISC1 in vivo, several mouse DISC1 models have been generated based on different approaches, including constitutive and inducible over-expression of different fragments of DISC1, targeted mutagenesis, and viral vector knockdown. Each model has provided important information regarding DISC1 functions and helped in elucidating the molecular pathways underlying behavioral disorders. The existing models also serve as valuable tools to address complex issues of the pathogenesis of schizophrenia, including gene– gene and gene–environment interactions. Here, we critically overview current DISC1 mouse models. Future directions in DISC1 mouse models and alternative approaches are discussed. Key words: DISC1, schizophrenia, depression, mouse models, neurodevelopment.
1. Introduction Animal models have been instrumental in advancing psychiatric research (1–4). Various pharmacological or lesion-based animal models of schizophrenia have been proposed (5). The progress in human psychiatric genetics and identification of several candidate genes for schizophrenia has stimulated generation of a number of genetic mouse models of the disease (6–8). In this chapter, we will evaluate Disrupted-in-Schizophrenia 1 (DISC1) genetic mouse models. We will first overview the discovery of the DISC1
P. O’Donnell (ed.), Animal Models of Schizophrenia and Related Disorders, Neuromethods 59, DOI 10.1007/978-1-61779-157-4_9, © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
211
212
Ayhan et al.
gene and its significance for psychiatry and neurodevelopment, and we will critically discuss existing DISC1 mouse models. The DISC1 gene was first identified in a t(1;11) translocation in a Scottish pedigree with high load of major mental disorders (9). The LOD ratio for this translocation is 7.1 for all mental disorders and 3.6 for schizophrenia (10). Fine mapping and cloning resulted in identification of a disrupted gene at chromosome 1, and hence the name disrupted-in-schizophrenia 1. The breakpoint was in the middle of an open reading frame region for the gene, suggesting the possibility of expression of a truncated product, although its existence remains to be demonstrated (9, 11). The existence of a clear, identifiable mutation with high LOD scores has put DISC1 in a unique position in schizophrenia research. In the neuropsychiatry literature, there are examples of how such mutations have been instrumental in enlightening the biology of diseases, including familial forms of Parkinson disease and Alzheimer disease (12–15). Thus, the familial DISC1 mutation stimulated studies of the gene’s functions and effects of the mutation on neurodevelopment (16–18). Human genetic research has associated different haplotypes or SNPs of DISC1 with various mental disorders such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, depression, and autism; also with neurocognitive features; and with structural brain changes in human studies (19–22). Early in vitro studies have demonstrated a role of DISC1 in neuronal proliferation, migration, and synaptogenesis (23, 24). In order to provide better understanding of the function of DISC1 in the developing and adult brain in vivo, animal models were generated to evaluate both the outcomes of the translocation mutation and the effects of different genetic manipulations. In the following text, we will introduce DISC1 models published to date and discuss their strengths and weaknesses. Models that over-express the proposed mutant DISC1 protein product are presented as a separate group.
2. Methods 2.1. Genetic Manipulation In Utero 2.1.1. Background
2.1.2. Approach
This model was reported when the DISC1 neurobiology research was mostly limited to cell culture techniques. At the time, the structure of the gene and its main interactors were identified (25, 26). DISC1 was shown to play a role in neurite outgrowth via interaction with NDEL (17) and participate in axonal elongation by binding to FEZ1 (27). In order to characterize the effects of DISC1 on neuronal migration and dendrite arborization in vivo, in utero electroporation approach was used to knock down expression of endogenous
DISC1 Mouse Models
213
mouse DISC1 or to over-express mutant human DISC1 (18). Small interference (si) RNAs were injected directly into the ventricular zones of mouse embryos at embryonic day 14.5. The brain morphology was assessed at postnatal day (P)2 and P14. 2.1.3. Results
In utero electroporation of Disc1 siRNA or human mutant DISC1 resulted in deficits of neuronal maturation and differentiation of cortical neurons. Knockdown of Disc1 expression produced delayed migration of neurons as evidenced by fewer cells that reached the superficial cortical layers at P2. When evaluated at P14, mis-positioned neurons with shorter arbors were found at cortical layers II/III. Similar effects were also observed after electroporation of mutant DISC1.
2.1.4. Conclusion
This model is the first in vivo model to demonstrate the role of DISC1 in neurodevelopment of the cortex. On-going investigations are evaluating the long-term effects of knockdown of Disc1 in developing cortex, including resultant behavioral alterations. In addition, future research should attempt to determine what Disc1 isoforms may be responsible for the observed effects.
2.2. DISC1 Knockdown In Vivo 2.2.1. Adult Neurogenesis 2.2.1.1. Background
The effects of knockdown of Disc1 on cortical development had suggested a possible role for this gene in adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus, a phenomenon thought to be involved in some mental disorders, e.g., mood disorders (28, 29).
2.2.1.2. Approach
Duan et al. used RNAi approach to identify the effects of knockdown of Disc1 on neuronal progenitor cells in the adult hippocampus when postnatal neurogenesis takes place (30). Oncoretroviral constructs engineered to co-express green fluorescent protein (GFP) and Disc1 shRNA were injected in the hilar region of the hippocampus of 2-month-old female C57BL/6 mice.
2.2.1.3. Results
The authors found no difference in the number of immature neurons. However, there were significant morphological differences in newborn cells. Neurons with Disc1 knockdown exhibited multiple primary dendrites, a significant amount of neurons maintained their basal dendrites, and variable degrees of the ectopic dendrite phenotype were observed. The migration of newly born neurons was augmented without affecting differentiation. Disc1 knockdown also altered structure of dendrites. By reconstruction of dendrite arborization, dendrite complexity was found to be increased after treatment with Disc1 shRNA. The functional maturation and integration of these newborn neurons into existing networks was assessed by
214
Ayhan et al.
electrophysiological analysis of membrane resistance and the ability to fire repetitively, as functional markers of maturation. Knockdown of Disc1 produced significantly lower cell membrane resistance and increased cells’ ability to fire repetitively, indicating accelerated functional maturation. Also, dendritic spines were formed earlier and faster. Furthermore, the electrophysiological data indicated accelerated formation of functional GABAergic and glutamatergic synaptic inputs to new neurons. 2.2.1.4. Conclusions
The effects on adult neurogenesis reported by this chapter have suggested multiple and varying functions of DISC1 across different stages of prenatal and postnatal neurodevelopment. In addition, given the growing literature on the role of adult neurogenesis in mood disorders and schizophrenia, this study has suggested a new mechanism whereby DISC1 can be involved in the pathogenesis of major psychiatric diseases.
2.2.2. Progenitor Proliferation and a New DISC1 Pathway
Further evidence of the role of DISC1 in neurogenesis has been provided by the observation of high expression of DISC1 in neural progenitor cells in the ventricular (VZ) and subventricular zones (SVZ), with expression being reduced when cells exit the cell cycle (28).
2.2.2.1. Background 2.2.2.2. Approach
Disc1 shRNAs were electroporated at E13 into the embryonic brains to evaluate the effects of DISC1 on proliferation of neuronal progenitor cells and their fate. The brain samples were processed at postnatal day 7. Another approach included injections of Disc1 shRNAs into the hippocampus to assess the effects on adult neurogenesis.
2.2.2.3. Results
Knockdown of Disc1 at E13 decreased proliferation of neuronal progenitors as indicated by reduced numbers of cells at VZ/SVZ, reduced BrdU labeling, and decreased mitotic index. Disc1 overexpression had the opposite effects. Knockdown also caused premature neuronal differentiation and increased differentiation. These effects of DISC1 were shown to be related to a direct interaction of DISC1 with GSK3β, leading to decreased activity of the enzyme and reduced phosphorylation of β-catenin and its rescue from ubiquitinization. Increased levels of β-catenin in neural progenitors elevated levels of targets of this transcriptional factor and rescued the effects of Disc1 knockdown on cell cycle. In vitro experiments demonstrated that DISC1 regulated β-catenin abundance and stability. Consistent with the prior report, knockdown of Disc1 in granule cells of the adult dentate gyrus decreased neurogenesis and, additionally, was associated with novelty-induced hyperlocomotion and increased floating time in forced swim test. Remarkably, all these brain and behavioral effects of knockdown of Disc1 were rescued by administration of a selective GSK3β inhibitor.
DISC1 Mouse Models
215
2.2.2.4. Conclusions
The study has demonstrated a role of DISC1 in proliferation of embryonic and adult neural progenitor cells and identified a new molecular pathway whereby DISC1 is involved in regulation of proliferation of neuronal progenitor cells, lending additional support for the proposed role of neurogenesis in human mental disorders (29, 30). Given some opposite effects of DISC1 on adult and embryonic neurogenesis (18, 28, 31, 32), it would be interesting to further compare morphological, neurochemical, electrophysiological, and behavioral effects of embryonic vs. adult knockdown of DISC1 in mice at different time points of prenatal and postnatal development.
2.3. DISC1 Polymorphism Model
An unexpected discovery of 25 bp deletion mutation in exon 6 and a stop codon in exon 7 in Disc1 in 129S6/EvSv substrain during a gene targeting process led to an attempt to generate a mouse model of the translocation. In this mouse model, expression of a full-length Disc1 isoform was absent when assayed with Western blotting using a custom-made polyclonal antibody against the N-terminal portion. Modified by introducing a polyadenylation site in intron 7 and a stop codon in exon 8, the mutation was transferred to C57Bl/6 J background. This modification aimed to generate a fragment similar to the Scottish translocation in which the breakpoint is between exons 8 and 9 (33, 34).
2.3.1. Background
2.3.2. Results
The first report with this model (35) characterized the morphological and behavioral abnormalities in mice. The authors used a delayed non-match to place task (T-maze) to evaluate the function of the prefrontal cortex such as visuospatial working memory known to be impaired in schizophrenia (36, 37). No differences between mutant and control mice were seen in the learning phase of the test. In contrast, during the test phase, significant impairment in working memory was found at the 15- and 30-s choice delays for both heterozygous and homozygous animals. In addition, a deficit was seen at the 5-s delay in homozygous mutants. No differences between groups were detected in novelty-induced activity or PPI. The low magnification evaluation of the brain morphology showed no gross abnormalities. A follow-up study (38) evaluated the pathology of the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in more detail. A 14% decrease in volume of mPFC and no changes in volume of the hippocampus were observed. In order to get a better resolution of the cortical morphology, DISC1 mice were mated with GFP-M and Thy-1-GFP transgenic mice that express GFP in neurons (39, 40). The authors found increased quantities of immature neurons in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus in mutant mice and increased rates of neuronal migration indicated
216
Ayhan et al.
by the presence of a larger number of cells localized in the outer layers of the granule cell layer. These cells were misoriented and tended to have decreased apical dendritic branch points and dendritic length. Morphology of mature granule cells was analyzed in DISC1–Thy-1 mutant mice that express GFP in post-mitotic neurons. The similar misorientation phenotype was also seen in mature granule cells in mutant mice. In addition, a subset of mature granule cells showed decreased total dendritic length and numbers of dendritic pines. The structural abnormalities were associated with altered short-term potentiation detected in CA1– CA3 synapses. The authors also performed a comprehensive behavioral evaluation of mutant mice, including contextual fear conditioning, novel object recognition, Morris water maze and a win-shift version of the eight-arm radial maze, and two-choice delayed nonmatch-to-position (DNMP) task. Mutant mice showed a significant deficit in DNMP test only. DNMP is a measure of working memory and consists of training and testing phases. Both heterozygous and homozygous mutants displayed a deficit in the learning phase and compared to control mice, the performance of mutant mice was significantly worse at the 70-s delay during the testing phase. 2.3.3. Conclusions
This model with a loss of one full-length Disc1 isoform provides important clues on the function of DISC1. The main strength of this model is a modification of the endogenous gene whose expression is regulated by the natural promoter. However, the possible existence of other full-length isoforms of the protein in these mice as suggested by a different study may complicate mechanistic interpretations of neurobehavioral alterations. This also highlights a difficulty in generating complete knockout models for genes with multiple spliced isoforms of RNA (41, 42).
2.4. ENU-Induced Mutagenesis Mouse Model
In the Scottish family, schizophrenia has not been diagnosed in all translocation carriers. Some members suffer from recurrent major depression, bipolar disorder, or alcohol and substance abuse. Recent postmortem and genetic epidemiology studies have also implicated DISC1 in mood disorders and autism (43–47). Thus, different mutations in the gene may lead to variable clinical and pathological manifestations.
2.4.1. Background
2.4.2. Approach
Mutagenesis with N-nitroso-N-ethylurea (ENU) was used to generate several mouse lines, carrying different mutations of the gene (48). Since all isoforms of the gene have exon 2, and exon 2 has the binding site for PDE4B (49, 50), mutants in exon 2 were screened. Two novel, independent missense mutations were identified, with one resulting in thymine instead of adenine at 127th position and the other resulting in cytosine instead of Thymine
DISC1 Mouse Models
217
in 334th position. These single nucleotide mutations produced Q31L and L100P amino acid exchanges, respectively. Mutant mice were backcrossed to C57BL/6 J background and homozygous (31L/31L; 100P/100P), heterozygous (31L/+; 100P/+), and compound heterozygous (31L/100P) mutants were tested. 2.4.3. Results
The 31L mutant mice showed increased immobility in forced swim test (FST), decreased sociability and social novelty, and decreased sucrose consumption, resembling that of depression in humans. The 100P mutant mice predominantly exhibited increase in horizontal activity, decreased prepulse inhibition (PPI), decreased latent inhibition (LI), and decreased performance in T-maze, consistent with abnormalities in schizophrenia. Interestingly, bupropion, an antidepressant with properties of a dopamine and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor, decreased immobility in FST and normalized PPI deficit in 31L homozygous animals. Rolipram, a phosphodiesterase-4 inhibitor, ameliorated PPI deficit in “schizophrenic-like” 100P mice, so did clozapine and haloperidol. Of note, clozapine but not other drugs significantly increased PPI in control mice. The biochemical assay found reduced binding of PDE4B to both mutant forms of Disc1, with 100P type being affected to a greater extent. The activity of PDE4B was found decreased in brains of 31L mice without altering levels of the protein. The decreased activity of the enzyme has been suggested to explain lower responsiveness to rolipram in these mice as the drug inhibits phosphodiesterase.
2.4.4. Conclusion
The study has demonstrated associations of two different mutations in Disc1 with distinct neurobehavioral and pharmacological phenotypes, resembling schizophrenia and mood disorders. In addition, the new result of the study is an association of “schizophrenia-like” phenotype of 100P mice with decreased levels of PDE4. A caveat of the model is that the mutated region is not conserved in humans, somewhat decreasing relevance of the induced mutations to human disorders.
2.5. Transgenic Models
Although the Scottish translocation mutation is a rare genetic variant, the potential outcome of the mutant, i.e., a production of mutant protein, is an intriguing possibility for mechanistic research. Indeed, in vitro studies demonstrated that mutant DISC1 might produce its effects via dominant-negative mechanisms (51). Thus, several transgenic mouse models of mutant human DISC1 have been generated.
2.5.1. Constitutive Expression 2.5.1.1. Background 2.5.1.2. Approach
Hikida et al. generated a transgenic mouse model with constitutive expression of mutant DISC1 under the CaMKII promoter (72). Expression of mRNA for the mutant protein product was
218
Ayhan et al.
demonstrated with Northern blotting and in situ hybridization in the olfactory bulb, frontal cortex, hippocampus, and basal ganglia at different developmental time points. 2.5.1.3. Results
Compared to control littermates, mutant DISC1 mice were more active in open field test and displayed a deficit in PPI when the intensity of prepulse was set at 74 dB. Mutant mice were more immobile in FST than control animals, suggesting depression-like phenotype. Also, it took longer for mutant mice to find the hidden food, indicating impaired olfaction or decreased motivation for food. When immunoreactivity of parvalbumin, calretinin, and calbindin was evaluated in the medial prefrontal cortex, decreased numbers of parvalbumin-positive cells were found, with no significant changes in numbers of calbindin- or calretinin-positive neurons. In vivo and ex vivo MRI analysis demonstrated enlargement of the lateral ventricles in two different lines of 6-week-old mice, although this pathology was no longer present in 3-month-old DISC1 mice, suggesting a compensatory process. The lateral ventricle enlargement was accompanied with a decreased left/right ratio for the hippocampus but not the cortex or basal ganglia.
2.5.1.4. Conclusions
Expression of mutant DISC1 under the CaMKII promoter resulted in the schizophrenia-like phenotypic features associated with schizophrenia and provides a useful experimental tool for future translational studies, including testing novel therapeutics.
2.5.2. Inducible Expression of the C-Terminal Fragment
As expression of DISC1 is high during neurodevelopment, an early disruption of DISC1 functions may result in long-lasting abnormalities present even in adulthood. A model of inducible expression of the truncated human DISC1 has been generated to test a hypothesis that a transient perturbation in the function of endogenous DISC1 by dominant-negative effects could have permanent neurobehavioral effects.
2.5.2.1. Background
2.5.2.2. Approach
The group of investigators led by T. Cannon and A. Silva described an interesting mouse model of inducible expression of the fragment of DISC1 (DISC1-cc) that is deleted in the affected members of the Scottish family. DISC1-cc expression is regulated by the CaMKII promoter, which is active in forebrain neurons. DISC1-cc spans residues 671–852, which is the portion of DISC1 crucial for binding to NUDEL and Lis1 (16–18). The DISC1-cc protein was fused to a HA-tagged mutant (G521R) estrogen receptor ligand-binding domain (LBD). In this inducible system, the transgenic protein is degraded after sequestration by heat-shock chaperone proteins. When tamoxifen (tam) is administered to transgenic mice, it binds the LBD, the fusion protein complex, which includes DISC1-cc,
DISC1 Mouse Models
219
undergoes a conformational switch such that the transgenic protein dissociates from the chaperone proteins and becomes functional. After a quick metabolism of tam, the transgenic protein acquires non-functional state again (56). 2.5.2.3. Results
DISC1-cc was found to be bound to Nudel1 and Lis1 for 2 days, transiently decreasing expression of DISC1 interacting partners in transgenic mice. The neurobehavioral effects of DISC1-cc were evaluated in adult mice after tam administration at postnatal day (PND) 7 or in adulthood. Expression of DISC1-cc at PND 7 but not in adulthood impaired working memory assayed by DNMPT test with a 10-s delay. No effects were found when 1-, 5-, 10-, or and 20-s delay intervals were used. In similar vein, sociability of mice was significantly attenuated by early postnatal but not adult expression of the fragment. Mice treated with tam at PND 7 spent significantly less time with a live mouse than an inanimate object, while all control mice displayed an expected pattern of interaction, spending more time with live partners. In addition, early postnatal expression of DISC-cc led to a shorter latency to immobilization in forced swim test compared to other groups. The behavioral alterations induced by early postnatal expression of DISC1-cc were found to be associated with reduced dendritic complexity and decreased hippocampal synaptic transmission, without affecting paired-pulse facilitation, a marker of presynaptic activity and long-term potentiation.
2.5.2.4. Conclusions
This model has provided the first evidence that an early postnatal transient perturbation in the DISC1 function can have lasting permanent effects on brain morphology and behavior in adult mice. Although the observed effects convincingly support the hypothesis put forward by the authors, there is a caveat in the study related to expression of the fragment that is not found in humans. Additional control experiments would be necessary to directly compare the effects of early postnatal expression period (Tg/Tam/P7) and adulthood expression only (Tg/Tam/A) after a longer period to time (not 6 hours) as was done for the postnatal group, as well as to evaluate the effects of adult expression of this mutation.
2.5.3. Inducible Expression of Mutant DISC1
Given potential multiple functions of DISC1 across different stages of neurodevelopment to contribute to the pathogenesis of psychiatric disorders (52–54), studying selective contributions of prenatal vs. postnatal expressions of mutant human DISC1 is of great interest.
2.5.3.1. Background 2.5.3.2. Approach
Inducible DISC1 mouse model is a standard bi-transgenic Tetoff system. In order to turn off transgene expression, DOX is added to mouse food or drinking water. As transcription of tTA
220
Ayhan et al.
is regulated by the α-calmodulin kinase II (CaMKII) promoter, expression of mutant DISC1 is present in neurons of the olfactory bulbs, cortex, hippocampus, striatum but not cerebellum. It was found that expression of mutant DISC1 starts prenatally as early as embryonic day (E) 15 as detected by Western blot and E9 when assayed by RT-PCR (unpublished data). Thus, this model provides the opportunity to regulate both prenatal and postnatal expression of mutant DISC1. 2.5.3.3. Results
The initial study with the model (55) evaluated the neurobehavioral effects of constitutive expression of mutant DISC1. Compared to littermate control mice that did not express mutant DISC1, transgenic male mice exhibited increased spontaneous locomotor activity, decreased social interaction, and increased aggressive behaviors, while transgenic female mice demonstrated poorer spatial recognition memory in Morris water maze. These behavioral alterations were accompanied by enlargement of the lateral ventricles when assayed in 9-month-old mice. Evaluation of neurite outgrowth in primary cortical neurons showed a reduction in dendritic complexity measured with Scholl method.
2.5.3.4. Conclusions
The advantages of this model lie in the ability to turn on and off gene expression via doxycycline administration. Expression of the gene can be regulated in utero and across postnatal development for varying durations. Regulation of expression in developing and adult animals may be used in drug screen aimed at targeting stable and reversible aspects of the behavioral phenotype. Potential caveats of the model include unwanted effects of doxycycline on the brain, particularly during development, and multiple control groups for assessing non-specific effects of transgenesis and tTA. In addition, the feasibility to generate large cohorts might be an issue if several experimental groups are needed.
2.5.4. BAC Transgenic DISC1 Model
The transgenic DISC1 models described above utilize the CaMKII promoter to drive expression of the mutant gene in forebrain neurons. A different mouse system has been generated with regulation of expression by the endogenous Disc1 promoter (56).
2.5.4.1. Background 2.5.4.2. Approach
The construct was prepared from mouse bacterial artificial chromosome (BAC) RP23–236F19 containing Disc1 exons 1–9 with its entire upstream sequences fused with an EGFP cDNA to the end of exon 8 as an identifying tag, followed by a SV40 polyA signal.
2.5.4.3. Results
Transcripts were detected at E17.5. The authors observed comparable levels of expression of full-length and mutant DISC1 by RT-PCR. At E17.5, expression of mutant DISC1 was predominantly observed in the hippocampus and cortex, while cerebellar
DISC1 Mouse Models
221
expression was also noted in adult mice. Nissl staining revealed enlarged lateral ventricles and reduced volumes of the cortex in male mice. In addition, thinning of the cortical layers II and III and a partial agenesis of the corpus callosum were noted. Immunohistochemical analysis revealed reduced numbers of parvalbumin-positive cells in the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex. Neurogenesis was reduced in the cortex of newborn mutant mice as shown by BrdU staining. Primary cortical neurons derived from P1 pups had fewer and shorter neurites. Compared to control animals, mutant mice exhibited latent inhibition and increased immobility in FST and TST. Significantly fewer stress calls by mutant mice were detected during a maternal separation test. 2.5.4.4. Conclusions
The expression of mutant DISC1 under the endogenous promoter resulted in the neurobehavioral abnormalities reminiscent of those observed in schizophrenia and mood disorders. The phenotype reported for these mice is similar to that found in other DISC1 mouse models. In addition, the study was the first one to demonstrate decreased cortical neurogenesis in a mutant DISC1 mouse model.
3. Summary In this chapter, current mouse models of DISC1 were reviewed (Table 9.1). These models need further characterization and confirmation to be accepted as reliable DISC1 mouse models. This particularly relates to the mutant DISC1 mouse models as, although not unexpectedly, both consistent and discordant phenotypic changes have been reported for mice of different models. It seems that a head-to-head comparison would be interesting. As interplay between different susceptibility genes and/or genetic × environmental factors seems to contribute to the pathogenesis of major mental illnesses, DISC1 mouse models may serve as valuable experimental tools to evaluate the mechanisms whereby those complex interactions take place (57). The DISC1 mutation may act as an additive or synergistic factor in interactions with environmental pathogens which by themselves would cause small if any abnormalities in mice without the mutant gene. The current models are based on expression of the Scottish mutation or a natural polymorphism of the endogenous mouse gene. A knockout model, which would have a complete deletion of the gene and main Disc1 isoforms, would be very valuable for a better understanding of the gene function. New models of DISC1 will advance our knowledge of the gene. One approach can include generating models with cell
In utero E14.5 KD function of endogenous Disc1
Oncoretrovirus KD endogenous injected into Disc1 the adult dentate gyrus
KD endogenous Disc1
KD endogenous Disc1
In utero E13, BrdU 2 h before sacrificing at E16
Lentivirus injected into the adult dentate gyrus, BrdU 5 weeks later
Kamiya
Duan
Mao
Function
Approach
Model
Table 9.1 DISC1 mouse models
Disc1 shRNA
Disc1 shRNA
Disc1 RNAi
Disc1 RNAi or express C-terminally truncated DISC1
Construct n/d
Hippocampus n/d
Hippocampus n/d
Reduced BrdU positive cells, rescued by GSK3-β inhibitor
Reduced BrdU positive cells, rescued by degradation of resistant β-catenin or GSK3-β inhibitor
Soma hypertrophy, multiple primary dendrites, overextended migration, and accelerated synaptogenesis
P2: Inhibition of neuronal migration. P14: Shorter, misoriented dendrites
Gross Cellular anatomy abnormalities
Hippocampus n/d
Pyramidal cortical neurons
Expression pattern
Locomotor hyperactivity increased immobility in forced swim test
n/d
n/d
n/d
Behavior
DISC1 regulates neuronal progenitor proliferation by inhibiting GSK3-β
DISC1 is involved in embryonic and adult neuronal migration
KD of Disc1 either with RNAi or with mDISC1 leads to abnormal development of the cortex
Significance
222 Ayhan et al.
Approach
Function
Construct
Spontaneous mutation in 129S6/SvEv, transferred to C57BL/6
Transgenic
Koike Kvajo
Hikida
Express C-terminally truncated human DISC1, dominant negative
Some Disc1 isoforms are missing
Endogenous
Expression pattern
Reduced prefrontal cortex volume
Reduced brain volume
Gross anatomy
Constitutive Olfactory Enlarged under bulbs, lateral CaMKII frontal ventripromoter cortex, hipcles pocampus, basal ganglia postnatally
25 bp Endogenous deletion in exon 6, results in premature stop codon in exon 7
Clapcote ENU Missense point n/a mutagenesis, mutations: screening of Q31L, L100P DISC1 exon 2
Model
Table 9.1 (continued) Behavior
Significance
Decreased parvalbumin+ immunoreactivity in the medial prefrontal cortex
Altered organization of the dentate gyrus, No difference in parvalbumin+ or calbindin+ staining in medial prefrontal cortex
Increased immobility in forced swim test, mild hyperactive in the open field, PPI deficit
Impaired working memory
First DISC1 model based on a human mutation
Loss of some isoforms altered hippocampal morphology and memory performance. Note that all 129 substrains have the mentioned mutation which may affect the phenotypes of the models generated in them.
Reduced binding Q31L: greater A point mutation in to PDE4B immobility in the Disc1 produced forced swim test, various reduced abnormalities. sociability, The two point reduced sucrose mutants had consumption. different behavioral L100P: phenotypes (Q31L novelty-induced depression-like, activity, deficits in L100P PPI, latent schizophrenia-like) inhibition, and working memory
Cellular abnormalities
DISC1 Mouse Models 223
BAC (bacterial artificial chromosome) transgenic
Shen
Express Fused GFP C-terminally in-frame to the truncated end of exon 8 mouse DISC1
Express the Transgenic under C-terminus of CaMKII DISC1, which promoter, competes on inducible by binding to tamoxifen for NDEL1 and